Effects of Perceived Gender on Perception of Stereotype Conformity in Micropublishing A thesis submitted to the faculty of San Francisco State University In partial fulfillment of The Requirements for The Degree Masters of Arts In Psychology: Research Psychology by Tameem Jonathan El-Bizri San Francisco, California August 2011 1 Copyright by Tameem Jonathan El-Bizri 2011 2 CERTIFICATION OF APPROVAL I certify that I have read Effects of Perceived Gender on Perception of Stereotype Conformity in Micropublishing by Tameem Jonathan El-Bizri, and that in my opinion this work meets the criteria for approving a thesis submitted in the partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree: Masters of Arts in Psychology: Research Psychology at San Francisco State University Dr. Charlotte Tate Professor of Psychology Dr. Avi Ben-Zeev Professor of Psychology 3 Effects of Perceived Gender on Perception of Stereotype Conformity in Micropublishing Jonathan Tameem El-Bizri San Francisco, California 2011 ABSTRACT: Micropublishing, the primary process provided by social network services, has become the most prevalent form of Computer Based Communication. This experiment examined how gender, perceived within a micropublishing environment, affects perception of personality. After completing an intake survey, 29 participants were ostensibly directed to subscribe to a fellow participant's micropublishing stream on the Twitter.com social network service for two weeks. In actual fact, all participants received identical messages, served from individual twitter accounts, with either a male or female avatar photo. Upon completion, the participants rated their ostensible partners on a subset of Prescriptive and Proscriptive Traits developed by Prentice and Carranza (2002). No significant results were found in the primary measure. Reasons for this finding are discussed. 4 I certify that the Abstract is a correct representation of the content of this thesis. Chair, Thesis Committee Date 5 PREFACE AND ACKNOWLEDGEMENT I would like to thank Professors Tate and Ben Zeev, Tarah Denehy, Katharine Sorenson and the Spring 2011 Colloquium class for providing invaluable guidance, direction and feedback in the refinement of this experiment. iv Table of Contents Introduction .................................................................................................................................................... 1 Perception of presentation in Computer Mediated Communication ............................... 1 Gender in computer mediated communication ......................................................................... 2 Method .............................................................................................................................................................. 5 Participants .................................................................................................................................................. 5 Materials ...................................................................................................................................................... 5 Measures ..................................................................................................................................................... 5 Primary Measure. ............................................................................................................................... 5 Intake process. ..................................................................................................................................... 7 Daily process....................................................................................................................................... 10 Results ............................................................................................................................................................. 11 Exploratory Data Analysis ................................................................................................................. 11 Primary Measures ................................................................................................................................. 12 Secondary Measures ............................................................................................................................ 12 Discussion ...................................................................................................................................................... 13 Future directions .............................................................................................................................. 15 References ..................................................................................................................................................... 16 Appendix A: Tweets Sent ........................................................................................................................ 18 Tables............................................................................................................................................................... 20 Table 1 - Independent Means Test of Individual Trait Scores ........................................... 20 v Table 2 - T-Test of Trait Groups, Female .................................................................................... 22 Table 3 - T-Test of Trait Groups, Male.......................................................................................... 23 vi LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Independent Means Test of Individual Trait Scores.............................22 2. T-Test of Trait Groups, Female......................................................................24 3. T-Test of Trait Groups, Male...........................................................................25 vii LIST OF APPENDICES Page A. List of Tweets Sent................................................................................................... 20 viii Introduction The aim of this study is to explore how gender stereotypes operate in the realm of web-based micropublishing streams. The term micropublishing stream refers to an ongoing inventory of short text messages delivered via social networking and micropublishing sites, such as Facebook, MySpace, Meebo or Twitter. These are known colloquially on the various social network services as “Status Updates” or “Tweets.” Perception of presentation in Computer Mediated Communication Computer mediated communication (CMC) comprises a rapidly growing proportion of social interactions and is consequently the medium for a growing proportion of social perceptions. Micropublishing has become the most prevalent form of CMC, with the Nielsen media research company finding that one in every four minutes online is spent on micropublishing aggregation sites, such as social networks (Nielsen Wire, 2010). The same study found that U.S. individuals spend an average of 6 hours and 13 minutes a month publishing and consuming micropublishing streams on social networking websites, as of May 2010. Additionally, of all U.S. households with internet connectivity, 75% visited a social networking site that month, and that 55% of U.S. adults have one or more social networking accounts. Baron (1998) states that the online world is “a communication domain with its own linguistic structure and social consequences” (p.166). So far, much exploration of this new domain has focused on the presentation of people interacting via CMC tools. Specifically, research has focused on self-presentation on personal web pages (Hua Qian & Scott, 2007) and in social network profiles(Salimkhan, Manago, & Greenfield, 2010), impression management on dating sites (Toma & Hancock, 2010), and presentation behaviour when interacting in online spaces(Hammond, 2010). 1 However, interactions involve not only the presentation of oneself, but also the perception of the presentations made by others, and much less attention has been placed on this 'other half' of the interactive process. From the research performed, it is clear that the impressions we form are not necessarily the ones we might form from conventional interactions (Jacobson, 1999). In regards to certain traits of character, people may be more accurate than when evaluating others, or during real world interactions (Markey & Wells, 2002). It is clear that there are differences in how people perceive others online. The process appears nuanced, and influenced by routine misassumptions that endure after initial orientation to the new communication form. With conflicting results, there is room for further study of the process of personality conception online, to determine where these conceptions spring from, and what biases we may bring to the situation. Gender in computer mediated communication "A preponderance of the literature indicates gender differences in written communication," write Sussman and Tyson (2000). They found that gender based differences in communication transcend differences between new and old mediums of the written form. These differences have also been noted by Mazman (2011) in the use of social networks. Researchers have observed a number of gender-specific networking behaviors in online settings (Bond, 2009), despite a lack of differences between the genders in regards to publishing and interaction frequency in micropublishing environments (Berhane & Torey, 2011). Men and women who utilize these communication mediums do so at similar levels of engagement, but in markedly different ways. This has led some scholars to argue that gender makes a difference (Heil & Piskorski, 2009). What sets genders apart in regards to their interactions online? Gender schema theory may provide us with a model to investigate this phenomenon. According to Sandra Bem’s gender schema theory (Bem, 1974), people have a generalized tendency to understand and process 2 behaviors based on gender-linked associations that constitute the gender schema. This schema works at the level of organizing information about oneself, as Bem (1974) originally argued, and also seems to work in the manners by which people understand each other. Since people present gender-associated behavior when online, it is logical to assume that our conception of others is, in part, mediated by their presentation of gender. This assumption that gender schemas operate in people’s perception of others has been borne out by research. Prentice and Carranza (2002) had participants indicate the typicality and desirability of traits for men and women and the desirability of traits for people in general. They found that the qualities that are expected from each gender are also the ones that are attributed to each gender; gender stereotypes are ascriptive as well as prescriptive. One's online identity is typically comprised of the actions performed online along with the information provided about oneself (Turkle, 1997, p. 177). Typically, this is in the owner information for one’s social network or micropublishing account. Frequently, gender identity is conveyed with an image that is supplied to represent the self in visual form. I expect that manipulation of the gender presented by the avatar and owner information of a micropublishing account will alter the perception of the traits presented by the micropublishing entity according to the gender stereotype expectations expressed by the participants. Prentice and Carranza’s study also provides a framework for my investigation: a subselection of the trait list they developed will be used as my primary measure. This subselection comprises of traits with the largest differences between the genders in expected typical behaviors and in the strength of positive or negative judgment ascribed to the trait. This strength of negative or positive judgment is known as proscriptiveness (negative) or prescriptiveness (positive). 3 Hypothesis 1 The gender identity presented by the avatar and owner information of a micropublishing account will alter the perception of personality traits exhibited by the publishing entity, as measured by the endorsed perception of personality traits selected for their gender-association and prescriptiveness/proscriptiveness. Additionally, I have an opportunity to search for potential sources for any correlation that help to form such preconceptions. Research into entitativity – the perception of human groups as abstractions, apart from their individual members – has shown that “people who believe that human attributes are immutable... are more likely to endorse social stereotypes and to explain [social stereotypes] with reference to innate factors” (Bastian & Haslam, 2006, p. 228). Hypothesis 2 Gender based differences in perception of the micropublishers personality will covary with their endorsement of the strength of the micropublisher’s gender’s entitativity. 4 Method Participants Twenty Nine participants (14 men, 15 women) were recruited from summer classes at San Francisco State, along with online recruitment comprising of opportunity sample from friends of friends of the investigator via http://facebook.com, along with posts to the public news aggregation site http://reddit.com and http://twitter.com. The sample included 22 participants who identified as Caucasian, 3 as Asian, 1 as Latino, 1 East Indian, 1 Mixed Asian, and one participant that declined to state a racial category. Participants were required to be 18 years in age or older, have experience with social networking sites and daily access to the internet for the duration of their participation (two weeks). Materials Using software developed in Flash, PHP and the JavaScript programming languages, along with an SQL database server, and making use of the Twitter Application Programming Interface (API) and the Qualtrics survey administration service, I developed a system for managing and delivering the experiment. The system processes incoming participants, delivers the experimental medium through Twitter.com’s micropublishing platform, and removes members who have completed their participation in the experiment, directing them via email to the debriefing survey and script. Measures Primary Measure. My primary measure is a sub-set of Prentice and Carranza’s (2002) Measure of Prescriptive and Proscriptive Gender Traits. For this study, I have used the 20 traits 5 for each gender where Prentice and Carranza found the largest difference between the prescriptive/proscriptiveness ascribed to the trait for the respective gender as compared to people at large. Since nine of the traits chosen presented a difference for both genders, only 31 individual traits were actually used to provide 20 for each gender. During intake, experimental participants were asked to rate the following question for each trait: "How socially valued do you think each one of the following characteristics is in ADULT AMERICANS, on a scale from 1 (NOT socially valued) to 9 (VERY socially valued)?" performing this rating task for 'Adult American Males', 'Adult American Females' and 'Adult Americans'. During the outtake, they were asked to "think of the personality of the person whose has been sharing their tweets" and rate the same list of traits in regard to this. Prentice and Carranza's original study used the term "valued" rather than "socially valued." However, during pilot studies, multiple participants reported perceiving a sexual charge in this term; they were unclear whether value was intended to imply value as a mate (to themselves, or to a person attracted to the gender in question) or value as a member of society. Consequently, I replaced the term with one considered less ambiguous by the pilot group participants. Secondary Measures. Experimental participants performed an entitativity rating task: rating a list of human groups, such as “members of an orchestra” or “women” on a 1 to 9 scale, with 1 meaning ‘not a group at all’, and 9 meaning ‘very much a group.’ The responses to the human categories representing the male and female genders were the only measure of interest: the remainder of the ten categories were diversionary. The ten categories comprise of a modified subset of the category list used by Lickel, Hamilton, and Sherman (2001) and Pickett and Perrott (2004). 6 Experimental participants also completed the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (Glick & Fiske, 1996), which measures sexist attitudes toward women as a target group, or the Ambivalent Sexism Towards Men Inventory (Glick & Fiske, 1999), dependent on the experimental group to which they were assigned. Specifically, participants who were assigned to interact with a female gendered twitter account being administered the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory, and participants assigned a male gendered twitter account receiving the Ambivalent Sexism Towards Men Inventory. Additional data on demographics, technology use and online social network activity were also collected. Intake process. Participants were directed to a website ( http://quizitwithscience.com/twitterstudy ), which began the intake process and assigned them to either a yoking group or one of two experimental groups. Assignment was dependent on establishing a minimum of five yoking group members. Once this condition was fulfilled, assignment was random between the experimental groups. Additionally, the yoking group, being among the first members recruited for the study, were only active for the first two weeks of the experiment, following which their work was reused. Experimental group intake. Members assigned to the experimental group were directed to a modified version of Prentice and Carranza’s (2002) Measure of Prescriptive and Proscriptive Gender Traits described in the Measures section above. If they failed to complete all the directions within 24 hours, they received an automated message reminding them to do so. Once an experimental participant completed the intake script, they were informed that they would be connected to another participant in the experiment, and 7 instructed to set up a reciprocal 'follow' from their twitter account to this participant's. This instruction meant that they received any messages posted to twitter from this other person in their micropublishing 'stream' (along with the messages published by any other people that they are 'following' on the twitter system). They are to follow this twitter stream for the duration of the experiment. In actual fact, the twitter account they are interacting with is not a fellow participant, but a surrogate for the experimental system. Messages were posted to the account by the system from among those messages vetted by the yoking group. One experimental group received messages from an account with a female avatar. The other received messages from an account with a male avatar: this is my experimental manipulation. Apart from the gender of the avatar, these accounts have been set up to be as identical as possible, with only the gender of the account avatar differing. All the accounts names start with the 'Jamie', followed by a three or four digit number, such as "@Jamie445" and "@Jamie3425". To maintain privacy and believability, each participant interacted with their own experimental twitter account, rather than a single twitter account for each of the two experimental groups: the twitter system would otherwise make the participants visible to each other. The experimental twitter accounts were set up with privacy enabled within the twitter system, so they cannot be viewed in searches or in public twitter spaces. This was to ensure that participants did not come across messages from experimental accounts other than their own in their everyday use of twitter. Yoking group intake. Members assigned to the yoking group were directed to the same Intake briefing and questionnaire as the experimental groups, and then instructed in their daily task for the experiment. When the yoking group members logged into the experimental site to perform their task for the day, they were presented with a web page that 8 mimicked the look and feel of a Twitter account page, showing the messages that were published to that account, alongside the account owners screen name, avatar and information. In addition, a dialog box floating over the page, asked participants to: "Please select the tweet that you think was most likely to be written by the person who tweets are listed on this page." The dialog box also listed three tweets for the participant to select from. In actual fact, the messages listed on the page are those that were selected for use in the experiment, and their selection during the yoking group task each day determined the tweets published on subsequent days. (The messages the yoking group viewed on the initial day of the experiment were selected by the experimenter.) The purpose of this deception was to ensure that yoking groups participants compared the messages they evaluated and selected from previous experiences of online statements and behavior, rather than those of a person of their own invention. This was intended to improve the believability of the twitter messages selected. Like the experimental groups, the yoking group is divided into male and female groups, with each seeing a male or female avatar on their task page, respectively. The messages that the yoking group participants vetted were selected from publicly published tweets in the main twitter timeline, tweet tags local to San Francisco and to the school of San Francisco, and other messages appropriate for the message stream created. Effort was taken to make the messages believable while avoiding topics that could be considered controversial or polarizing, that could consequently effect the study. A complete list of the tweets used in the experiment, along with a timeline of their publication is presented as the appendix. The yoking group members were asked to perform the selection task five times each day, after which they are thanked for their participation and reminded to return for next day’s task. 9 Daily process Daily tally. Each night, at 10 pm, the experimental system evaluated the activities of the day. The system recorded which five of the fifteen messages presented to the yoking group had received the most votes that day, marked these as ready for delivery and determined the number of messages to publish on the following day, along with the timing for those messages. The remaining messages were discarded. The system randomly sends between 1 and 4 messages each day, setting a time between 9am and 9pm to send them, and assigning a tweet to each time. The timing and messages were then reviewed by the experimenter each night, to ensure that tweets with time or date specific contexts (e.g. “Thank god it’s Friday"; "That was a very long morning...”) were sent at an appropriate time. The system then prepared the next set of 15 tweets for evaluation by the yoking group on the following day. At this time, the system also administered the participants by removing those that had completed their two weeks in the experiment, and sending them the debriefing email; by reminding any new participants who have yet to 'follow' the surrogate twitter account assigned to them to do so; and, finally by emailing a report of the day's activity to the experimenter for review. Once two weeks of messages have been sent, the system no longer assigns new times for messages. Instead, it re-publishes the messages in the order, and at the daily times already established. Since no participants is in the experiment for longer than two weeks, this ensures that every participant is sent the same set of messages, though not necessarily starting at the same point in the message queue. A list of the messages sent, and the times they were sent is attached as Appendix A. Message Delivery. 10 The next day, the system delivered the messages to the experimental groups on a schedule described above. Additionally, it sent reminders to members of the experimental group to visit their twitter stream. This is performed at a random time each day. It was hoped that varying the time of the reminder would improve responsiveness to it. Conclusion of the experiment. Participants who had completed their two week stint in the experiment were marked in the database, so they would no longer receive tweets, and received an email directing them to complete the concluding survey and debriefing. The concluding survey asked participants to rate the micropublisher on the subset of items from Prentice and Carranza’s (2002) Measure of Prescriptive and Proscriptive Gender Traits in regards to the micropublisher, as described in the Measures section. Results Exploratory Data Analysis Twenty Nine participants completed the experiment. Fifteen participants (7 women, 9 men) were in the male avatar condition and fourteen participants (8 women, 6 men) were in the female avatar condition. No participant reported spending fewer than two hours online each day. The majority of participants reported that they spend more than three hours. They considered themselves 'experienced' or 'very experienced', compared to their peers, with Social Network Services (M = 1.57, SD = 0.77) and with computers (M = 1.7, SD = .952), Most of the participants were already users of the twitter platform, and 20% considered it their primary social network service. Forty percent used it 'frequently' in addition to additional platform. 11 In order to establish a baseline for the trait ratings that comprise the primary measure, the median between the distribution for male and female ratings for each trait was calculated and the individual participant’s ratings for each item was normalized against this, giving each score as a difference from the mean. This baseline allowed comparison of the differences between the scores in the two groups. Primary Measures An independent samples t-test of the 31 traits measures uncovers two traits with pvalues of less than .05: Business Sense (p = .025) and Helpful (p = .030). Stubborn also approached significance (p = .054). However, considering the number of traits tested, these results are too weak to be considered of any consequence. Type I error rate increase across 31 comparisons requires an adjustment of only reporting effects less than .0016 (.05 divided by 31). To counteract the comparison-wise Type I error rate increase, the traits were grouped into the eight categories originally defined in Prentice and Carranza's paper. These eight categories are summarized in Table 1. Reliability analysis was performed on each group. (Also see Table 1). Five of the eight trait groups presented a Cronbach's alpha between 0.7 and 0.8, and can be considered suitable for averaging into a composite score for the trade group. The three that did not meet this criteria were Male Relaxed Proscriptions, Male Intensified Proscriptions and Female Intensified Proscriptions. Removing Solemn from Male Relaxed Proscription trait list increased the group's Cronbach's Alpha to 0.789. The other two groups could not be made internally consistent in this manner, and were dropped. However, a t-test of the six remaining qualifying trait groupings (see Table 1) also showed no significant differences between the two experimental groups on any of the groupings created. Secondary Measures 12 Since evidence could not be found supporting the primary hypothesis, secondary measures dependent on the primary measure could not be analyzed. Discussion I observed no significant effect from the primary measure of the experiment and must retain the null hypothesis. I will now discuss the limitations of the experiment that may have resulted in this lack of observable effect, presuming that an effect existed to be observed. Most likely, the effect I was trying to measure is simply too weak for the experimental design and the sample size recruited. The fact that only three of the 31 individual measures and none of the 6 combined scores even approached statistical significance indicates that the comparisons suffered from low statistical power. To further illustrate this, a power analysis to determine a priori sample size indicates that for an effect size of d = .30 at 80% power (i.e., being able to detect this effect at this size 4 out of 5 times), I would need a total sample size of N = 352 (or 176 people in each condition). A post hoc power analysis shows that power in my study was 13%-extremely low. Could the experimental medium have been more believable? Maintaining the environmental validity of the experiment hinged on convincing participants that they are interacting with a real person. However, the experimental design, and the environment in which it was performed put limitations on convincing the experiment could be. Since the experiment relies on sending identical messages from two accounts presenting a different gender, any references to gender had to be avoided in the messages themselves. The very absence of gender cues these may have reduced the environmental validity of the experiment. Additionally, the messages that were sent had to be non-specific in nature, not only avoiding comments that would unveil gender, but also avoiding any topics that might polarize participant opinion. 13 Messages also meeting with the requirements of the Institutional Review Board in regards to treatment of human subjects and avoid any comments or messages that could cause distress to the participants. Consequently, the message stream mainly comprised of personal observations and comments on day to day life--not the most stimulating reading--though well within a common use of the micropublishing medium: "pointless babble" comprises 40% of messages published on the twitter service (Pear Analytics, 2009). Avoiding issues that might compromise the deception underlying the experiment may have resulted in presenting the participants with a stilted, one-dimensional personality. However, comments left during the experimental debriefing survey suggest that the process succeeded in creating a believable, though less than genial person. Participants that chose to comment mostly assumed the lack of meaningful messages was due to the person's lack of experience with, or eloquence of use of, the micropublishing medium. One participant indicated, "Clearly my partner was a newbie on twitter." Another commented, "My matched account was boring and plain. Shared empty tidbits of her daily life and I would not have followed her on normal conditions." A third indicated, "I couldn't wait for the study to end so I wouldn't have to read [her tweets] anymore. She is probably a nice person; she was clearly not malicious or meanspirited, but she also was vapid, self-absorbed, and horribly dull. However, this is pretty much the content of the majority of social networks." When participant were asked, "Did you think that any of the messages you were reading were fake messages?" most of the experimental participants responded that they did think so. However 20% (seven participants) indicated that they did not, and ostensibly assuming that the interaction they had was a genuine one, with another participant in the study. Furthermore, due to the wording of the question, I do not know how much of the interaction was unconvincing to other participants. Some participants may have assumed that some of the messages were contrived and that 14 the interaction was otherwise genuine. Since the experiment was able to achieve a level of environmental validity, as far as some of the participants were concerned, any shortcomings in environmental validity may be slight. Nonetheless, believability may still have had played a part in the lack of effect observed. Future directions Repeating the experiment with more participants is most obvious next step to take in this line of inquiry. The effect I am measuring, if present, is too weak for examination with the 29 participants we were able to recruit for this study. Other than the small sample size, the weakest link in the experimental design appears to be the reliance on misdirection to provide the experimental manipulation. Eighty percent of the participants did not feel that the interaction they were having was genuine. An experimental design that analyzed actual interactions between participants, rather than a simulation of interaction, would provide a direct access to the phenomenon being investigated. Five years have passed since the inception of publically available micropublishing services (Smith, 2011). In that time micropublishing has become a mainstay of human communication, with more than 15% of the world's population interacting with micropublishing streams on a daily basis (Nielsen Wire, 2010). This comprises 80% of all internet users, suggesting that as internet availability and usage increases, micropublishing will become a universal form of human communication and expression. As our society’s reliance on computer mediated communication in all its forms increases, so will the relative amount of time people spend communicating and interacting with others through them. And as a consequence, they will increasingly define and transform people’s relationships and people themselves. Understanding how people’s preconceptions affect interactions and how aspects of will likewise increase in importance. 15 References Baron, N. S. (1998). Letters by phone or speech by other means: The linguistics of email. Language and communication, 18, 133–170. Bastian, B. & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2),228-235. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. Bond, B. J. (2009). He posted, she posted: Gender differences in self-disclosure on social network sites. Rocky Mountain Communication Review, 6(2), 29-37. Hammond, C. C. (2010). Self presentation through avatars: The influence of homophily, identifying with the image, and social reflection on choice of avatar. ProQuest Information & Learning, US. Heil, B., & Piskorski, M. (2009). New Twitter research: Men follow men and nobody tweets. Harvard Business Voices. Retrieved January 26, 2011, from Harvardbusiness. org Hua Qian, & Scott, C. R. (2007). Anonymity and Self-Disclosure on Weblogs. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(4), 1428-1451. doi:10.1111/j.10836101.2007.00380.x Jacobson, D. (1999). Impression Formation in Cyberspace: Online Expectations and Offline Experiences in Text-based Virtual Communities. Journal of ComputerMediated Communication, 5(1), 0–0. Lickel, B., Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (2001). Elements of a lay theory of groups: Types of groups, relational styles, and the perception of group entitativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(2), 129. Markey, P. M., & Wells, S. M. (2002). Interpersonal Perception in Internet Chat Rooms. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(2), 134. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2002.2340 16 Mazman, S. G. (2011). GENDER DIFFERENCES IN USING SOCIAL NETWORKS. Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 10(2), 133-139. Nielsen Wire. (2010, June 15). Social Networks/Blogs Now Account for One in Every Four and a Half Minutes Online | Nielsen Wire. Nielsen Wire. Retrieved January 26, 2011, from http://blog.nielsen.com/nielsenwire/global/socialmedia-accounts-for-22-percent-of-time-online/ Pickett, C. L., & Perrott, D. A. (2004). Shall I compare thee? Perceived entitativity and ease of comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(3), 283– 289. Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(4), 269–281. Salimkhan, G., Manago, A. M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2010). The Construction of the Virtual Self on MySpace. Cyberpsychology, 4(1), 1-18. Smith, C. (2011, March 21). Twitter Turns Five! See The First Tweet Ever. Huffington Post. Retrieved October 11, 2011, from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/03/21/twitter-first-tweet-fifthanniversary_n_838280.html Sussman, N. M., & Tyson, D. H. (2000). Sex and power: gender differences in computer-mediated interactions. Computers in Human Behavior, 16(4), 381394. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00020-0 Toma, C. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2010). Looks and Lies: The Role of Physical Attractiveness in Online Dating Self-Presentation and Deception. Communication Research, 37(3), 335-351. doi:10.1177/0093650209356437 Turkle, S. (1997). Life on the Screen: Identity in the Age of the Internet. Simon & Schuster. 17 Appendices Appendix A: Tweets Sent Tweet now THAT was funny. What I learned today: U'll never know something until u ask. I'm hungry. Again. #foodgoesrightthrume Eyes gettin heavy. Trying, trying, trying................. Transmission dies, coworkers quit (3 of them) vacation canceled. Story of my life. Should be 30000 feet in the air instead of this dealing with this still. nothing. Idk what to do with myself.....so bored. Bad mood. Facebook chat never works for me Love how my phone said it was dying for 4 hours today. Smh. hmmmm. got something interesting brewing up tonight. Now what can I write that won't bore the crap out of everyone? #idk im in a good mood :) warm chocolate cakee & a cold glass of milk not the worst way to end the day If I have so much stuff tosay, why can't I ever fit it into a tweet? I'm different, not weird. Sometimes, I forget. I dropped my phone so bad at work today there's hella scratches on my screen protector. thank God I have that on! everyone's asleep and I can't. great. I just had a conversation that felt like it lasted 9 hours. I wish I had the cash for an ipad, why does apple keep making such cool stuff? Lil ceasar: Nastiest pizza ever. Just say no. I want an ipad. Even the one version would do. Studying on my days off from work. Is there ever an end? i need somethin to do tonight...where my friends at? just got home! i still wanna be out..but i enjoyed myself 18 Day Hour Minute 1 19 35 2 2 2 3 18 15 21 12 14 15 23 23 3 17 36 3 3 5 5 5 14 19 10 13 13 20 37 22 17 47 6 13 56 6 16 16 6 7 11 15 43 2 7 9 40 8 8 10 15 57 48 8 8 19 23 41 56 9 21 25 9 10 10 17 18 14 35 46 35 10 11 12 18 47 5 11 23 45 lmao.. life is funny.. OH:\Admit it... You wouldn't know it was your friend's birthday if Facebook didn't alert you.\"" #AmyWinehouse? I saw it coming since that video of her in europe. #kurtcobain all over again :( #goplankintraffic - best twitter meme ever watching a movie w/ my brother & sister. wish we could see eachother more. a hude spider just crawled out of my room.... hopefully that was the only one.. Monday again... 19 11 9 18 12 9 23 12 13 9 15 30 22 14 16 52 14 14 17 14 9 47 Tables Table 1 - Independent Means Test of Individual Trait Scores Independent Means Test of Individual Trait Scores Levene's Test for Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means 95% Confidence Interval Sig. (2- Trait F Sig. T Df Mean Std. Error tailed) Difference Difference of the Difference Lower Upper Interest in children .017 .898 .185 29 .855 .1000000 .5405404 -1.0055292 1.2055292 Loyal .384 .540 .094 29 .926 .0625000 .6638297 -1.2951842 1.4201842 Sensitive .179 .676 .588 29 .561 .3125000 .5315563 -.7746547 1.3996547 Friendly .048 .829 -.777 29 .444 -.3333333 .4290683 -1.2108766 .5442099 7.957 .009 1.455 29 .156 .8250000 .5668843 -.3344085 1.9844085 .000 .983 -.266 29 .792 -.1625000 .6099819 -1.4100531 1.0850531 High Self-esteem 2.306 .140 .662 29 .513 .4083333 .6170452 -.8536657 1.6703324 Common Sense .628 .434 .971 29 .340 .5333333 .5492593 -.5900282 1.6566948 Sense of humor 5.674 .024 -.803 29 .428 -.3583333 .4461172 -1.2707455 .5540789 .192 .665 .616 29 .543 .3916667 .6355357 -.9081498 1.6914832 Impressionable 1.449 .238 1.394 29 .174 .4500000 .3228376 -.2102771 1.1102771 Child-like 1.364 .252 29 .299 -.4083333 .3861807 -1.1981616 .3814949 Shy .017 .898 .657 29 .516 .3625000 .5517035 -.7658604 1.4908604 Cynical .094 .761 -.312 29 .757 -.1916667 .6141511 -1.4477466 1.0644133 Business Sense .605 .443 -.657 29 .516 -.4291667 .6527787 -1.7642491 .9059158 Athletic 9.916 .004 -2.366 29 .025 -.9333333 .3944539 -1.7400821 -.1265845 Leadership ability 2.826 .104 -1.336 29 .192 -.5958333 .4460548 -1.5081179 .3164512 Self-reliant .221 .642 -.176 29 .861 -.0791667 .4495361 -.9985713 .8402380 Dependable .200 .658 -.575 29 .569 -.3333333 .5792550 -1.5180429 .8513762 Happy 1.867 .182 .595 29 .556 .2291667 .3849533 -.5581513 1.0164846 Helpful .815 .374 .300 29 .766 .1708333 .5697090 -.9943524 1.3360191 Clean .267 .609 2.280 29 .030 .8791667 .3855314 .0906664 1.6676669 Intelligent Mature Yielding 1.057 20 Emotional .328 .571 .332 29 .743 .1125000 .3392356 -.5813148 .8063148 Rebellious .221 .642 -.263 29 .795 -.1041667 .3963548 -.9148032 .7064699 Solemn .032 .860 -1.037 29 .308 -.4375000 .4217612 -1.3000986 .4250986 1.039 .316 -.869 29 .392 -.5583333 .6425058 -1.8724052 .7557386 Stubborn .402 .531 -1.048 29 .303 -.4500000 .4294142 -1.3282506 .4282506 Promiscuous .671 .419 -2.009 29 .054 -.9583333 .4769972 -1.9339022 .0172355 Approval Seeking .121 .731 .349 29 .730 .1791667 .5133695 -.8707919 1.2291252 3.432 .074 1.131 29 .267 .5916667 .5233051 -.4786123 1.6619457 Controlling Superstitious 21 Table 2 - T-Test of Trait Groups, Female T-Test of Trait Groups, Female Levene's Test for Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means 95% Confidence Interval of the Trait F Sig. T Df Sig. (2- Mean Std. Error tailed) Difference Difference Difference Lower Upper EnhPre 1.951 .173 -5.212 29 .000 -2.0220259 .3879532 -2.8154792 -1.2285725 RelPre .504 .483 -1.878 29 .070 -.7747414 .4125339 -1.6184680 .0689853 EnhPre = Enhance Prescription RelPre = Relaxed Prescription 22 Table 3 - T-Test of Trait Groups, Male T-Test of Trait Groups, Male Levene's Test for Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means 95% Confidence Interval of the Trait F Sig. T Df Sig. (2- Mean Std. Error tailed) Difference Difference Difference Lower Upper IntPre .008 .929 -.556 28 .583 -.1949405 .3506008 -.9131136 .5232326 RelPre .745 .395 1.206 28 .238 .4946429 .4102780 -.3457736 1.3350593 RelPro .232 .634 -1.046 28 .304 -.3928571 .3754098 -1.1618492 .3761349 IntPro 1.040 .317 .193 28 .849 .0803571 .4174309 -.7747113 .9354256 IntPre = Intensified Prescription RelPre = Relaxed Prescription IntPro = Intensified Proscription RelPro = Relaxed Proscription 23 24