March 2006] HERPETOLOGICA in a forest: implications for animal communication. American Naturalist 115:381–399. RICHARDS, S. J., AND C. JAMES. 1992. Foot flagging displays of some Australian frogs. Memoirs of the Queensland Museum 32:302. RODGERS, W. A., AND K. M. HOMEWOOD. 1982. Species richness and endemism in the Usambara mountain forests, Tanzania. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 18:197–242. RÖDEL, M.-O. 2000. Herpetofauna of West Africa, Vol. 1. Amphibians of the West African Savanna. Edition Chimaira, Frankfurt, Germany. RÖDEL, M.-O. 2003. The amphibians of Mont Sangbé National Park, Ivory Coast. Salamandra 39:91–110. RÖDEL, M.-O., AND A. C. AGYEI. 2003. Amphibians of the Togo-Volta highlands, eastern Ghana. Salamandra 39: 207–234. ROSENTHAL, G. G., A. S. RAND, AND M. J. RYAN. 2004. The vocal sac as a visual cue in anuran communication: an experimental analysis using video playback. Animal Behaviour 68:55–58. SEXTON, O. J. 1960. Some aspects of the behavior and of the territory of a dendrobatid frog, Protherapis trinitatis. Ecology 41:107–115. 27 WELLS, K. D. 1977. The social behaviour of anuran amphibians. Animal Behaviour 25:666–693. WELLS, K. D. 1980. Social behavior and communication of a dendrobatid frog (Colostethus trinitatis). Herpetologica 36:189–199. WELLS, K. D. 1988. The effect of social interactions on anuran vocal behavior. Pp. 433–454. In B. Fritsch, M. J. Ryan, W. Wilcynski, T. E. Hetherignton and W. Walkowiak (Eds.), The Evolution of the Amphibian Auditory System. John Wiley, New York, New York, U.S.A. WHITE, F. 1983. The Vegetation of Africa. United Nations Education Scientific and Cultural Organization, Paris, France. ZIMMERMAN, H., AND E. ZIMMERMAN. 1988. EthnoTaxonomie und zoogeographische Artengruppenbildung bei Pfeilgiftfröschen (Anura: Dendrobatidae). Salamandra 24:125–160. Accepted: 11 August 2005 Associate Editor: J. Kelly McCoy Herpetologica, 62(1), 2006, 27–36 Ó 2006 by The Herpetologists’ League, Inc. PHENOTYPIC EFFECTS OF THERMAL MEANS AND VARIANCES ON SMOOTH SOFTSHELL TURTLE (APALONE MUTICA) EMBRYOS AND HATCHLINGS MICHAEL A. MULLINS1 1 AND FREDRIC J. JANZEN1,2 Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011, USA ABSTRACT: Temperature is a crucial factor in the development of oviparous organisms. Under natural conditions, the eggs of many species are subjected to changing thermal environments, but most laboratory studies have incubated eggs at constant temperatures. To evaluate the phenotypic effects of different thermal means and variances and to separate temperature effects from maternal effects, eggs from 10 clutches of smooth softshell turtles (Apalone mutica) were equally distributed among six temperature treatments that reflect thermal conditions observed in natural nests: two eggs each at a mean of 28.5 or 32.5 C, with ranges of 6 0, 2, and 4 C. In addition to embryonic traits (change in egg mass, hatching success, and incubation length), we measured and evaluated body size, swimming performance, and righting time of the hatchlings. The interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation exerted a significant influence on eight of the ten traits we measured, indicating that fluctuating temperatures do not have equivalent phenotypic effects at different mean temperatures. Clutch of origin also was responsible for explaining a large fraction of the variation for nearly all but two of the traits. Altogether, these results suggests that clutch effects are pervasive and that thermal effects during embryonic development are complex and deserve further investigation. Key words: Apalone mutica; Clutch effects; Eggs; Incubation temperature; Offspring; Turtle PHENOTYPE is the end result of an amalgam of genetic, maternal, and environmental effects experienced by an embryo during its 2 CORRESPONDENCE: e-mail, fjanzen@iastate.edu development (Ackerman, 1991). Due to the combination of effects involved in the natural development of a phenotype, describing a physical trait in terms of a single causative factor is difficult. In a laboratory setting, 28 HERPETOLOGICA however, conditions can be controlled so that the effects of a single factor can be made apparent free of interaction. Temperature can have large effects on phenotype. Experiments employing different temperature treatments have documented phenotypic effects on traits such as offspring sex, yolk utilization, body size, and posthatching growth in the development of reptile embryos (reviewed in Deeming and Ferguson, 1991). Most previous laboratory experiments concerning temperature and egg development in reptiles have relied on constant-temperature regimes (e.g., Janzen, 1993; Plummer et al., 1994). In their natural setting, however, developing reptilian embryos experience nest temperatures that fluctuate daily, so the results of such constant-temperature treatments may not accurately convey the effects of natural conditions. Insect studies (e.g., Hagstrum and Milliken, 1991) and a few recent reptile studies (reviewed in Ashmore and Janzen, 2003) have demonstrated the potential effects of fluctuating temperatures on embryo development. Ratte (1985) reviewed the numerous differences between the results of insect experiments using fluctuating and constant temperatures. For example, hatching success rates in many species under a regime of fluctuating temperatures differed markedly from rates for eggs reared at a constant temperature with an equivalent mean. At a low mean temperature (20 C), the success rate of the fluctuating-temperature group was much higher (49–65%) than the constant temperature group (18%); at high mean temperatures, the effect was reversed. Thus, the phenotypic effects of thermal variances might not be equal at different thermal means. Another review (Hagstrum and Milliken, 1991) found that constant-temperature studies ‘‘poorly predicted’’ hatching times for 17 insect species incubated at a variety of fluctuatingtemperature regimes. Given this evidence, studies of other taxa that involve differing thermal means and levels of temperature variance should be pursued, especially where fluctuating temperatures are an ecologically relevant phenomenon. We devised a series of embryonic temperature treatments to examine the effects of both mean temperature and temperature variance on traits of the smooth softshell turtle (Apalone [Vol. 62, No. 1 mutica). The eggshells of A. mutica are rigid and act as a natural barrier to environmental variables other than temperature (e.g., humidity, soil water potential) (Packard et al., 1979, 1981). Therefore, incubation temperature and maternal effects (genetics, physiology, behavior) are the primary influences on A. mutica hatchling phenotype. In this experiment, we used a factorial design with two mean temperatures and three levels of temperature fluctuation for each mean to evaluate change in egg mass, incubation length, size and mass at hatching, and ability in performance trials two days post-hatching. This experimental design improves upon that used in Ashmore and Janzen (2003), enabling us to use statistical methods to test whether the phenotypic effects of fluctuating temperatures are the same at different mean temperatures. MATERIALS AND METHODS Egg Collection and Treatment We identified 10 Apalone mutica nests at our study site on a Cedar River sandbar west of Muscatine, Iowa on 17–18 June 2001. All clutches of eggs were deposited within two days of collection, as indicated by track freshness and presence of either a small chalk spot on the eggs or lack of a spot altogether (Janzen, 1993). We removed eggs from the nest cavity, individually numbered them with a permanent marker, and packed them into foam coolers with sand from the nest for transport to environmental chambers at Iowa State University. Upon arrival, eggs were cleaned and weighted to the nearest 0.01 g (‘‘Initial Egg Mass’’). We selected 12 eggs from each clutch at random for this experiment (except for Clutch 9 which had only nine eggs, in which case all were used) for a total of 117 eggs. We then chose two eggs from each clutch at random for placement into one of six temperature treatments that span the range of viable nest conditions: 28.5 6 0, 2, and 4 C and 32.5 6 0, 2, and 4 C. These temperature treatments reflect thermal conditions experienced in natural nests at the Cedar River field site (Mullins, 2002). We arranged these eggs in plastic boxes filled with moistened vermiculite at a water potential of approximately 150 kPa (300 g dry vermiculite plus 337 g water) (Janzen March 2006] HERPETOLOGICA et al., 1990). We then placed each box into its pre-set environmental chamber in the evening of 18 June. During the study, we maintained relatively constant water potential by weighting boxes weekly and adding water as needed. Six Revco incubators (Kendro Laboratory Products) were used to incubate eggs. Additional chambers were not available to provide replication at the treatment level; however, prior research in this (e.g., Janzen et al., 1995) and other labs (e.g., Packard and Packard, 1993) documents that little if any variation in offspring phenotype is attributable to environmental chamber differences. We set treatments with zero degrees of fluctuation around the mean to a constant temperature, while those with two or four degrees of fluctuation shifted temperatures on 12-h cycles. Temperatures within egg boxes were manifested as approximately sinusoidal traces, reflective of the thermal environments of natural nests at the Cedar River field site (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Mullins, 2002). Box positions and orientations were changed weekly within incubators to minimize any potential thermal gradients. Incubator temperatures were calibrated prior to the start of the experiment and were confirmed weekly with external thermometers. Incubation Duration and Hatching Success On day 44 of incubation, we briefly removed eggs from their boxes, brushed them free of vermiculite, and reweighed them (‘‘Final Egg Mass’’). Because A. mutica embryos tend to hatch rapidly following pipping (the initial cracking of the eggshell by the hatchling; F. Janzen, personal observation), we placed bottomless plastic cups around each egg at this time to assure identification of individuals as they hatched. Starting 1 August, we checked boxes three times daily (0800 h, 1400 h, 2000 h) and recorded pipping and hatching times as turtles emerged. In almost all instances, turtles hatched within several hours of pipping, although a few took somewhat longer (12 h), and in one instance, a turtle became stuck halfway out of the egg for ;24 h before we manually assisted it and designated it ‘‘hatched’’. Sixty-six of the 109 turtles that hatched (61%) pipped between the 2000 h and 0800 h checks; in these instances, we recorded pip time as 0000 h and hatch time as 0800 h. Once completely hatched, 29 we removed a turtle from its incubator box and housed it in a plastic cup lined with moistened paper towels. Cups were stored in covered sweaterboxes kept at room temperature (24.5– 28.0 C among days). We dissected eggs that were visibly undeveloped or that had not hatched by 1 September for evidence of development. Two eggs contained embryos that appeared almost fully developed; all others contained no visible evidence of development. Body Size and Performance Two days post-hatching, turtles were cleaned and weighted on an electronic balance to the nearest 0.01 g. We measured carapace length, carapace width at the widest point, and plastron length to the nearest 0.1 mm with dial calipers. We then subjected each turtle to two performance tests: swimming speed and righting time. We conducted trials at room temperature, with consistent lighting, and between 1200 h and 1600 h in attempt to minimize any temporal differences in activity. Room and water temperatures were recorded daily (see above) and turtles were tested in random order. Each turtle experienced three swimming trials, with a 30 min break between each trial. We measured time with a stopwatch over a 1 m interval in a 1.2 m * 0.08 m trough filled with carbon-filtered water to a depth of 2.5 cm, placing a 60 s limit on each trial. Between trials, we returned turtles to their individual cups. At the beginning of each trial, we placed the turtle into the trough behind the start line and then encouraged the animal to swim with a large pair of forceps. We used the forceps to simulate a bird beak and thereby initiate swimming by pecking the trough behind the turtle. Once a turtle was released into the trough, however, we made no physical contact between the forceps and the turtle. In a few instances, turtles refused to swim away from the ‘‘predator.’’ In these cases, if the 60 s time limit expired, we removed turtles from the trough and recorded no time for that trial. Following the completion of three swim trials, we gave all turtles an additional 30 min break before starting the righting trials. We used the fastest swim time for each turtle for further analysis. Righting trials were conducted at room temperature on a 24 * 24 cm board covered with 3M 423Q WetordryÔ sandpaper for a constant-friction surface that simulated a hard- 30 HERPETOLOGICA ened sandbar substrate. Each turtle was flipped five times, with a 15 s break between each trial, to stimulate a defensive righting response. We removed each turtle from its cup, held it plastron-down in our palm until it appeared calm, and then inverted it onto the board. Timing began as the carapace of the turtle touched the board and stopped when the turtle righted itself onto its plastron. We manually righted turtles that failed to flip in 60 s, and recorded no time for that trial. For each turtle, we used the fastest righting time for further analysis. Statistics We performed all statistical analyses in this experiment using JMP version 5.1.1. For all dependent variables except hatching success, we fit the data with a standard least squares model; in the latter case, we fit the data with a nominal logistic model. We evaluated the data with ANCOVAs, treating mean temperature, temperature fluctuation, and their interaction as fixed effects. Where the interaction term was statistically significant, we made posthoc comparisons among the six temperature treatment combinations with Tukey’s HSD test with alpha 5 0.05. We used initial egg mass as the covariate for ANCOVAs of change in egg mass, final egg mass, and hatchling size. We ran two ANCOVAs for each performance test, assessing both carapace length and hatchling mass as separate covariates. In all tests, if the covariate was not significant, we removed it and reran the analysis as a simple ANOVA. All statistical tests also included clutch as a random effect. Consequently, we report the percentage of the total variance in the dependent variable explained by this factor, rather than F- and P-values, which are appropriate only for fixed effects. RESULTS Eggs Eggs from the 10 nests in this experiment tended to be from larger clutches (mean 6 SD 5 18.6 6 3.7, range 5 9–23 eggs) and were slightly heavier (Table 1) than those collected from this same population of A. mutica in previous years. Clutch sizes in 1989, 1990, and 2000 were 14.0 6 4.4, 13.1 6 3.2, and 13.5 6 2.4 eggs, respectively (Ashmore and Janzen, [Vol. 62, No. 1 2003; Janzen, 1993). Initial egg masses in those same years were 6.41 6 0.60 g (1989), 6.55 6 1.07 g (1990), and 6.29 6 0.50 g (2000), respectively (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Janzen, 1993). Clutch size and initial egg mass from this study both were significantly larger than in the 2000 study (t 5 3.28, P , 0.0050 and t 5 88.12, P , 0.0001, respectively). Eggs experienced a small average decline in mass during the course of incubation (Table 1). One egg lost 0.72 g and was excluded from this analysis, as this change was four times greater than any other egg in the experiment (about 20 standard deviations outside the mean). Change in individual egg mass ranged from 0.19 g to 0.04 g. Eggs in the 32.5 6 2 C treatment averaged the greatest loss in mass, whereas eggs in the constant 28.5 C treatment lost the least (about a threefold difference) (Table 1). Initial egg mass did not affect change in egg mass (F1,100 5 2.6769, P 5 0.1050), so the following results derive from a subsequent ANOVA excluding initial egg mass. Change in mass was significantly affected only by an interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation (F2,101 5 3.1381, P 5 0.0476), with the 32.5 6 2 C treatment having a greater decrease in mass than the other five treatments. Clutch of origin explained 19% of the total variance in change in egg mass; neither mean temperature nor temperature fluctuation alone affected change in egg mass (F1,101 5 0.1922, P 5 0.6620 and F2,101 5 1.3047, P 5 0.2758, respectively). Average final egg mass for all 109 viable eggs in this experiment was 6.93 6 0.51 g. Final egg mass was significantly affected by initial egg mass (F1,100 5 12,704.1600, P , 0.0001), such that larger eggs at oviposition were larger at the end of incubation as well, and by an interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation (F2,100 5 3.3230, P 5 0.0401), with eggs from the 32.5 6 2 C treatment being significantly lighter at the end of incubation than eggs in the other five treatments (Table 1). Clutch of origin explained 21% of the total variance in final egg mass. As with change in egg mass, neither mean temperature nor temperature fluctuation alone affected final egg mass (F1,100 5 0.3155, P 5 0.5756 and F2,100 5 1.0772, P 5 0.3444, respectively). 5 4 3 2 (n 5 17) 61.71 6 0.34 5.13 6 0.09 38.41 6 0.30 33.05 6 0.24 27.33 6 0.22 12.91 6 3.09 0.64 6 0.05 (n 5 19) 7.08 6 0.46 0.038 6 0.081 6.95 6 0.01 89.5% 28.5 6 2 C (n 5 19) 62.28 6 0.34 5.14 6 0.09 38.65 6 0.29 33.09 6 0.23 27.30 6 0.21 12.21 6 2.92 0.66 6 0.04 (n 5 20) 6.97 6 0.49 0.033 6 0.079 6.95 6 0.01 95.0% 28.5 6 4 C (n 5 20) 59.10 6 0.33 5.17 6 0.09 37.91 6 0.29 32.78 6 0.23 26.76 6 0.21 9.80 6 2.85 0.56 6 0.04 (n 5 20) 6.89 6 0.53 0.027 6 0.080 6.96 6 0.01 100.0% 32.5 C (n 5 17) 50.38 6 0.34 4.86 6 0.09 37.50 6 0.30 32.77 6 0.24 26.72 6 0.22 17.27 6 3.09 0.65 6 0.05 (n 5 19) 7.01 6 0.45 0.073 6 0.081 6.91 6 0.01 89.5% 32.5 6 2 C 32.5 6 4 C (n 5 17) 52.34 6 0.34 4.87 6 0.09 37.20 6 0.30 32.14 6 0.24 25.99 6 0.22 12.32 6 3.09 0.73 6 0.05 (n 5 19) 6.99 6 0.53 0.043 6 0.082 6.94 6 0.01 89.5% One egg with a change in egg mass of 0.72 g was excluded from the calculations for change in egg mass and final egg mass for the 32.5 6 4 C treatment and for the overall mean. Significantly influenced by initial egg mass. Significantly influenced by mean temperature. Significantly influenced by temperature fluctuation. Significantly influenced by the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation. (n 5 19) 63.26 6 0.34 5.09 6 0.09 38.10 6 0.29 32.72 6 0.23 27.04 6 0.21 13.12 6 2.92 0.70 6 0.04 Hatchlings Incubation length (d)3,4,5 Hatchling mass (g)2,5 Carapace length (mm)2,5 Carapace width (mm) 2,5 Plastron length (mm)2,5 Fastest swimming time (s) Fastest righting time (s)3,5 1 (n 5 20) 6.96 6 0.45 0.023 6 0.008 6.96 6 0.01 95.0% 28.5 C Eggs Initial egg mass (g) Change in mass (g)1,5 Final egg mass (g)1,2,5 Hatching success (%) Variable (n 5 109) 58.35 6 0.49 5.05 6 0.46 37.99 6 1.43 32.77 6 1.13 26.89 6 1.12 12.84 6 12.63 0.66 6 0.19 (n 5 117) 6.98 6 0.48 0.038 6 0.035 6.94 6 0.49 93.2% Overall TABLE 1.—Statistics (units 6 SD) for days to hatch, hatching success, egg and hatchling measurements, and hatchling performance as functions of incubation temperature treatments. Raw means are reported for initial egg mass, change in mass, incubation length, and performance times. Least square means (LSM) are given for final egg mass and all body size measures. LSM could not be estimated for the 32.5 6 4 C treatment because no eggs from clutch 3 hatched therein. For those values, raw means are reported in bold. March 2006] HERPETOLOGICA 31 32 HERPETOLOGICA Hatching Success and Incubation Duration Hatching began on 3 August and lasted until 21 August. Eggs from naturally incubated field nests laid in June 2001 also hatched during this time (Mullins, 2002). Of the 117 eggs used in this experiment, 109 (93.2%) hatched. Hatching success ranged from 89.5% in the 28.5 6 2 C, 32.5 6 2 C, and 32.5 6 4 C treatments to 100% in the constant 32.5 C treatment (Table 1). Under a nominal logistic model, hatching success was not significantly affected by any of the independent variables examined: initial egg mass (Wald V2 5 1.0458, P 5 0.3065), mean temperature (Wald V2 5 0.0020, P 5 0.9636), temperature fluctuation (Wald V2 5 0.3798, P 5 0.8270), the interaction of mean temperature and temperature fluctuation (Wald V2 5 0.2481, P 5 0.8834), and clutch of origin (Wald V2 5 1.3071, P 5 0.9983). Although initial egg mass was not an important effect (F1,93 5 1.3794, P 5 0.2432), all three temperature variables significantly influenced incubation time (mean temperature: F1,94 5 205.9664, P , 0.0001; temperature fluctuation: F2,94 5 13.6276, P , 0.0001; the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation: F2,94 5 163.1050, P , 0.0001). On average, eggs in the 32.5 6 2 C temperature treatment hatched first (mean incubation time 5 50.4 d), while eggs in the constant 28.5 C treatment hatched last (mean incubation time 5 63.2 d) (Table 1). Overall, Tukey’s HSD tests indicated that only the incubation times of eggs in the 28.5 6 2 C and 28.5 6 4 C treatments did not differ significantly from each other. Notably in both mean temperature classes, eggs in 62 C treatments hatched first, followed by those in 64 C treatments. Eggs in constant temperature treatments took longest to hatch. Despite the significant impacts of all these temperature treatments on incubation time, clutch of origin still contributed substantively to this dependent variable, explaining 46% of the variance. Body Size and Performance Initial egg mass and the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation greatly affected the four measures of hatchling size (Table 1). Clutch of origin also contributed substantively to each size variable (hatchling mass: 72% carapace length: 41%; [Vol. 62, No. 1 carapace width: 44%; plastron length: 48%). Neither mean temperature nor temperature fluctuation alone significantly influenced any of the four size variables (hatchling mass: F2,93 5 2.5987, P 5 0.1103 and F2,93 5 0.5840, P 5 0.5597, respectively; carapace length: F2,93 5 0.5064, P 5 0.4785 and F2,93 5 2.0620, P 5 0.1330, respectively; carapace width: F2,93 5 0.0686, P 5 0.7939 and F2,93 5 1.7993, P 5 0.1711, respectively; plastron length: F2,93 5 2.2868, P 5 0.1339 and F2,93 5 1.4377, P 5 0.2427, respectively). With respect to initial egg mass, larger eggs produced significantly larger hatchlings (hatchling mass: F1,93 5 44.8692, P , 0.0001; carapace length: F1,93 5 21.2397, P , 0.0001; carapace width: F1,93 5 34.9544, P , 0.0001; plastron length: F1,93 5 30.8329, P , 0.0001). On the other hand, while the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation significantly influenced all four measures of hatchling size (hatchling mass: F2,93 5 15.6275, P , 0.0001; carapace length: F2,93 5 5.3925, P 5 0.0061; carapace width: F2,93 5 5.8138, P 5 0.0042; plastron length: F2,93 5 8.0350, P 5 0.0006), the impact of the interaction varied among the size variables. Hatchlings from the 32.5 6 2 C and 32.5 6 4 C treatments were significantly lighter than hatchlings from the other four treatments. However, patterns of significant differences among treatments were more complex for the linear measures of size, yielding no consistent pattern. Tukey’s HSD tests for carapace length identified three different groupings: the constant 28.5 C, 28.5 6 2 C, 28.5 6 4 C, and constant 32.5 C group was longer than the constant 28.5 C, constant 32.5 C, and 32.5 6 2 C group, which in turn was longer than the constant 32.5 C, 32.5 6 2 C, and 32.5 6 4 C group. By comparison, a group consisting of hatchlings from the constant 28.5 C, 28.5 6 2 C, 28.5 6 4 C, constant 32.5 C, and 32.5 6 2 C treatments had wider carapaces than a group consisting of hatchlings from the constant 28.5 C, 32.5 6 2 C, and 32.5 6 4 C treatments. Tukey’s HSD tests of plastron length yielded still another pattern: the constant 28.5 C, 28.5 6 2 C, and 28.5 6 4 C group was longer than the constant 28.5 C, constant 32.5 C, and 32.5 6 2 C group, which in turn was longer than hatchlings from the 32.5 6 4 C treatment. To illustrate these March 2006] HERPETOLOGICA inconsistent results further, note that carapace length increased with increasing temperature fluctuation in 28.5 C treatments and decreased with increasing temperature fluctuation in 32.5 C treatments, but that this was not unambiguously true for the other size variables (Table 1). The average swimming time for all turtles in this experiment that performed in at least one of the three 1 m trials (n 5 104) was 15.40 6 9.82 s (mean 6 SD). Five turtles refused to perform in all three trials and were excluded from this analysis. The fastest swimming time was not significantly affected by any of the three temperature variables (mean temperature: F1,94 5 0.7205, P 5 0.3981; temperature fluctuation: F2,94 5 0.0189, P 5 0.9813; the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation: F2,94 5 0.8282, P 5 0.4400) or by either potential covariate (hatchling mass: F1,93 5 0.0004, P 5 0.9832; carapace length: F1,93 5 0.0127, P 5 0.9106). Clutch of origin explained 18% of the total variance in the fastest swimming time. Average righting time of performing hatchlings (n 5 108) was 1.27 6 1.54 s (mean 6 SD). One turtle was unable to flip in all five attempts and was excluded from this analysis. Neither potential covariate significantly affected the fastest righting time (hatchling mass: F1,92 5 0.0047, P 5 0.9453; carapace length: F1,92 5 1.0226, P 5 0.3146) nor did temperature fluctuation (F2,93 5 0.4532, P 5 0.6370). In contrast, both mean temperature and the interaction between mean temperature and temperature fluctuation influenced the fastest righting time (F2,93 5 5.1583, P 5 0.0254 and F2,93 5 3.0913, P 5 0.0501, respectively). Hatchlings from the constant 32.5 C treatment had the fastest righting times, whereas those from the 32.5 6 4 C treatment had the slowest righting times (Table 1), but no comparisons using Tukey’s HSD tests were statistically significant. Clutch of origin explained only about 7% of the variance in fastest righting time. DISCUSSION Numerous studies have been undertaken to elucidate specific effects of temperature on developing organisms. Many previous experiments have incubated eggs at constant temperatures (e.g., Plummer et al., 1994). However, 33 the ecological relevance of these constanttemperature studies has been questioned (Doody, 1999), because natural nests experience daily and seasonal fluctuations in temperature (e.g., Plummer et al., 1994). Peer-reviewed studies explicitly comparing the influence of fluctuating versus constant temperature regimes on phenotypic effects in reptiles are limited (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003). Controlling for clutch effects, we used an improved factorial experimental design to evaluate whether fluctuating incubation temperatures, as well as different mean temperatures, significantly and consistently affected phenotypic traits of embryonic and hatchling smooth softshell turtles (Apalone mutica). We found that incubation temperature was often a significant factor underlying offspring phenotypes, but that its impact was inconsistent among the traits examined. Similarly, clutch effects explained varying quantities of the variance in these traits, ranging from about 50% for incubation length and the measures of body size to about 20% or less for change in egg mass, hatching success, and offspring performance. Eggs Eggs changed very little in mass during this experiment ( 0.038 g on average). This result is consistent with that found in previous research on softshell turtles (Packard et al., 1979, 1981): eggs tend to change little in mass at a variety of substrate water potentials and, in all but the wettest treatments, tend to lose mass (;5% maximum loss in mass in the driest treatment). There was a consistent pattern of change in egg mass within both mean temperature classes in this experiment: 62 C treatments lost the most, 64 C treatments lost an intermediate amount, and constant treatments lost the least (Table 1). The same pattern was observed for incubation length, where eggs in 62 C treatments hatched fastest and eggs in constant treatments hatched slowest for both mean temperature classes. This finding suggests that incubation temperature variance might exert consistent measurable effects on some important embryonic traits. However, the effect of temperature on change in egg mass was so small (hundredths of grams) that its biological 34 HERPETOLOGICA impact is likely to be negligible. Also, because the eggs in the 28.5 C treatments incubated significantly longer (;13 d) than those in the 32.5 C treatments, these cooler eggs could have lost more water than the warmer eggs after the ‘‘Final Egg Mass’’ measurement was taken. Egg and clutch sizes were significantly larger in 2001 than in 1989, 1990, and 2000 at the same field site (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Janzen, 1993); the reason for these increases is unknown. Temperatures at the site stayed exceptionally warm until early November 2000; this long summer may have permitted females to eat more, allowing them to produce larger eggs and clutches in June 2001. Alternatively, larger eggs and clutch sizes may indicate that there were more larger, older females (e.g., Bowden et al., 2004) in the population than in previous years. Whether or not our larger egg and clutch sizes are responsible for the inconsistency (see below) between our results and previous research is unclear. Further study of this population should reveal whether the increased egg and clutch sizes observed in 2001 are temporary or more permanent. Hatching Success and Incubation Duration Hatching success was high and relatively unaffected by the different temperature treatments, similar to previous studies on this population (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Janzen, 1993). Plummer et al. (1994) reported that A. mutica eggs in their study failed to hatch at constant temperatures 36 C, similar to our population in Iowa (M. L. Balk, L. M. Solberg, J. S. Doody, M. V. Plummer, and F. J. Janzen, unpublished data). In insects, continual thermal fluctuations into the upper lethal range, even when the mean temperature was below the lethal range, yielded very high mortality (Ratte, 1985). One therefore might expect that the 32.5 6 4 C treatment would have reduced hatching success, as the high end of this temperature regime (36.5 C) is in the constant temperature ‘‘lethal range’’. However, this treatment yielded only two unhatched eggs. Therefore, detrimental effects of otherwise lethal temperatures were mitigated by fluctuation into lower temperatures, consistent with temperatures observed in natural nests [Vol. 62, No. 1 (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Mullins, 2002; Plummer et al., 1994). Fluctuating temperatures in one treatment (28.5 6 4 C) were in the vicinity of the lower lethal range. Smooth softshell eggs from Arkansas failed to hatch at a constant 24 C (Plummer et al., 1994); those from Minnesota and Iowa hatched but did poorly at 25–26 C (Ewert, 1979; Janzen, 1993). Our lowest temperature reached 24.5 C, yet this treatment did not experience unusual mortality. Thus, fluctuations into both the lower and upper lethal ranges in this experiment were insufficient to have any effect on hatching success. Interestingly, Ratte (1985) observed that dips into the lower lethal range in insects also had little negative effect in terms of survival. Future experiments with greater fluctuation and greater expeditions into both lethal ranges could determine what amount and duration of temperature in the lethal range will produce the reduced hatching success noted in constant-temperature experiments, much as similar experiments have been able to manipulate sex in turtle species with temperature-dependent sex determination (e.g., Wilhoft et al., 1983). Eggs in this study hatched in the same amount of time as naturally incubated eggs in a companion experiment (Mullins, 2002). This similarity suggests that the incubator temperatures have ecological relevance and confirms that laboratory-based experiments can be constructed to broadly reflect ‘‘real-world’’ thermal conditions (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Mullins, 2002). As expected from prior research (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; reviewed in Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Janzen, 1993), turtles from eggs incubated at higher mean temperatures hatched sooner than those from cooler temperature treatments. Similar to change in egg mass, the pattern for hatching across variance treatments was consistent between the two mean temperature classes: treatments with 62 C variance hatched first and zero-variance treatments took the longest to hatch. This result differs somewhat from previous work with A. mutica where developmental duration was shortest at 62 C (with a mean of 30.5 C) but longest at 64 C (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003). The reason for the differences between the two studies is unclear. March 2006] HERPETOLOGICA Body Size and Performance The lower temperature treatments generally produced larger hatchlings for all measurements (Table 1), although this finding was not statistically significant. Even so, this pattern is consistent with findings of previous studies of hatchlings from eggs incubated at different constant temperatures (e.g., Steyermark and Spotila, 2001). At the same time, temperature variance had no consistent effect on body size (see also Ashmore and Janzen, 2003). Indeed, increasing fluctuations tended to produce smaller hatchlings at 32.5 C, but larger hatchlings at 28.5 C (Table 1). Why this should be so is not entirely clear, but this result may reflect the disproportionate impact of higher temperatures on metabolic processes compared to lower temperatures (e.g., Scholander et al., 1953). From this and previous research, it appears that there might be a trade-off between development time and hatchling size. Eggs incubating under warmer conditions hatch sooner, allowing neonates to more quickly leave the nest cavity where they are vulnerable to predation, flooding, etc., but at a smaller size, potentially making them more susceptible to post-emergence predation (Janzen et al., 2000). Nest-site choice might therefore play a variable role in survival of hatchlings among years. In years with late flooding, hatchlings from nests laid in warmer, more variable sites in the nesting area might have a greater chance of survival because they would escape the nest cavity sooner. In drier years, hatchlings from cooler, less variable nest sites might be more likely to survive because they emerge larger and are therefore less subject to post-hatching predation. Future research should determine the relative importance of these possible fitness tradeoffs. Incubation temperature treatments had no significant effect on fastest swimming speed. This outcome is in contrast with prior experiments on the same population in which higher temperatures (Janzen, 1993) and greater temperature variance (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003) resulted in faster mean swimming speeds. These differing results may be caused by our focus here on what we believe to be a more ecologically relevant measure of swimming speed (fastest as opposed to mean). 35 Alternatively, poor building ventilation led to room temperatures that varied from 24.5 C to 28 C between testing days during performance trials. Moreover, temperatures during performance trials in previous studies were lower (24 C and 20 C in Janzen [1993] and 23 C in Ashmore and Janzen [2003]). As warmer temperatures can increase hatchling activity levels like swimming speed (O’Steen and Janzen, 1999), actual trends may have been obscured by an overlying effect of room temperature. In contrast to the findings for swimming speed, temperatures experienced during embryonic development significantly impacted the fastest righting times of the hatchlings. Turtles from the warmer incubation treatments tended to have faster maximal righting times than turtles from the cooler incubation treatments (Table 1). These righting times also slowed as temperature fluctuations increased at 32.5 C, consistent with the results of Ashmore and Janzen (2003), but this same pattern was not evident at 28.5 C. Summary Our experiment yields two main messages. Clutch effects, be they genetic or nongenetic, can wield a significant and widespread influence on the phenotypes of embryonic and hatchling turtles, although their impact varies depending on the particular trait (Ashmore and Janzen, 2003; Janzen et al., 1995; Packard and Packard, 1993; Steyermark and Spotila, 2001). The substrate for microevolutionary change in response to phenotypic selection thus appears to be ample for many traits. These findings also imply that no proper study of reptilian embryonic development can afford to ignore clutch effects in experimental design. At the same time, incubation temperature influenced many phenotypic characteristics that may be related to fitness. However, the impact of increasing thermal variance on these traits was inconsistent: the effects of thermal fluctuations were typically nonlinear or inconsistent when compared between means. Not only do constant incubation temperatures thus provide an incomplete understanding of thermally dependent aspects of development, but also the impact of particular fluctuating temperature regimes varies depending on the mean temperature. Extensive future research 36 HERPETOLOGICA that incorporates these findings will be necessary to appropriately explore the biological effects of fluctuating temperatures that characterize natural developmental conditions. Acknowledgments.—We thank the following people for their assistance with this study: A. Gauger, H. Harms, M. Knutzen, R. Paitz, and J. Rohloff for help with nest location, egg care, running performance tests, and animal care; A. Bronikowski for statistical advice; R. Bowden and C. Eckerman for advice on A. mutica eggs and habits; and R. Ackerman, J. Obrycki, and three anonymous reviewers for suggestions and guidance. The research was conducted in accordance with Iowa State University Animal Care protocol 7-1-4893-J and under scientific collecting permit SC 14 0001 from the Iowa Department of Natural Resources. MAM was supported by a fellowship from the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology graduate program at Iowa State University and by NSF grant DEB-0089680 to FJJ. LITERATURE CITED ACKERMAN, R. A. 1991. Physical factors affecting the water exchange of buried reptile eggs. Pp. 193–211. In D. C. Deeming and M. W. J. Ferguson (Eds.), Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K. ASHMORE, G. M., AND F. J. JANZEN. 2003. Phenotypic variation in smooth softshell turtles (Apalone mutica) from eggs incubated in constant versus fluctuating temperatures. Oecologia 134:182–188. BOWDEN, R. M., H. K. HARMS, R. T. PAITZ, AND F. J. JANZEN. 2004. Does optimal egg size vary with demographic stage because of a physiological constraint? Functional Ecology 18:522–529. DEEMING, D. C., AND M. W. J. FERGUSON. 1991. Physiological effects of incubation temperature on embryonic development in reptiles and birds. Pp. 147–171. In D. C. Deeming and M. W. J. Ferguson (Eds.), Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K. DOODY, J. S. 1999. A test of comparative influences of constant and fluctuating incubation temperatures on phenotypes of hatchling turtles. Chelonian Conversation and Biology 3:529–531. EWERT, M. A. 1979. The embryo and its egg: development and natural history. Pp. 333–413. In M. Harless and H. Morlock (Eds.), Turtles: Perspectives and Research. Wiley, New York, New York, U.S.A. HAGSTRUM, D. W., AND G. A. MILLIKEN. 1991. Modeling differences in insect developmental times between constant and fluctuating temperatures. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 84:369–379. JANZEN, F. J. 1993. The influence of incubation temperature and family on eggs, embryos, and hatchlings of the smooth softshell turtle (Apalone mutica). Physiological Zoology 66:349–373. [Vol. 62, No. 1 JANZEN, F. J., J. C. AST, AND G. L. PAUKSTIS. 1995. Influence of the hydric environment and clutch on eggs and embryos of two sympatric map turtles. Functional Ecology 9:913–922. JANZEN, F. J., J. K. TUCKER, AND G. L. PAUKSTIS. 2000. Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: selection on size of hatchling turtles. Ecology 81: 2290–2304. JANZEN, F. J., G. C. PACKARD, M. J. PACKARD, T. J. BOARDMAN, AND J. R. ZUMBRUNNEN. 1990. Mobilization of lipid and protein by embryonic snapping turtles in wet and dry environments. Journal of Experimental Zoology 255:155–162. MULLINS, M. A. 2002. Temperature and clutch effects on embryos and hatchlings of the smooth softshell turtle (Apalone mutica). M. S. Thesis, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa, U.S.A. O’STEEN, S., AND F. J. JANZEN. 1999. Embryonic temperature affects metabolic compensation and thyroid hormones in hatchling snapping turtles. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 72:520–533. PACKARD, G. C., AND M. J. PACKARD. 1993. Sources of variation in laboratory measurements of water relations of reptilian eggs and embryos. Physiological Zoology 66:115–127. PACKARD, G. C., T. L. TAIGEN, T. J. BOARDMAN, M. J. PACKARD, AND C. R. TRACY. 1979. Changes in mass of softshell turtle (Trionyx spiniferus) eggs incubated on substrates of differing water potential. Herpetologica 35:78–86. PACKARD, G. C., T. L. TAIGEN, M. J. PACKARD, AND T. J. BOARDMAN. 1981. Changes in mass of eggs of softshell turtles (Trionyx spiniferus) incubated under hydric conditions simulating those of natural nests. Journal of Zoology 193:81–90. PLUMMER, M. V., C. E. SHADRIX, AND R. C. COX. 1994. Thermal limits of incubation in embryos of softshell turtles (Apalone mutica). Chelonian Conservation and Biology 1:141–144. RATTE, H. T. 1985. Temperature and insect development. Pp. 33–66. In K. H. Hoffmann (Ed.), Environmental Physiology and Biochemistry of Insects. SpringerVerlag, Berlin, Germany. SCHOLANDER, P. F., W. FLAGG, V. WALTERS, AND L. IRVING. 1953. Climatic adaptation in arctic and tropical poikilotherms. Physiological Zoology 26:67–92. STEYERMARK, A. C., AND J. R. SPOTILA. 2001. Effects of maternal identity and incubation temperature on hatching and hatchling morphology in snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentina. Copeia 2001: 129–135. WILHOFT, D. C., E. HOTALING, AND P. FRANKS. 1983. Effects of temperature on sex determination in embryos of the snapping turtle, Chelydra serpentina. Journal of Herpetology 17:8–42. Accepted: 25 September 2005 Associate Editor: Michael E. Dorcas