Chapter 11 Emotion Emotions as response patterns Consists of 3 types of components: Behavioral – consists of muscular movements that are appropriate to the situation that elicits them Autonomic – facilitate the behaviors and provide quick mobilization of energy for vigorous movement e.g. dog defending its territory, growls and assumes aggressive posture e.g. heart rate increase Hormonal – reinforce the autonomic responses e.g. hormones secreted by the adrenal medulla (epinephrine and NE) further increase blood flow to muscles Fear Research with lab animals The integration of the 3 components of fear appears to be controlled by the amygdala Various nuclei of the amygdala become active when emotionally relevant stimuli are presented Major regions of the amygdala Medial nucleus – receives sensory input, including info about the presence of odors, and relays it to the medial basal forebrain and hypothalamus Lateral nucleus (LA) – receives sensory info from the primary somatosensory cortex, assc. Cortex, thalamus and hippocampal formation; sends projections to basal, accessory basal, and central nucleus of the amygdala Central nucleus (CE) – region of the amygdala that receives sensory info from the basal, lateral, and accessory basal nuclei and projects to a wide variety of regions in the brain; involved in emotional responses; damage to the CE results in a reduction or abolishment of a wide range of emotional behaviors and physiological responses Fear Research with lab animals (con’t) CE important for aversive emotional learning Conditioned emotional response – a classically conditioned response that occurs when a neutral stimulus (e.g. bell) is followed by an aversive stimulus (e.g. shock); usually included autonomic, behavioral, and endocrine components such as changes in heart rate, freezing, and secretion of stress-related hormones However, if an organism can learn a coping response (a response that terminates, avoids, or minimizes an aversive stimulus), the emotional responses will not occur CE necessary for development of a conditioned emotional response Research with humans Lesions of the amygdala decrease people’s emotional responses Damage to the amygdala interferes with the effects of emotions on memory Anger and aggression Species-typical behaviors Many related to reproduction (e.g. gain access to mate) Threat behaviors – a stereotypical species-typical behavior that warns another animal that it may be attacked if it does not flee or show a submissive behavior; displayed more often than actual attacks Defensive behaviors – a species-typical behavior by which an animal defends itself against the threat of another animal Submissive behaviors – a stereotyped behavior shown by an animal in response to threat behavior by another animal; serves to prevent an attack Predation – attack of one animal directed at an individual of another species on which the attacking animals normally preys Anger and aggression Neural control of aggressive behavior Particular muscle movements an animal makes in attacking or defending itself are programmed by neural circuits in the brain stem Activity of these circuits controlled by hypothalamus and amygdala Defensive behavior and predation can be elicited by stimulating the periaqueductal gray (PAG) of the cat’s midbrain The activity of serotonergic synapses inhibits aggression; while destruction of serotonergic axons in the forebrain facilitates aggressive attack 5-HT does not simply inhibit aggression however, it appears to exert a controlling influence on risky behaviors In human subjects, depressed rates of 5-HT release are associated with aggression and other forms of antisocial behavior Functional imaging studies found an association b/t differences in the genes responsible for production of 5-HT transporters and the reaction of people’s amygdala to the viewing of facial expressions of negative emotions Anger and aggression Many investigators believe that impulsive violence is a consequence of faulty emotional regulation The prefrontal cortex plays an important role in recognizing the emotional significance of complex social situations and in regulating our responses to such situations Orbitofrontal cortex – region of the prefrontal cortex at the base of the anterior frontal cortex; its inputs provide it with info about what is happening in the env’t and what plans are being made by the rest of the frontal lobes, and its outputs permit it to affect a variety of behaviors and physiological responses e.g. Phineas Gage – damage to orbitofrontal cortex caused change in personality Hormonal control of aggressive behavior Many instances of aggressive behavior are in some way related to reproduction; thus many forms of aggressive behavior are affected by hormones Aggression in males Inter-male aggressiveness begins around the time of puberty, suggesting that the behavior is controlled by neural circuits that are stimulated by androgens Early androgenization has an organizational effect that stimulates the development of testosterone-sensitive neural circuits that facilitate inter-male aggression Males able to discriminate sex of another animal by pheromones in order to not attack females, only other males Aggression in females Although not as common as in males, inter-female aggression appears to also be facilitated by testosterone While in the womb, female mouse fetuses that are situated b/t two male fetuses have significantly higher levels of testosterone Hormonal control of aggressive behavior Maternal aggression Most parents who actively raise their offspring will vigorously defend them against intruders Usually begins during pregnancy; stimulated by progesterone (like nest building) However, if offspring are removed (e.g. via experimenter), or if the mother’s nipples are surgically removed, will not display aggressive behaviors; this is due to the necessity of either the pups’ odors or suckling stimuli Effects of androgens on human aggressive behavior Boys > girls Socialization definitely effects this difference, but does biology too? Prenatal androgenization increases aggressive behaviors in all species that have been studied, including primates Difficult to study lack of androgens in human subjects; thus, data with androgens in humans not very reliable Primary social effect of androgens may be not on aggression but on dominance However, CORRELATION does not imply CAUSATION! Hormonal control of aggressive behavior Effects of androgens on human aggressive behavior Synthetic hormones given to patients with abnormally low levels of testosterone (hypogonadal syndrome) does not increase aggressiveness However, athletes who take steroids (which include natural androgens) reported to be more hostile and aggressive; although may be other reasons besides androgens for these behaviors Alcohol consumption may interact with the effects of androgen; alcohol intake increases inter-male aggression in dominant squirrel monkeys, but only during mating season Communication of emotions Many species of animals, including our own, communicate their emotions to others by means of postural changes, facial expressions, and nonverbal sounds; they all serve useful social functions Facial expression of emotions Darwin suggested that human expressions evolved from similar expressions in other animals; he said that they are innate, unlearned responses consisting of a complex set of movements, principally of the facial muscles People in different cultures use the same patterns of movement of facial muscles to express a similar emotional state Display rules – a culturally determined rule that modifies the expression of emotion in a particular situation Communication of emotions Neural basis of the communication of emotions Recognition The ability to display one’s emotional state by changes in expression is useful only if other people are able to recognize them Emotional expressions greater when others present Right hemisphere more important than left in comprehension of emotions; found a left-ear and left-visual field advantage in recognition of emotionally related stimuli When a message is heard, the right hemisphere assesses the emotional expression of the voice while the left hemisphere assesses the meaning of the words Patients with R hemisphere lesions have no trouble making emotional judgments about particular situations, but were impaired in judging the emotions conveyed by facial expressions or hand gestures (“Your house seems empty without her” vs. “He scowled”) Most severe damage to the ability to recognize facial emotional expressions was caused by damage to the somatosensory cortex of the R hemisphere Communication of emotions Neural basis of the communication of emotions Recognition (con’t) A possible explanation for this may be that when we see a facial expression of an emotion, we imagine ourselves making that expression Indeed, patients with R hemisphere damage had both somatosensory impairments and impairments in recognition of emotions Amygdala plays a special role in emotional responses; also, patients with amygdala lesions have an impairment in recognizing facial expressions, especially those of fear; however, can still recognize emotions in tone of voice Gaze (i.e. the direction the other person/animal is looking) is important when recognizing facial expressions; helps interpret whether or not expression is aimed at you Damage to basal ganglia disrupts a person’s ability to recognize expressions of disgust Communication of emotions Neural basis of the communication of emotions Expression Automatic and involuntary Genuinely happy smiles involve the contraction of a muscle near the eyes, the lateral part of the orbicularis oculi Facial expressions follow real emotions; difficult to express fully without any emotion Volitional facial paresis – difficulty in moving the facial muscles voluntarily; caused by damage to the face region of the primary motor cortex or its subcortial connections Emotional facial paresis – lack of movement of facial muscles in response to emotions in people who have no difficulty moving these muscles voluntarily; caused by damage to the insular prefrontal cortex, subcortical white matter of the frontal lobe, or parts of the thalamus Anterior cingulate cortex may be involved in the muscular movements that produce laughter; damage to this area impairs ability to understand and be amused by jokes R hemisphere not only helps in recognizing emotion, but also more important for expressing them; when people show emotions, the left side of face usually makes a more intense expression Communication of emotions Neural basis of the communication of emotions Expression (con’t) Wada test – a test that is often performed before brain surgery; verifies the functions of one hemisphere by testing patients while the other hemisphere is anesthetized R hemisphere plays a role in “primary” emotions; i.e. negative emotions Amygdala not involve in emotional expression Feelings of emotions The James-Lange theory A theory of emotion that suggests that behaviors and physiological responses are directly elicited by situations and that feelings of emotions are produced by feedback from these behaviors and responses Our own emotional feelings are based on what we find ourselves doing and on the sensory feedback we receive from the activity of our muscles and internal organs e.g. if we find ourselves trembling, we experience fear However, critiques say that internal organs are relatively insensitive, and could not respond quickly, so feedback from them could account for our feelings of emotions Theory difficult to verify experimentally Feedback from simulated emotions Feedback from the contraction of facial muscles can affect people’s moods and alter the activity of the ANS