CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES AND ADULT AGGRESSION Sarah Castagnola B.A., California State University of Sacramento, 2007 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in CHILD DEVELOPMENT (Applied Settings) at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO SUMMER 2011 CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES AND ADULT AGGRESSION A Thesis by Sarah Castagnola Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Dr. Juliana Raskauskas __________________________________, Second Reader Dr. Todd Migliaccio ____________________________ Date ii Student: Sarah Castagnola I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Dr. Karen Davis-O’Hara Department of Child Development iii ___________________ Date Abstract of CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES AND ADULT AGGRESSION by Sarah Castagnola Violence/aggression is a public health problem, and children who witness or experience acts of violence are particularly vulnerable to its effects. What are the childhood experiences that lead to adult aggression? Research is needed to examine the relationship between different forms of aggression in childhood to adult adjustment. Investigation of the contribution of different forms of aggression can also help with the identification of factors that are related to stability between childhood exposure and later aggression. Identification of these factors can provide avenues for prevention as well as a better understanding of the effects of these experiences. The present study will include research on family aggression (i.e. between parents, parent-child, and child-child). With new information about childhood experiences and how it can lead to adult aggression, researchers may find a way to reduce the amount of violence from adults who have aggressive childhood experiences. _______________________, Committee Chair Dr. Juliana Raskauskas _______________________ Date iv DEDICATION To my handsome husband Travis, my beautiful daughter Sidney, my Mother, and all of my family and friends, thank you for all of the support you have given me. v ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First, I would like to thank my sponsor, Dr. Juliana Raskauskas. Without her encouragement, quick feedback and patience, I would have been unable to complete this thesis. I would also like to thank my second reader, Dr. Todd Migliaccio. Thank you both for taking time to work with me to complete my Thesis. Thank you to the Child Development Department at Sacramento State University and all of the staff for making my college experience so wonderful. I am proud to say received my graduate degree is from Sacramento State. A special thanks to the college and anger management facilities for allowing me to come in and collect data and to the participants who took the time to fill out the survey. Thank you to my husband for your support and for believing in me. It has been a crazy four years but you have helped me believe in myself. To my daughter Sidney, you are my world and I am so thankful we had you; your smile makes my heart melt. Thank you to my mother, you have always had encouraging words for me whenever I was down. You have a always believed in me, that means more to me than you will ever know. You are the most amazing, loving person, and I am so happy that you are my mommy! Finally, thank you to everyone who supported me, listened to me, and encouraged me to keep going. Melissa Brown, thank you for all your help from the very beginning. Without you keeping me motivated and encouraged, this would not have happened. I am so thankful for each and every one of you! vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Dedication ................................................................................................................... iv Acknowledgments..........................................................................................................v List of Tables ............................................................................................................. viii Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................................1 Purpose of the Study ..........................................................................................2 Statement of the Problem ...................................................................................3 Significance of the Study .................................................................................. 3 Methods..............................................................................................................4 Definition of Terms ...........................................................................................5 Key Findings .....................................................................................................6 Limitations ........................................................................................................7 Organization of the Study .................................................................................8 2. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .........................................................................9 Theoretical Frameworks .................................................................................10 Aggression .......................................................................................................15 Parent Violence ...............................................................................................17 Siblings ...........................................................................................................20 Related Factors.................................................................................................23 The Present Study ...........................................................................................24 3. METHODS ...........................................................................................................26 Design and Research Questions ..................................................................... 26 Participants ......................................................................................................27 Measures ..........................................................................................................29 Procedures .......................................................................................................31 vii Summary ..........................................................................................................32 4. RESULTS ..............................................................................................................33 Question 1 .......................................................................................................33 Question 2 .......................................................................................................34 Question 3 ........................................................................................................36 Conclusion ......................................................................................................37 5. DISCUSSION .........................................................................................................38 Aggressive Childhood Experiences and Adult Aggression .............................38 Childhood Experiences and Adult Aggression Compared Between Community College and Anger Management .................................................40 Factors Relating to Adult Aggression ..............................................................42 Implications......................................................................................................43 Limitations ......................................................................................................44 Appendix A. Information Sheet for Participation .......................................................47 Appendix B. Aggression Survey..................................................................................49 References ....................................................................................................................54 viii LIST OF TABLES Page 1. Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the sample ........................................26 2. Table 2 Correlations Between Childhood Experiences ...................................32 3. Table 3 Independent Samples T-test Comparing Reports ..............................33 4. Table 4 Regression Predicting Total Aggression.............................................35 ix 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Domestic violence (DV) is a widespread issue affecting many families and children. Defined as abuse that occurs between two people including current and former spouses and dating partners, Domestic violence or “intimate partner violence” can include physical, verbal, emotional, monetary, social control or harm (Center for Disease Control, 2007). The center for disease control (2007), states that one in four women, and one in nine men in the United States are victim of DV at some point in their lives. In lower income homes 35% of women and 21% of men were victims of DV. It is estimated that more than 1,200 deaths and two million injuries among women and roughly 600,000 reported injuries among men per year are due to DV. On average, 24% of women and 12% of men aged 18 years or more have a lifetime history of DV (2007). These numbers indicate that a significantly large number of individuals are affected by violence. Further, statistics indicate that more than half of female victims of DV live in households with children under the age of 12, therefore as many as 3 to 10 million children witness DV. The National Center for Children Exposed to Violence (2006) reported that child abuse occurs in 30-60% of family violence cases that involve families with children. Not only are the two people involved in violence affected, but others around them are affected as well. The exposure to violence in the home can lead to aggression in the child’s adult relationships later life it would indicate intergenerational transmission of violence. Victimization in childhood from adults or peers has been shown to contribute to 2 later aggression and crime perpetration (Kwong, Bartholomew, Henderson, & Trinke, 2003). This connection between crime and aggression and exposure to violence in childhood should make this a vital area for study. Previous research has shown that while there is a relationship not all children exposed to violence and aggression become aggressive themselves. Research is needed to identify factors that are related to the relationship between childhood exposure and later aggression. Identification of these factors might provide avenues for prevention and a better understanding of the effects of these experiences on children. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the proposed study is to investigate the nature of the relationship between childhood experiences of family violence and adult aggression. In this study childhood experiences is divided into two categories, witnessing aggression and experiencing aggression. Witnessing aggression includes witnessing domestic violence, parental violence, and sibling violence. The present study included a quantitative survey of men and women currently attending a community college and those attending domestic violence and anger management meetings. Factors suspected of influence exposure to DV and the relationship between childhood and adult aggression such as education, SES, gender, parental relationships, social support, age, recreation, parental involvement, and addictions, were included as possible covariates. 3 It was hypothesized that individuals who exhibit aggression or excessive anger in adulthood will report witnessing aggression in childhood. The relative predictive power of each form of childhood aggression to frequency and severity of aggression in adulthood will also be examined. Statement of the Problem There is substantial research related to intergenerational transmission of violence and aggression and or violence perpetration in adulthood. The current study is an extension of previous research on aggression. The data collected in this study help identify sources of childhood experiences with aggression that relate to adult aggression. The research questions that are drawn from the literature and the theoretical framework are (1) Is there a relationship between aggressive childhood experiences (from all sources) and adult aggression? (2) Is there a difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between a community college and an anger management sample? (3) Do factors such as age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency statistically predict adult aggression? Significance of the Study Violence affects people in all stages of life from infancy to elderly. It was reported in 2006 that 18,573 people died as a result of homicide and 33,300 took their 4 own life (CDC, 2008a, 2008b). The number of violent deaths only tells part of the story that is behind the scenes. Many more people survive violence than those who are who have died as a result of violence. Those individuals who have survived are left with permanent physical and emotional scars. Knowing that violence is a public health problem, there are important factors needed to understand to that protect people or put them at risk for experiencing or perpetrating violence. Identifying how transmission of aggression and violence across different forms or sources and what factors may be related can contribute to prevention and intervention efforts in the future. Methods The study examined the relationship between childhood experiences with family violence and adult aggression. This study used a quantitative design with suveys about current and childhood experiences being collected with two targeted samples. The study sample included 38 adults from two anger management treatment programs in Southern California and 43 adults from a community college in Northern California. The individuals from the anger management facility were targeted due to the nature of this study and their increased risk of exposure to aggression in childhood identified in the literature. The community college sample served as a comparison group for this study. Surveys were administered in classrooms at both the anger management treatment program and community college. All measures were administered to participating 5 students at the same time. All information regarding the proposed study and forms that explain the survey were read out loud and surveys completed silently and independently by participating adults. Precautions were taken to protect confidentiality of participant’s answers and for their protection no items asked participants to identify their name. Consent to participate in this study was given by completing the survey. The survey for this study included four sections: demographic information such as age and gender, level of education, occupation, household income, childhood information, childhood experiences with in the family, and information regarding aggression in the past six months. Definition of Terms For the purpose of clarity, this thesis used the following terms as defined below: Aggression According to previous research aggression can be defined as (a) intent to harm another person; (b) intense autonomic arousal and strong responses to perceived threats; and (c) hostility (Coie & Dodge, 1998; Kokko & Pilkkinen, 2005). 6 Violence Violence is the actual act of aggression, intent to cause pain or injury. Violence can be defined as (a) maltreatment and/or neglect; (b) physical abuse; and (c) sexual abuse (Margolin & Gordis, 2004; Slep & O’Leary, 2007; Linder & Collins, 2005). Anger management Anger management refers to classes taken by individuals who are court ordered to receive therapy for aggressive/violent behavior. Child Abuse Child abuse is defined as any act or series of acts by a parent or caregiver that results in harm, potential for harm, or threat of harm to a child (CDC, 2009). Key Findings The key findings from the study support the hypothesis that early experiences with aggression as a target or witness is associated with adult aggression. Correlations between childhood experiences and adult aggression were significant for all sources of childhood aggression experiences. All correlations were positive which means that high levels of adult aggression were associated with exposure to aggression during childhood from the mother, father and/or siblings. It was also shown that the anger management sample was, on average, higher on both current physical and verbal aggression than the 7 community college sample. Variables for dependency and anger management significantly predicted total aggression, while gender was not a significant predictor in regression analyses. When comparing the reports of childhood exposure to aggression between community college participants and participants in anger management, the findings support the hypothesis that the anger management group was significantly higher in all early experiences with aggression at home. Limitations It is important to note the limitations of the current study. One limitation was the use of self-report surveys which can be vulnerable to participants misreporting or reporting in a manner that they feel is socially desirable. Future research should include other means of self report such as focus groups and interviews, as well as attempt to get corroboration from other sources (e.g., teachers, social workers, family members, police reports/government statistics). Another limitation is that this is a retrospective. Participants were asked to recall their childhood experiences but memory can be vulnerable to deterioration. It is possible that for those who were in anger management aggressive memories were more salient. Also since the data were collected all at the same time and are correlational, no conclusions about causality can be drawn. It can only show that there is a relationship between the variables. 8 Finally, the current study has a relatively small sample size. Future research should include a large population of participants in order to ensure that group comparisons are reliable and valid. Lastly, the questionnaire was long and individuals expressed displeasure with the time and cognitive load required, some participants even stopped completing the survey before the end. Future research should use shorter questionnaires or divide the items into parts to be taken at different times. Organization of the Study This chapter has provided an introduction to the topic and serves as the overview of the thesis. The next chapter, Chapter 2, presents a review of the literature and examines the relationship between childhood experiences and adult aggression. Chapter 3 describes the methodology used in this study. It describes methods used to examine the specific research question: (1) Is there a relationship between aggressive childhood experiences and adult aggression? (2) Is there a difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between the community college and anger management samples? (3) Do factors such as age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency predict concurrent adult aggression? Chapter 4 presents a summary of the results of the study. Chapter 5 includes a discussion of conclusions reached, the limitations of the study and the major findings of the study in the context of existing and future research. 9 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Family violence is a public health problem, and children who witness or experience acts of violence within the family are particularly vulnerable to its effects (Margolin & Gordis, 2004). What are the childhood experiences that lead to adult aggression? Research is needed to examine the relationship between different forms of aggression within the family (i.e. between parents, parent-child, and sibling-sibling) and adult adjustment. Investigation of family violence can lead to the identification of factors that moderate the relationship between childhood exposure and later aggression. Identification of these factors will provide avenues for prevention and a better understanding of the effects of these experiences. There are two theories that can help explain how aggression is passed down from generation to generation. These theories explain why seeing or experiencing family violence within your family of origin may lead to the use of violence or aggression with others as an adult (Capelle & Heiner, 1990; MacEwem, 1994). The first theory is the Social Learning Theory and the second is Intergenerational Transmission Theory. Both theories focus on modeling and direct tuition as ways to learn behaviors and for behavior to be transmitted from one generation to the next (Bandura, 1977; MacEwem, 1994). 10 Theoretical Frameworks Intergenerational Transmission theory explains how aggression can be passed down to each generation. This theory explains children are socialized by parents about appropriate behavior in relationships. This transmission of aggression is often learned as appropriate behavior with spouse and parent through witnessing aggression or experiencing violence from a family member. Intergenerational transmission influences the goals, choices and opportunities available to offspring (Story, Karney, Lawrence, Bradbury, & Thomas, 2004). Adolescents most likely use their parents’ marital relationships as models for their own relationships (Hare, Migra & Allen, 2009). Hare, Miga, and Allen (2009) found characteristics of parents’ marital relationships were a key predictor of characteristics of adolescents’ romantic relationships. There were 75 adolescent participants who were interviewed with their parents and re-interviewed five years later. Hare, Miga and Allen (2009) also found that higher levels of paternal aggression during marital conflict were associated with greater perpetration of and victimization by aggression in romantic relationships five years later. Intergenerational transmission includes aggressions/violence amongst family members and influences more than two people. Within a family there are several dyads in which aggression can occur: Fathermother, mother-father, father-self, mother-self, sibling-self and parents-siblings. These are the ways that children can either witness aggression as well as experience aggression. The intergenerational transmission of domestic violence has been one of the most commonly reported influences of domestic violence in adulthood (Corvo, 2006). The 11 subjects in Corvos’s research were part of a domestic violence treatment program. Measures used were from the conflict tactic scale (CTS). Corvo found that greater effects for parental child abuse upon current levels of violence than witnessing domestic violence. Prior research indicated that parents’ treatment of one another during times of conflict may depict the overall tone for how children interpret conflict in the family system (Hare, Miga, & Allen, 2009). Marital conflict that happens frequently may cause individuals to have intense emotional hesitant relationships in which they do not connect with their spouse emotionally and tension within the family environment (Hare, Miga, & Allen, 2009). Kwong, Bartholomew, Henderson, and Trinke (2003) found that children who witness or experience aggression through intergenerational transmission of violence may be violent or have aggression issues later in life. Intergenerational transmission theory has some of the same explanation as the social learning theory, but it is not based on the behaviorist orientation that focuses on rewarding of behavior rather the establishment of normal or expected behavior. Behaviorist theorists argue that observable behavior is shaped by responses to it such as reinforcement that increase the likelihood that behavior will continue or punishment that decreases the likelihood that behavior will be repeated. One theorist that comes from the behaviorist perspective is Albert Bandura (1977, 1986), and his behaviorist theory is called Social Learning Theory. The basic tenant of the theory is that people not only respond to reinforcement/punishment they receive but also learn new behaviors through observational learning. Bandura (1977) explained that most human behavior is learned by watching other individuals model the behavior and observing 12 whether the model is reinforced or punished. Social Learning Theory directly addresses the transmission of aggression, in fact Bandura (1965; 1973) proposed that children can learn aggression from watching aggressive models such as parents. Bandura’s experiments included showing children videos of models being aggressive and seeing whether the children who viewed the aggression would respond aggressively in the lab. The findings indicated that those that viewed the aggressive models showed more aggressive behavior, more aggressive vocalizations, and that these effects were increased if models were reinforced and less if the models were punished. It was also shown that the effect of observing aggressive models was more for male than female children (Bandura, 1965). Much of the subsequent application of this theory has focused on children imitating aggressive modeled by actors in the media (Boyatzis, Matillo & Nesbitt, 1995; Huesmann et al., 2003) or on how parents using physical abuse can model the use of violence in children (Corvo, 2006; Kuppens, Grietens, Onghena, & Michiels, 2009). When individuals observe others, ideas of how to perform new behaviors are formed. If people observe positive, desired outcomes of the observed behavior, they are more likely to model, imitate, and adopt the behavior themselves. Individuals learn through observation, this influences how one responds to and reacts in any given situation. Modeled behavior is more likely to be imitated if the behavior is perceived as resulting in desirable outcome (Corvo, 2006). Therefore, individuals who observe others model aggressive/violent behaviors and achieve their goals will learn how to show aggression or acts of violence. 13 Not every individual who is exposed to violence will become violent, this is because every individual will act in a different manner depending on their personal beliefs and characteristics. Many individuals will self-regulate their thoughts and actions due to their internal standards. Children observe people around them and learn standards for others and themselves. Behaviors that measure up to internal standards are judged positively (self-praise), and behaviors that fall short of these standards are judged negatively (self-blame) (Grusec, 1992). If individuals are exposed to aggressive behaviors that are rewarded, they may learn that this is a positive standard and not learn that this social standard is not accepted. People who do not learn normative morals will not have the correct tools to self-regulate (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996). It is also evident that individuals are not only products of their life circumstances; they are contributors (Bandura, 2006). The person’s own agency contributes to the development and whether he/she will follow aggressive models. Social learning theory identifies four core properties of human agency: The first agency is intentionality, this includes action plans and strategies for realizing intentions. The second agency is forethought, this involves goals and anticipation of outcomes. The third agency is selfreactiveness, agents are planners and forethinkers, and are self-regulators. The fourth and final agency is self-reflectiveness, they are self-aware, reflect on personal efficacy and meanings of their pursuits and make corrective adjustments when needed (Bandura, 2006). Individuals may follow aggressive models if they cannot regulate their emotions, set positive goals, make adjustments in their thinking, and do not have the capacity for 14 realizing intentions because their thinking is one track and follow other models does not require thinking, it only requires actions. Individuals who do not follow aggressive models are able to think about appropriate strategies and make adjustment according to each individual situation. When aggression and/or violence occurs in one’s family of origin, it may create ideas and norms about how, when, and towards whom aggression is appropriate (Corvo, 2006). In order for a person to learn and model a behavior there are four requirements people must obtain which include retention, reproduction, motor reproduction, and motivation to want to adopt the behavior. Albert Bandura’s latter additions to Social Learning Theory (Social Cognitive Theory) states that behavior is explained in terms of continuous bidirectional relationships between cognitive, behavioral, and environmental influences. Individuals do not need external rewards to model a behavior they observe. Bandura’s theory discusses that individuals experience intrinsic reinforcement as a form of internal reward. Internal rewards include pride, satisfaction, and accomplishment that lead to multiple perpetration of aggression (Bandura, 1977). This past research directly relates to the proposed study regarding family dynamics and the effects it can have on the children involved because individuals who experience violence in their lifetime can have different issues relating back to their childhood experiences of violence. Social cognitive theory also explains human behavior in terms of continuous reciprocal interactions called reciprocal determinism. Reciprocal determinism explains that behaviors are not created as a result of the environment, personal, or behavioral factors independently but by the threes factors together. Reciprocal determinism explains 15 that an individuals’ behavior and the world cause each other, and that one’s environment causes one’s behavior (Bandura, 2001). Some influences are stronger than others. Factors between the three interactions will be determined based on the individual (Bandura, 1986). The interaction between an individual and behavior involves the influences of an individual’s thoughts and actions (Bandura, 2001). Every individual understands and responds to situations differently, which helps explain why individuals that grew up in the same environment can respond differently to the same situations. The interaction between the individual and the environment involves cognition and human beliefs that are developed and modified by social influences and structures within the environment. The interaction between the environment and behavior, involves an individual’s behavior thoughts about their environment then the behavior is modified by that environment (Bandura, 2001). By understanding the basic function of the Social Learning Theory (and by extension Social Cognitive Theory) one can begin to understand how aggression can be learned through observation and be transmitted from one generation to the next. Aggression Aggression is generally defined as the intent to do harm. Previous research on aggression, has defined it as the intent to harm another person, intense autonomic arousal and strong responses to perceived threats and hostility (Coie & Dodge, 1998; Kokko & Pilkkinen, 2005). Violence, on the other hand, is the actual act of aggression. Violence 16 within the family can take the form of maltreatment and/or neglect, physical abuse, and sexual abuse (Margolin & Gordis, 2004; Slep & O’Leary, 2007). Aggression and violence are important to study due to the vast amounts of individuals affected by it everyday. Family violence statistical information, reports that in 2007, as many as 40,200 incidents of spousal violence were reported. Nearly 53,400 children and youth were reported to have been assaulted; someone they knew perpetrated 3 in 10 incidences of assaults against children and youth (Wallace, 2009). Approximately, 3 to 10 million children witness family violence each year (National Center for Children Exposed to Violence, 2006). Experiencing or witnessing aggression and/or violence in one’s family of origin increases the likelihood of experiencing it in future intimate relationships and ones own family (Kwong et al. 2003). Parenting practices that include interactions that involve yelling and threatening, were associated with many types of disruptive behavior problems such as aggressive behavior (Sheehan & Watson, 2007). As shown above being exposed to aggression and experiencing aggression both have detrimental affects on the lives of children with affects that stay with these children throughout their lives. The experience or effects of violence may differ by the form of aggression (e.g., physical/verbal) or the source of the violence within the family. Specifically, children may experience physical aggression that includes hitting, kicking and punching, verbal aggression including threats, name-calling, or emotion aggression such as intimidation, control, or hurting the victim psychologically. Children may also experience aggression in the family from different sources (e.g., parents/siblings). 17 Parent Violence Witnessing Aggression/Violence Domestic violence is widespread. The Center for Disease Control (2005) states that 1 in 4 women, and 1 in 9 men in the United States are victim of domestic violence at some point in their lives. There are an estimated 3.2 million children per year who witness incidences of domestic violence. Individuals who observe parental hitting during childhood had twice the likelihood of experiencing severe marital aggression compared to individuals who did not report aggression in the home (Kwong, 2003; Jackson 1996). Marital conflict negatively affects family functioning and sibling relationships (Cox, Paley, & Harter, 2001). According to the Social Learning perspective, parental aggression during childhood is related to later relationship aggression through the influences of modeling (Chermack & Walton, 1999; Owens, Thompson, Shaffer, Jackson, & Kaslow, 2009). Individuals exposed to marital violence during childhood are more likely to justify the use of violence in dating relationships (Hare et al., 2009; Jackson, 1996). Research has been conducted examining marital conflict in the family of origin and affect adult relationships (Hare, et al., 2009; Jackson, 1996; Jenkins, 2005). Poor parental modeling of problem solving skills provides little opportunity for children to develop these skills. Witnessing inter-parental aggression has negative effects on children’s social processing functions and over time this causes children to learn this behavior and take on this as a stable personality attribute. Repeatedly witnessing destructive inter-parental conflicts on children has an effect on mental representations of 18 how conflicts should be handled in adulthood (Kennedy, Bolger, & Shrout, 2002). Children who observe aggression between their parents may perceive aggression and conflict as threatening, and develop difficulty regulating their own emotions with interpersonal conflict (Margolin & Gordis, 2004; Owen et al, 2009). The next subsection will discuss childhood experiences of aggression from parents and the effects it has on children. Linder and Collins (2005) conducted research on parent and peer predictors of physical aggression and conflict management in romantic relationships. In this study there were 121 participants from a larger study, 21 or 23 years of age. Findings were that individuals who witnessed or experienced violence in their families learned aggressive behaviors from modeling and may become victims or violent towards an intimate partner (Linder & Collins, 2005). Experiencing Aggression Many children experience aggression from their parents in the home. Each year there are approximately two million children that are abused and/or neglected (Giant, 2003). Received aggression from parents is related to aggression in non-dating/ spousal relationships, but observing parental aggression was related to dating aggression (Chermack & Walton, 1999). Parents’ use of physical aggression is normative, and occurs in over 85% of families (Slep & O’Leary, 2007). Researchers also found that males and females who experienced high levels of physical abuse as children were more likely to engage in violence against an intimate partner and children as adults than individuals who did not have these childhood experiences (Felson & Lane, 2009; Karley, 19 2010). Inmates who have been physically abused as children are more likely to engage in assaultive violence targeting adults (Felson & Lane, 2009). White and Widom (2003) found a significant relationship between childhood abuse and neglect and Intimate Partner Violence for both men and women. White and Widom (2003) also found higher rates of domestic violence perpetration among women compared to men. Children with a history of physical abuse, sexual abuse, or a mother who was battered are almost four times more likely to be perpetrators and victims of family violence in adulthood (Karley, 2010). Studies indicate that juveniles with conduct disorders report higher incidents of traumatic experiences (Vandenberg & Marsh, 2009). Vandenber and Marsh (2009), found that aggressive adults had higher percentages of traumatic history. Research found that women rather than men reported being beaten as children and to perceive them abused. Researchers found victimization from mothers predicted marital perpetration whearas victimization from fathers’ predicted marital victimization (Simmons, Lehmann, & Craun, 2008). Physical punishment suggests that it is permissible to hit, slap, or use force under certain conditions. Parental physical punishment may provoke aggression or violence in children (Kuppens, Grietens et al., 2009). Children will believe that it is appropriate to use physical force against others (Hoffman & Edwards, 2004). Parents are not the only influence on their children. Siblings are very influential and can have lasting effect o their siblings. 20 Siblings Witnessing Aggression Siblings tend to behave similarly to one another during childhood. Patterns of aggressive behavior of siblings increase the likelihood that younger siblings will adopt similar behavior over time (Perlman, Garfinkel, & Turrell, 2007). Children learn from other children by observing their behaviors and seeing the results that come from that type of behavior (Slomkowski, Rende, Conger, Simons, & Conger, 2001). If a child knows there were no negative consequences, they are more likely to repeat the same behavior. Children who witness their sibling’s aggressive behavior tend to watch for the reinforcements and benefits that their sibling will receive following this type of behavior (Hoffman &Edwards, 2004; Sims, Dodd & Teheda, 2007). Reinforcements have more impact on future actions than punishment has, especially if punishment is delayed. When children witness their siblings acting aggressively and receive reinforcement, such as gaining control of and object or changing a family member’s behavior, they learn that this type of behavior has benefits (Hoffman & Edwards, 2004). Siblings can be the most influential people throughout a lifetime. It has been found that siblings tend to behave similarly to one another; younger siblings typically mimic the behavior of the older sibling (Perlman et al., 2007; Whiteman, McHale, & Crouter, 2007). Sibling influences can be negative and/or positive. Siblings develop similar attributes, attitudes, and behaviors (Whiteman et al., 2007). Siblings are similar to one another due to observational learning and imitation (Perlman, Garfinkel, & Turrell, 2007; Whiteman, McHale, & Crouter, 2007). When older siblings show prosocial 21 behavior such as helping and sharing, younger siblings express the same types of behaviors. Consistent with influences of prosocial behavior, aggressive behavior by older siblings increases the chances that younger siblings with mimic similar behavior. There is evidence that when younger siblings use aggression, the older sibling with also use aggressive behavior (Perlman, Garfinkel, & Turrell, 2007; Snyder, Bank, & Burraston, 2005). There is not much research conducted on witnessing sibling aggression compared to experiencing aggression from siblings. The next section will discuss the effects of experiencing sibling aggression. Experiencing Aggression Research suggests that sibling violence is highly prevalent. Research characterizes sibling relationships as intense, loving and having rivalry. Physical aggression by a sibling is defined as causing physical harm to the other by shoving, hitting, slapping, kicking, biting, pinching, scratching, pulling hair (Bermann, Cutler, Litzenberger, Schwartz, 1994; Garcia et al., 2000; Kettrey & Emery, 2006). Physical aggression that is more severe can include use of objects to inflict harm and injury. Parents report that siblings conflict are one of the most common childhood problems. Kettrey and Emery (2006) reported that almost 76% of American children between the ages of three and 17 who had a sibling at home committed at least one violent act towards their sibling. Even more, 48% of these children engaged in severe violence based on the Conflict Tactics Scale. Age is an important factor to consider in relation to sibling aggression. Children under the age of 8 years tend to use physical aggression to resolve conflict over objects, 22 whereas adolescents between the ages 9-13 years use physical aggression to assert spatial boundaries. Parental treatment towards each child has an impact on sibling relationships. If the family structure supports power imbalances, with the use differential treatment of siblings and lack parental supervision, there is an increased risk for sibling abuse (Kiselica & Morrill-Richards,2007; Sims, Dodd & Tejeda, 2007). Children who bully their siblings do it to gain power because they to have been victimized by their parents, an older sibling, or someone outside the family (Kiselica & Morrill-Richards, 2007). There are many different long-term effects due to sibling violence. Some longterm effects include difficulty with interpersonal relationship, repeating the victim role in other relationships, over-sensitivity, continued self-blame, anger toward perpetrator, and substance abuse (Bermann et al., 1994; Garcia et al., 2000; Kettery, 2006). For example, men who were in treatment programs for anger control issues had reported both having experienced more physical abuse by a sibling and being more physically abusive toward a sibling (Kettery, 2006). Research findings indicate that the rate of sibling violence is highest among children in homes where both sibling and parental abuse are present (Kiselica & Morrill-Richards, 2007). Much research has been conducted leading to the cycle of violence in the house that is a contributing factor to aggression in the home. Kiselica and Morrill-Richards (2007) evidence suggests that a violent family environment has shown to promote sibling abuse as well as a cycle of violence that may be repeated throughout life and across generations (Dunn, Slomskowski, Beardsall, & Rende, 1994). All childhood experiences will make some type of impact on individuals, but not all interactions will have negative effects on children. 23 Related Factors The following sections will look at factors related to violence and aggression within families. The related factors of SES, relationship quality with parents or teachers and addictions may be related to whether aggression exists within family and in adulthood with those who have experienced family aggression in childhood. Socioeconomic Status (SES). Low-SES has been shown to have negative effects on families. Research suggests that 80% of children exposed to violence are low income, 17% of middle-upper class youth have been victims of violence, and 57% of children in Suburban and rural areas have witnessed violence (Berger, 2004). Poverty is one of the most robust and strongest predictors of recurrent intimate partner violence (Davies & Cummings, 1998; Sonis & Langer, 2008). Relationship Quality. Positive relationship qualities have shown to have positive impacts on at-risk youth. Supportive relationships with a parent or persons outside the child’s family may promote resiliency against violence (Behborin & McMahon, 2008). O’Keefe (1998) identified mediating factors between childhood witnessing of aggression and aggressive behavior. Factors that reduced aggressive behavior were good scholastic achievement and a positive relationship with one parent, while factors that increased aggressive behavior included low socioeconomic status, minority status, large family size, harsh parental discipline, and drug or alcohol dependencies. Parental support is associated with positive outcomes in youth. Parental support has been associated with increased self-esteem, school-related interests and academic goal orientation as well as 24 lower levels of conduct problems and drug use (Behborin & McMahon, 2008; Zimmerman, Ramirez-Valles, Zapert, & Maton, 2000). Teacher support provides both emotional and academic support, as well as serves as a positive role model. A close relationship with a teacher has been found to positively increase social, emotional, and academic adjustments (Benborin & McMahon, 2008; Murray & Greenberg, 2000). It was also found that higher perceived support from teachers and school personnel was related to higher grades, fewer suspensions, and lower levels of drug and alcohol use among disadvantaged youth. Addictions. Alcohol is the most widely used and abused drug in the United States. It is the most common drug associated with domestic violence (Mignone, Klostermann, & Fam, 2009 ). According to the US department of Justice (2009), nearly two- thirds of all instances of DV involved alcohol (Mignone et al., 2009). It has been consistently revealed that 50-60% of men seeking alcohol treatment reported one or more episodes of DV (O’Farrell & Murray, 1995; Stewart, 2003). Alcohol is present in a large number of both abusers and victims. Willson, McFarlane, Malecha, Watson, Lemmey, Schultz, Gist, & Fredland (2000) found that 19% of male perpetrators consumed alcohol and 30% both drank and used drugs. The Present Study There are many different factors that may contribute to adult aggression but childhood experiences with aggression may be a key one (Margolin & Gordis, 2004; 25 O’Keefe, 1998). As stated above, when a child witnesses physical aggression in the home, they are considered secondary victims because they too are affected by this act of violence. Individuals who witness aggression as children can either become victims later or are perpetrators of aggression. Children who are exposed to direct violence or aggression in the home may exhibit problem behaviors such as aggression and delinquency (Margolin & Gordis, 2004). The current study is an extension of previous research on aggression. The data collected in this study helps identify sources of childhood experiences with aggression that relate to adult aggression. The research questions that are drawn from the literature and the theoretical framework are (1) Is there a relationship between aggressive childhood experiences (from all sources) and adult aggression? (2) Is there a difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between a community college and an anger management sample? (3) Do factors such as age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency statistically predict adult aggression? People often learn aggression through social models. The purpose of this research is to examine the relationship between aggression in the childhood home with different sources (mother, father, and sibling) not just “parents” and adult aggression. With new information about childhood experiences and how it can lead to adult aggression, researchers may find a way to reduce the amount of violence from adults who have aggressive childhood experiences. 26 Chapter 3 METHODS In this study relationships between childhood experiences and adult aggression were examined. This chapter describes the methods that were used in the study. The research questions are listed first and then the measures are presented. The procedures and information about the sample size are also discussed. Design and Research Questions The purpose of the proposed study is to investigate the relationship between childhood experiences with aggression from different sources (father, mother, sibling, and self) and adult aggression. The three specific research questions are (1) Is there a relationship between aggressive childhood experiences (from all sources) and adult aggression? (2) Is there a difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between a community college and an anger management sample? (3) Do factors such as age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency statistically predict adult aggression? It is hypothesized in the proposed study that individuals who have experiences with childhood aggression in the home will report more aggression in adulthood. It is also hypothesized that a sample from an anger management program will report more childhood aggression exposure than those from a community college sample. Finally, 27 based on the literature review, it is hypothesized that factors like gender (male), employment status (unemployed), income (low), and substance dependency (yes) will be related to more aggression in adulthood. Participants The participants for this study were recruited from two locations. The target group was recruited from domestic violence and anger management facilities in Southern California. There were 60 adults enrolled in the three anger management classes, 38 adults completed the survey (63%). The comparison group consisted of three Sacramento community college classes. There were 72 students enrolled in the three classes at the community college, 43 students completed the survey (60%). The adults represent an ethnically diverse population from low to upper middleclass families. The participants were both male and females ages 18 and older. The results of this study are based on the final total of participants of n = 81. Characteristics of the sample are provided in Table 1. Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Sample % of Anger Management % of Community College Male 50.0 48.8 Female 50.0 51.2 Black 23.7 30.2 28 Mexican 21.1 7.0 White 31.6 3.2 Other 23.6 32.6 Working 58.3 81.4 Dependency 27.8 7.5 Anger 100.0 9.3 Management The participants’ ages for the Anger management participants ranged from 18-64 years, with a mean of 31.11 (SD=11.54). The participants’ ages for the community college ranged from 18-53 years, with a mean of 25.35 (SD= 7.39). The two groups differed in some key characteristics such as average age, prior drug dependency – with anger management group reporting more prior dependence. The community college sample reported more current employment. These variables will be entered in analysis with total aggression in chapter 4. The community college sample also reported “other” as the majority ethnicity, some of these students wrote in mixed ethnicities, others did not state, it is not clear why 33% were classified as other except that the community college may have had a more diverse group. 29 Measures A survey was used to collect information on participants’ past exposure to violence across contexts and possible mediating and moderating factors. The instrument used was a self-report survey and is provided in the Appendix. It included items about relationships between parents and others, relationships between siblings and others, relationships between self and others. It was used to understand the relationships between aggressive experiences as a child and adult aggressive behavior. Demographics. Individuals self-reported information on, age, gender, education during childhood and within the past six months, and dependencies to drugs or alcohol. Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS) (Straus, 1999). The CTS used for this study to test childhood experiences with aggression. Four modified scales from the CTS were included to ask about aggression from multiple sources within the family: Mother, Father, Sibling, and Self. There were 78 items for each individual in the original scale, for this study 28 items were used for each source (mother, father, sibling, self) from the conflict tactic scale to reduce completion time. The CTS was also modified by the rating scale, the original version had 0-7 rating scale. For this study, a rating scale from 1-4 was used. Participants were given possible responses on a 4-point scale that included: (1) Never, (2) Sometimes, (3) Often, (4) Always. Information on each subscale is presented below: Childhood Aggression- Mother-others. An example question was “caused another to pass out from being hit in head during a fight.” The mean was 1.37 (SD=.40) and the internal consistency is good at alpha = .92. 30 Childhood Aggression- Father-others. An example question was “beat up someone.” The mean was 1.48 (SD=.51) and the internal consistency was good with alpha = .95. Childhood Aggression- Sibling-others. An example question used was “Threatened to hit or throw something at another.” The average was 1.60 (SD=.45) and the internal consistency was alpha = .93. Childhood Aggression- Self-others. An example question used was “Caused another to go to the doctor because of a fight.” The mean for the total sample was1.53 (SD=.44) and the internal consistency was alpha = .93. Adult Aggression. The Overt Aggression scale, created by Yudofsky (1996), was used to collect information on current aggression. The scale included questions about overt verbal aggression and physical aggression. The current study included 12 total questions not including fillers. Scores are looked at as verbal aggression and physical aggression in some analyses and combined for total aggression in others. For this study a rating scale from 1-4 was used. Participants were given possible responses on a 4-point scale that included: (1) Never, (2) Sometimes, (3) Often, (4) Always. Information on each subscale is presented below: Total Aggression. There were 12 items included in the scale. A sample item included questions pertaining to aggression “Caused someone to have a sprain, bruise, or small cut because of a fight.” The average for the total scale was 1.50 (SD=.52) and internal consistency with alpha = .89. 31 Physical Aggression. There were 7 items included in the physical aggression subscale. A sample question was “had a broken bone from a fight with another.” The mean was 1.34 (SD=.48) with good internal consistency (alpha = .90). Verbal Aggression. There were 5 items included in the verbal aggression subscale. An example question was “shouted or yelled.” The average on the verbal subscale was mean =1.77(SD=.65) and alpha = .88. Procedures Before collecting any data at the facilities, the researcher talked with the instructors about all aspects of the study, and answered questions. On the day of data collection the participants were given a survey with a consent letter explaining the study with information regarding questions related to childhood experiences of witnessing aggression through family members and experiencing aggression from parents and siblings. Individuals who chose not to participate had the option to stay in the classroom or go outside. Further, they were told that if at any time they felt uncomfortable they may skip questions, mark NP, or stop the questionnaire. Participants were asked not to put names or signatures on the survey to maintain anonymity. Passive consent was used to protect the identity of these adults since aggression can be shameful to participants and this way their anonymity was completely protected. Participants were told that by completing the survey they were giving their consent. All measures were administered at the same time and all participating 32 individuals in the class were read the consent to participate sheet. After participants were finished with their survey they placed it in a manila folder at the front of the classroom. At no point did the school or anger management facility have access to student survey responses. Theses procedures approved by the CSUS Human Subjects committee. Summary This chapter described the methods used to conduct a study of childhood experiences and its relationship to adult aggression. The analysis of these data is reported in the following chapter. The following chapter will examine the hypotheses and look at related factors of gender, SES, relationship quality with parents, and addictions. 33 Chapter 4 RESULTS This chapter reports the results of the analysis of the survey measure as well as the analysis of the research questions presented in Chapter 2. Question 1: Is there a relationship between aggressive childhood experiences and adult aggression? To test whether a relationship exists between aggressive childhood experiences and adult aggression, correlations were run between the childhood experiences from all sources (mother, father, sibling, self) and the total aggression in adulthood (Table 2). Correlations between childhood experiences and adult aggression were significant for all sources of childhood aggression experiences. All correlations were positive which means that high levels of adult aggression were associated with exposure of aggression during childhood from the mother, father and/or siblings. Correlations were also run separately for the community college sample and anger management sample. These correlations are also provided in Table 2. While most of the correlations were significant and positive as with the total aggression for the sample, an interesting finding was that within the anger management group father-mother childhood experiences were not significantly associated with adult aggression (r = .18, p = .34) and for the community college mother-father experiences were not significantly correlated with adult aggression (r = .19, p = .23). 34 Table 2. Correlations Between Childhood Experiences With Aggression and Adult Aggression. Adult Aggression Total Childhood Total Anger Community Sample Management Mother Aggression .50** .60** .19 Father Aggression .33** .18 .42* Sibling Aggression .47** .37* .39* Self Aggression .54** .49** .29 Experiences College Note. *p<.05, **p<.01 Question 2: Is there a difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between the community college and anger management samples? Subjects rated each of the aggression questions including current physical aggression, verbal aggression, Aggression during childhood from mother, father, sibling and self. In order to examine the group differences we did Community College vs. Anger Management, an independent t-test was conducted (See Table 3). Table 3. Independent Samples T-test Comparing Reports of Aggression From Community College and Anger Management Participants 35 Name Type Anger Community Management College t p df d Total Aggression 1.75(0.57) 1.28(0.34) 4.54 .000 79 1.03 Physical Aggression 1.54(0.55) 1.15(0.32) 3.93 .000 79 0.90 Verbal Aggression 2.08(0.71) 1.49(0.45) 4.57 .00 79 1.02 Childhood Aggression 1.46(0.46) Mother 1.29(0.34) 1.97 .05 77 0.43 Childhood Aggression ather 1.62(0.58) Father 1.36(0.43) 2.08 .04 67 0.52 Childhood Aggression 1.77(0.44) Sibling 1.43(0.40) 3.24 .00 64 0.81 Childhood Aggression 1.75(0.45) Self 1.34(0.33) 4.79 .00 79 1.05 Each comparison between anger management and community college showed a significant difference between the groups. Current physical aggression was higher for participants in anger management (M = 1.54, SD=.55), than the control group, (M= 1.15, SD= .32). It was also found that current verbal aggression was higher for anger management participants, (M= 2.08, SD=.71), than for the community college participants, (M=1.49, SD=.45). When comparing reports of childhood aggression between community college participants and participants in anger management, the findings support the hypothesis that the anger management group, shown above to be higher in total current aggression, 36 was also higher in early experiences with aggression at home (as shown in Table 3) for each category including childhood aggression, mothers, fathers, sibling, and self, were reported higher in anger management participants. Question 3: Do factors such as age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency predict adult aggression? In order to examine whether the variables identified by prior literature predicted total aggression in our sample a linear regression analysis was conducted. The variables entered as the independent variables were age, gender, weather they were working (work status), current household income, self reported dependency on substances, and if they have now or ever taken anger management classes. As seen in Table 4, the regression model was significant. Table 4. Regression Predicting Total Aggression B -.11 .13 -.17 -.14 .34 .35 SE .00 .10 .12 .03 .14 .11 Age Gender Work Status Current Income Dependency Anger Management Note. F(6,63) = 7.477, p<.001, Adjusted R2 = .36 P .31 .22 .10 .19 .00 .00 37 Variable for dependency and anger management significantly predicted total aggression. Contrary to the hypothesis, gender was not a significant predictor. This regression explained 36% of variance in total aggression. Conclusion There was significance for each of the following, working (work status), current household income, self-reported dependency on substances, and if they have now or ever taken anger management classes. This study found that there was a relationship between reported aggression in the childhood home and adult aggression for the whole sample. It was also found that individuals who reported more aggression for all sources in childhood also reported higher levels of aggression in adulthood. The following chapter discusses the results in terms of the existing literature. Limitations for the study will also be discussed. 38 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION This study investigated the relationship between childhood experiences and adult aggression. The purpose of this study was to gain a better understanding of why some individuals are more aggressive than others. This research contributes to existing research on aggression by showing that individuals who experience aggression directly and indirectly are more likely to become aggressive as adults than those individuals who have not been exposed (Hare et al., 2009; Jackson, 1996; Jenkins, 2006; Kwong et al., 2003) and provides a better understanding of how aggression varies by gender, exposure, socioeconomic status, and dependency. This chapter will discusses the findings of the research with a focus on, (1) the relationship between aggressive childhood experiences and adult aggression (2) the difference in childhood experiences and adult aggression between the community college and anger management samples (3) age, gender, employment status, income, or substance dependency predict adult aggression. Finally, the limitations, recommendations, and areas of future research are discussed. Aggressive Childhood Experiences and Adult Aggression Consistent with the hypothesis, there was a relationship between reported aggression in the childhood home and adult aggression for the whole sample such that those reporting more aggression for all sources in childhood also reported higher levels of 39 aggression in adulthood. Kwong et al., (2003) found that relationships children who witness or experience aggression may be violent or have aggression issues later in life. Repeatedly witnessing destructive inter-parental conflicts on children has an effect on mental representations of how conflicts should be handled in adulthood (Kennedy, Bolger, & Shrout, 2002). Children who observe aggression between their parents may perceive aggression and conflict as threatening, and develop difficulty regulating their own emotions with interpersonal conflict (Margolin & Gordis, 2004; Owen et al, 2009). While most of the correlations were significant and positive as with the total aggression for the sample, an interesting finding was that within the anger management group aggressive father source childhood experiences were not significantly associated with adult aggression, and for the community college aggressive mother source experiences were not significantly correlated with adult aggression. As found in past research, anger management issues, especially among boys, may result from observed aggressive male role models in the home or fathers modeling aggression in their dealing with conflict with mothers or children (Hare, Miga, & Allen, 2009). It is possible that respondents in the anger management group all were high on father source aggression regardless their adult level of aggression. The present research findings also indicate that the rate of sibling violence was highest among children in homes where both children and parental abuse are present (Dunn et al. 1994; Kiselica & Morrill-Richards, 2007). Much research has been conducted leading to the cycle of violence in the house that is a contributing factor to aggression in the home. Kiselica and Morrill-Richards (2007) found that a violent family 40 environment has shown to promote sibling abuse as well as a cycle of violence that may be repeated throughout life and across generations. It was not surprising that the findings of the current study found a relationship between sibling aggression in the home and the participants’ adult aggression. Childhood Experiences and Adult Aggression Compared Between Community College and Anger Management It is important to study the similarities and differences in childhood aggression and related factors for the anger management and community college samples. It may be that in anger management have some type of issue with aggression as a group and comparing this group with individuals not involved in anger management allows us to compare those with childhood experiences with aggression and those without regardless of whether they were currently in anger management. Of course, this is not a perfect comparison since the anger management group did report, on the whole, more childhood experiences with aggression. When comparing reports of childhood aggression between community college participants and participants in anger management, the findings support the hypothesis that the anger management group, shown above to be higher in total current aggression, was also higher in early experiences with aggression at home for each of the childhood experiences of mothers, fathers, sibling, and self, were reported higher in anger management participants. Individuals in the anger management group reported more experiences of aggression. 41 One point that this study adds to the current discussion on family violence and aggression is comparing physical and verbal aggression in transmission from childhood to adulthood. Current physical aggression was higher for participants in anger management than the control group. Physical aggression questions were asked in regards to whether they had done any of the following in the past six months, such as throwing objects, strike, kick, push, pull hair, and/or caused mild injury to others. It was important to have these questions on the survey in order to find out if the individuals were physically aggressive. It was also found that current verbal aggression or conflicts was higher for anger management participants than for the community college participants. Verbal aggression questions included yelling, cursing in anger, and making threats towards others. These questions were important to ask to find if the individual was verbally aggressive. Aggressive behavior in children is one of several negative outcomes that are associated with physical punishment used by parents. Parenting practices that include interactions that involve yelling and threatening, were associated with many types of disruptive behavior problems such as aggressive behavior (Sheehan & Watson, 2007). Individuals who observe parental hitting during childhood had twice the odds of experiencing severe marital aggression compared to individuals who did not report aggression in the home (Kwong, 2003; Jackson 1996). The theoretical frameworks in the literature review support the idea that individuals who are aggressive most likely have learned the behaviors from some other source. Much research has been conducted on the intergenerational transmission of violence and the social learning theory. These two theories support that witnessing aggression through 42 family is a learned behavior (Bandura, 1965; 1973; Hare et al. 2009; Kwong et al., 2003; Story et al., 2004). Factors Relating to Adult Aggression There was significance for each of the following, working (work status), current household income, self-reported dependency on substances, and if they have now or ever taken anger management classes. This is consistent with findings that indicate that low income and substance dependency are related to aggression (Mignone, 2009; Wilson et al., 2000). In fact, it has been consistently revealed that 50-60% of men seeking alcohol treatment reported one or more episodes of DV (O’Farrell & Murray, 1995; Stewart, 2003). The significant finding for anger management classes may indicate that there is a difference in the combination or interaction of these factors in relating to current aggression. The sample size was not big enough to statistically test for interactions and more complex models but future research should examine these relationships. Contrary to hypothesis, gender was not a significant predictor. Past research has also found that there is a difference in gender and the likelihood of being an aggressor. For males, low socioeconomic status and exposure to violence influenced later adult aggression (Owens et al., 2009). For females, witnessing high levels of parental violence and experiencing child abuse had effects on adult aggression (Sims et al., 2008). One associated factor that was not assessed was supportive relationships with a parent or persons outside the child’s family may promote resiliency against effects of 43 violence (Behborin & McMahon, 2008). Some factors that reduce aggressive behavior include, good scholastic achievement and a positive relationship with one parent. Parental support is associated with positive outcomes in youth. Parental support has been associated with increased self-esteem, school-related interests and academic goal orientation as well as lower levels of conduct problems and drug use (Behborin & McMahon, 2008; Zimmerman et al, 2000). There are many factors that influence aggression and the current study supports the past research and theoretical frameworks. The focus of this study was to understand what childhood experience influence later adult aggression. Theory helps support my findings by explaining that most individual behavior is learned by watching other individuals model the behavior and observing whether the model is reinforced or punished (Bandura, 1977). This study supported the hypothesis that individuals who exhibit aggression or excessive anger in adulthood will report witnessing aggression in childhood. This research is relevant to study because aggression comes in all forms. Knowing related factors can help educators and research planners identify possible solutions to help decrease the likelihood of transmitting violence of those individuals who have experienced violence in the home. Future research is needed to examine whether supportive parent relationship can mediate between these factors, childhood exposure to aggression, and adult aggression as suggested by (O’Keefe, 1998). Implications Although the current study reflects a small population of adult who are very 44 aggressive and a non-random community college sample, this study and its finding can be useful to parents, practitioners, and law makers. Practitioners, such as counselors and social workers who work with children could benefit from understanding the warning signs of aggression and that it can come from more than one source. It is also important to keep in mind that childhood experiences can contribute to issues in adulthood so helping young children exposed to violence make connections with adults or build skills known to contribute to resilience like education and involvement with activities outside the home to help encourage other avenues to get aggression out (Berger, 2004; Sonis & Langer, 2008). Parents who have to take parenting classes or domestic violence and anger management should understand the long lasting effects that their behavior has on their children. The quality of relationship with parents can be an important intervening factor between long lasting effects. Finally, policy and lawmakers should note that, as in prior studies, income and lack of resources is clearly associated with aggression. Policies to reduce poverty and support families can reduce child exposure and adult aggression. Limitations It is important to report the limitations of the current study for those interested in doing future research. One limitation was the methods used to collect the data. Self-report surveys were used to collect data for all variables. Questions answered by participants may not have been answered truthfully, therefore may have caused the data to be inaccurate. Future research should include interviews to allow the participant to explain the information they were giving. This will allow researchers to gain a better 45 understanding of what happened during childhood and what was going on currently. This would also allow for some participants to verbalize their feelings. Another limitation was the modification of the CTS. In order to shorten the measure and gather data about aggression from various sources in the family of origin, respondents were asked about mother, father, sibling, and self engaging in various aggressive behaviors but the target of aggression was not identified, so it is unknown if parents were being aggressive toward each other or the children or both. Since both witnessing domestic violence and experiencing child abuse have previously been shown to contribute to adult aggression future research is needed to tease apart the witness vs. target of aggression from different sources within the family and its relationship with adult aggression. A third limitation is that this is a retrospective. Participants were asked to recall their childhood experiences but memory can be vulnerable to deterioration. It is possible that for those who were in anger management aggressive memories were more salient. Also since the data were collected all at the same time and are correlational to conclusions about causality can be drawn, it can only show that there is a relationship between the variables. Last, there are concerns the relatively small sample size. Previous research on aggression has included a much larger number of participants. This study however included 81 total participants but they were from two Anger Management Facilities in Southern California, and from one Community college in Sacramento. With a small number of participants involved in this total study a small percentage reported 46 aggression, which makes group comparisons questionable and may have contributed to small effect sizes. It is also possible that the samples from southern and northern California differed in overall aggression inflating group differences. Future research should include a larger and more diverse population of participants. Despite these limitations, these findings add to our understanding of childhood experiences and its potential consequences for adult aggression. 47 APPENDIX A Information Sheet for Participation Los Rios Community College Consent to Participate in Research You are being asked to participate in research which will be conducted by Sarah Castagnola, a graduate student in Child Development at California State University, Sacramento. The purpose of this study is to investigate the relationships of childhood experiences and aggression in adulthood. This research may provide information to help create awareness on factors that are associated with aggression in childhood and children’s ability to overcome it. If you participate you will be asked to complete a questionnaire about your current background information as well as information about your childhood background. You will also be asked questions about your experiences with aggression in the household as a child, from your mother, father, siblings as well as yourself. The questionnaire may require up to 20 minutes of your time. You have the right to stop the survey at any time or skip any questions you do not want to answer. You may also choose to mark the “Not Participating” box marked “NP,” if you do not want other individuals to know you are not taking the survey. Some questions may ask about illegal activities or past events for which you have unpleasant memories. If you would like to talk to someone after the questionnaire you may call 1800-799-SAFE (7233) a 24-hour free counseling hotline. All information that is obtained in this study will be anonymous and confidential. You will not be asked for your name or signature. Data will be reported in aggregate form and no individuals or schools will be identified. By completing the survey you are giving your consent. Information obtained will not be shown to anyone. For participating in this research, you will receive a raffle ticket for a drawing to win a $10 gift card. If you have any questions about this research, you may contact me Sarah Castagnola at scastagnola84@yahoo.com or my faculty sponsor at CSU Sacramento, Dr. Juliana Raskauskas, phone 916-278-7029 or email jraskauskas@csus.edu Your participation in this research is entirely voluntary. Your signature is not required, by completing and submitting the survey you are agreeing to participate in this research. Thank you, Sarah Castagnola 48 Psychological Health Services Consent to Participate in Research You are being asked to participate in research which will be conducted by Sarah Castagnola, a graduate student in Child Development at California State University, Sacramento. The purpose of this study is to investigate the relationships of childhood experiences and aggression in adulthood. This research may provide information to help create awareness on factors that are associated with aggression in childhood and children’s ability to overcome it. If you participate you will be asked to complete a questionnaire about your current background information as well as information about your childhood background. You will also be asked questions about your experiences with aggression in the household as a child, from your mother, father, siblings as well as yourself. The questionnaire may require up to 20 minutes of your time. You have the right to stop the survey at any time or skip any questions you do not want to answer. You may also choose to mark or circle the Not participating box marked NP, if you do not want other individuals to know you are not taking the survey. Some questions may ask about illegal activities or past events for which you have unpleasant memories. . If you would like to talk to someone after the questionnaire you may call 1800-799-SAFE (7233) a 24-hour free counseling hotline. All information that is obtained in this study will be anonymous and confidential. You will not be asked for your name or signature. Data will be reported in aggregate form and no individuals or schools will be identified. By completing the survey you are giving your consent. Information obtained will not be shown to anyone. For participating in this research, you will receive a raffle ticket for a drawing to win a $10 gift card. If you have any questions about this research, you may contact me Sarah Castagnola at scastagnola84@yahoo.com or my faculty sponsor at CSU Sacramento, Dr. Juliana Raskauskas, phone 916-278-7029 or email jraskauskas@csus.edu Your participation in this research is entirely voluntary. Your signature is not required, by completing and submitting the survey you are agreeing to participate in this research. Thank you, Sarah Castagnola 49 APPENDIX B Aggression Survey Thank you for agreeing to complete this survey. Before you begin the survey please complete the personal information below. This information is important for this study. Remember all information you provide here and on the survey will be kept confidential. If you are choosing not to participate, please select NP for each answer. Current Information What is your age? ____________________________ What is your gender (circle)? Male Female NP What was the highest level of education you have completed? _____Less than high school _____NP _____ High School _____ Some College _____ Bachelors Degree _____ Master’s Degree _____ Doctoral Degree _____ Professional Degree _____ Other (specify) _______________________________________________________________________ What is your race (eg. Asian, Caucasian, African American)? _____________________________________________________________________ What is your current occupation status? _____Working ____NP _____ Not working _____ Other If working, what is your occupation? ______________________________________________________ What is your total household income, including all earners in your household? _____Under $20,000 _____NP 50 _____$20,001 to 35,000 _____$35,001 to $50,000 _____$50,001 to $75,000 _____$75,001 to $100,000 _____$100,001 to $125,000 _____$125,001 to $150,000 _____$150,001 or more Do you have any dependency on alcohol or other drugs? _____Yes ____NP _____No Childhood Information- up to 12 years of age What was your parents’ total household income as a child? _____Under $20,000 ____NP _____$20,001 to 35,000 _____$35,001 to $50,000 _____$50,001 to $75,000 _____$75,001 to $100,000 _____$100,001 to $125,000 _____$125,001 to $150,000 _____$150,001 or more _____ Unknown What was your relationship like with your mother as a child? Please circle the response to rate the quality of your relationship with your mother. Not close 1 Somewhat close 2 Close 3 Very close 4 Not applicable 0 NP X What was your relationship like with your father as a child? Please circle the response to rate the quality of your relationship with your father. Not close 1 Somewhat close 2 Close 3 Very close 4 Not applicable 0 NP X 51 What was your relationship like with your siblings as a child? Please circle the response to rate the quality of your relationship with your sibling. Not close 1 Somewhat close 2 Close 3 Very close 4 Not applicable 0 NP X Were your parents actively involved with you during your childhood? Please rate your parents involvement on a scale from 1-4. Not involved Somewhat involved Involved 1 2 3 Extremely involved Not Applicable NP 4 0 X Mother Father Education Extracurricular activities Social Groups- friends How dependable were these community members to you during childhood. Please rate each community member’s support on a scale from 1-4. Never Sometimes Often Always Not Applicable NP 1 2 3 4 0 X Teachers Counselors Parents Grandparents Coaches What was your involvement in extra curricular activities during your childhood/adolescence? Have you ever participated in anger management or domestic violence classes. Please circle one. Yes No NP Please continue to the next page 52 These are questions about what it was like for you during childhood within the family. For each question there are four individuals to rate. Rate each individual on a scale from 1-4. Mark NP with an X if you are not participating. Never Sometimes Often Always 1 2 3 4 Mother Showed they cared when they disagreed Explained their side of a disagreement Insulted or swore at others Threw something that could hurt another Twisted another’s arm or hair Caused someone to have a sprain, bruise, or small cut because of a fight Showed respect for others feelings about an issue Pushed or shoved another Used a knife or gun on another Caused another to pass out from being hit on the head during a fight Called others fat and ugly Punched or hit another with an object Destroyed something belonging to another person Caused another to go to the doctor because of a fight Choked someone Shouted or yelled Slammed another against a wall Said they could work out problems Beat up someone Grabbed someone Stomped out of the room/house/yard when they had a disagreement Slapped someone Had a broken bone from a fight with another Suggested a compromise to a disagreement Burned another on purpose Did something to spite another Threatened to hit or throw something at another Felt Physical pain the next day because of fight Kicked another Agreed to try a solution to a disagreement suggested by another Father Sibling Self NP 53 Please answer these questions about yourself currently. How frequently have you done the following in the past 6 months. Rate each question on a scale from 1-4. Mark NP with an X if you are not participating. Never 1 Sometimes 2 Often 3 Always 4 SELF Make loud noises, shout angrily Yell mild personal insults (e.g. “You’re Stupid”) Curse viciously, uses foul language in anger, makes moderate threats to others or self Make clear threats of violence toward others or self Use yelling or pillow hitting to reduce anger. Slam doors, scatter clothing, make a mess Throw objects down, kick furniture without breaking it Break objects, smash windows Do something to reduce anxiety or anger (take a walk, mediation, etc.) Set fires, throw objects dangerously Make threatening gesture, swing at people, grab at another’s clothes Strike, kick, push, pull hair (without injury to self) Attack others, cause mild to moderate physical injury (bruises, sprain, welts) Attack others, cause severe physical injury (broken bones, deep lacerations, internal injury) Talk to someone else about feelings of anger Thank you for completing this survey NP 54 REFERENCES Bandura, A. (1965). Influence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3-11. Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1977) Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change. Psychology Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G.V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 17(2), 364-374. Bandura, A. (2001) Social cognitive theory: An agentive perspective, Annual Review of Psychology, 52 (1), 1-26. Bandura, A. (2006). Social cognitive theory. In S. Rogelberg (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Industrial/Organizational Psychology. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Benhorin, S. & McMahon,S.D. (2008). Exposure to violence and aggression: Protective roles of social support among urban African American youth. Journal of community psychology, 36 (6), 723-743. Berger, L. M. (2004). Income, family structure, and maltreatment risk. Child and Youth Services Review, 26, 725–748. Bermann, S.G., Cutler, S., Litzenberger, B., Schwartz, W. (1994). Perceived conflict and violence in childhood sibling relationships and later emotional adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 81(1), 85-97. Boyatzis, C., Matillo, G., Nesbitt, K. (1995). Effects of “The mighty morphin power rangers” on children’s aggression with peers. Child Study Journal, 25(1). Cappell, C., & Heiner, R. B. (1990). The intergenerational transmission of family aggression. Journal of Family Violence, 5, 135–152. 55 Center for Disease Control. (2008a). Child Maltreatment. Retrieved March 28, 2009, from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/pubs/cm09/index.htm Center for Disease Control. (2008b). Adverse Health Conditions and Health Risk Behaviors Associated with Intimate Partner Violence. Retrieved March 28, 2009, from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5705a1.htm Center for Disease Control. (2007). The Cost of Violence in the United States. Retrieved March 28, 2009, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/factsheets/CostOfViolence.htm Chapell, M.S., Hasselman, S.L., Kitchin, T., Lomon, S.N., MacIver, K.W., & Sarullo, P. (2006). Bullying in elementary school, high school, and college. Adolescence, 41. Chermack, S., Walton, M. (1999). The relationship between family aggression history and expressed aggression among college males. Aggrissive Behavior, 25(4), 255267. Coie, J.D., & Dodge, K.A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology. 3, 779-862. Corvo, K. (2006). Violence, separation, and loss in the families of origin of domestically violent men. Journal of Family Violence, 21(2). Cox, M. J., Paley, B. & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conflict and parent–child relationships. In J. Grych & F. Fincham (Eds.), Child development and interparental conflict (pp. 249–272). New York: Cambridge University Press. Davies, P., & Cummings, M. (1998). Exploring children’s emotional security as a mediator of the link between marital relations and child adjustment. Child Development, 69(1), 124-139. Dunn, J., Slomskowski, C., Beardsall, L., & Rende, R. (1994). Adjustment in middle childhood and early adolescence: Links withearlier and contemporary sibling relationships. Child Psychiatry, 35,491–504 Felson, R. B., & Lane, K. J. (2009). Social learning, sexual and physical abuse, and adult crime. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 489-501. Garcia, M., & Shaw, D. (2000). Destructive sibling conflict and the development of conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 44-53. 56 Giant, C., & Vartanian, L.R. (2003). Experiences with parental aggression during childhood and self-concept in adulthood: The importance of subjective perceptions. Journal of Family Violence, 18(6), 361. Grusec, J. E. (1992). Socializing Concern for Others in the Home. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 338-42. Hare, A.L., Miga, E.M. & Allen, J.P. (2009). Intergenerational transmission of aggression in romantic relationships: The moderating role of attachment security. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(6), 808-818. Hoffman, K.L., & Edwards, J.N. (2004). An integrated theoretical model of sibling violence and abuse. Journal of Family Violence, 19, 185-197. Huesmann, L.R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C.L., & Eron, L. (2003). Longitudinal relations between children’s exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 39, 201–221. Jackson, N. A. (1996). Observational experiences of intrapersonal conflict and teenage victimization: A comparative study among spouses and cohabitors. Journal of Family Violence, 11, 191–203. Jenkins, J.M., Simpson, A., Dunn, J., Rasbash, J., & O’Connor, T.G. (2005). Mutual influences of marital conflict and children’s behavior problems: Shared and nonshared family risks. Child Development, 76, 24-39. Kennedy, J., Bolger, N., & Shrout, P. (2002). Witnessing interpearental psychological aggression in childhood: Implications for daily conflict in adult intimate relationships. Journal of Personality, 70(6), 1051-1077. Kettrey, H., & Emery, B. (2010). The discourse of sibling violence. Journal of Family Violence, 21, 407-416. Kiselica, M.S., & Morrill-Richards, M. (2007). Sibling maltreatment: The forgotten abuse. Journal of Counseling & Development, 85. Kokko, K., & Pulkkinen, L. (2005). Stability of aggressive behavior from childhood to middle age in women and men. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 485-497. Kuppens, S., Grietens, H., Onghena, P., & Michiels, D. (2009). Associations between parental control and children’s overt relational aggression. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 607-63. 57 Kwong, M., Bartholomew, K., Henderson, A., & Trinke, S. (2003). The intergenerational transmission of relationship violence. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 288-301. Lehmann, P., & Simmons, C.A. (2008). Children exposed to domestic violence: Assessment and treatment protocols. In A.R. Roberts. (Eds.), Social Workers Desk Reference. 2nd Ed (pp. 1101-1104). New York: Oxford. Linder, J.R., & Collins, A.W. (2005). Parent and peer predictors of physical aggression and conflict management in romantic relationships in early adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 252-262. MacEwen, K. E. (1994). Refining the intergenerational transmission hypothesis. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 350–365 Margolin, G., Gordis, E.B. (2004). Children’s exposure to violence in the family and community. American Psychological Society, 13(4). Mignone, T., Klostermann, K. & Fam, R. (2009). The relationship between relapse to alcohol and relapse to violence. Family Violence, 24, 497–505 Murray, C., & Greenberg, M.T. (2000). Children’s relationship with teachers and bonds with school: An investigation of patterns and correlates in middle childhood. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 423–445. National Center for Children Exposed to Violence. 2006. Children and Violence, Domestic Violence. Retrieved March 29, 2009 from http://www.nccev.org/violence/domestic.html. O’Farrell, T. J., & Murphy, C. M. (1995). Marital violence before and after alcoholism treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 256–262. O’Keefe, M. (1998). Factors mediating the link between witnessing interparental violence and dating violence. Journal of Family Violence, 13(1), 39-57. Owens, A., Thompson, M., Shaffer, A., Jackson, E., & Kaslow, N. (2009). Family Variables that mediate the relation between intimate partner violence and child adjustment. Journal of Family Violence, 24(7), 433-445. Perlman, M., Garfinkel, D., & Turrell, S. (2007). Parent and sibling influences on the quality of children’s behaviours across the preschool period. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(4), 619-641. 58 Sheehan, M., Watson, M. (2008). Resciprocal influences between maternal discipline techniques and aggression in children and adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 34(3), 245-255. Sims, E., Dodd, V., Tejeda, M. (2008). The relationship between severity of violence in the home and dating violence. Journal of Forensic Nursing, 4(4), 166-173. Slep, A.M.S., & O’Leary, S.G. (2007). Multivariate models of mothers’ and fathers’ aggression toward their children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 739-751. Slomkowski, C., Rende, R., Conger, K.J., Simons, R.L., & Conger, R.D. (2001). Sisters, brothers, and delinquency: Evaluating social influence during early and middle adolescence. Child Development, 72, 271-283. Sonis, J. & Langer, M. (2008). Risk and protective factors for recurrent intimate partner violence in a cohort of low-income inner –city women. Journal of Family Violence, 23(7), 529-538. Stewart, W. (2003). The occurrence of partner physical aggression on days of alcohol consumption: a longitudinal diary study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 41–52. Story, L., Karney, B., Lawrence, E., Bradbury, A., & Thomas, N. (2004). Interpersonal Mediators in the Intergenerational Transmission of Marital Dysfunction. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(3), 519-529. Synder, J., Bank, L., & Burraston, B. (2005). The consequences of antisocial behavior in older male siblings for younger brothers and sisters. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 634-653. U.S. Department of Justice. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2009) Domestic Violence. Retrieved September 19, 2009 from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/cvictgen.htm. Vandenberg, B., & Marsh, U. (2009) Aggression in youths: Child abuse, gender and SES. Journal of Psychology, 11(3). Wallace, M. 2009. “Police-reported crime statistics in Canada, 2008. Juristat, 29(3). White, H.R., & Widom, C.S. (2003). Intimate partner violence among abuse and neglected children in young adulthood: The mediating effects of early aggression, antisocial personality, hostility and alcohol problems. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 332-345. 59 Whiteman, S., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2007). Completing processes of sibling influence: Observational learning sibling deidentification. Social Development, 16(4), 642-661. Willson, P., McFarlane, J., Malecha, A., Watson, K., Lemmey, D., Schultz, P., Gist, J., & Fredland, N. (2000). Severity of violence against women by intimate partner and associated use of alcohol and/or illicit drugs by the perpetrator. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15(9), 996-1009. Yudofsky S. C., Silver J. M., Jackson W., Endicott J. & Williams D. (1996) The overt aggression scale for the objective rating of verbal and physical aggression. American Journal of Psychiatry 143, 35–39. Zimmerman, M.A., Ramirez-Valles, J., Zapert, K.M., & Maton, K.I. (2000). A longitudinal study of stress-buffering effect for African-American male adolescent problem behaviors and mental health. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 17–33.