Shank 1 Jonathan Shank Professor Brian Ballentine English 303 19 January 2011 Direction of Our Minds As an individual growing up and evolving in the digital age, it is genuinely fascinating to read about the speculative future of the human brain, and how it is being gradually rewired as a result of perpetual exposure to technology. Interestingly enough, I am doing research and reading about this topic while checking my two email accounts, writing this paper, text messaging, and using notation software to write music for a drumline. I would have normally added Facebook to the list if I had not asked my girlfriend to change my password to prevent that distraction for a while. As an extremely susceptible individual to brain reformation in this technological age, I am inclined to research deeper into the debated categories of hyper and deep attention, cognition, multi-tasking, and also the idea that brains of current generations are being slowly rewired from technology use. Additionally, I will discuss a psychological process that could play a significant role in this change. Because I am submerged in this technological era, it is difficult to objectively side with either stance in the deep attention versus hyper attention controversy; however, I intend to shed some light on both issues in order to gain a deeper understanding of the matter. Hopefully I can do this without being too distracted or tempted by other available electronic activities. First, there are two terms that label and describe two different kinds of cognitive styles dealing with attention. One of these cognitive techniques is deep attention, and Shank 2 that is the style that that allows for clear focus and concentration upon single situations for extended periods of time. Conversely, hyper attention involves quickly changing focus from task to task, usually including various methods of obtaining information and stimulation (Hayles 187). The issue that arises, however, is the controversy regarding the shift from individuals possessing suitable deep attention spans, to developing more of a hyper attention span. This means that the brain is learning and adapting over time to work under such conditions that involve multi-tasking and divided attention (Hayles 187). Multi-tasking, which is defined by Rosen as “…the human attempt to do simultaneously as many things as possible, as quickly as possible...” (105) could perhaps over time result in the aforementioned rewiring of the human brain. Hayles mentions that from an evolutionary standpoint, hyper attention may have developed first. Primitive humans were probably not always in safe environments and attention needed to be distributed between many concerns like staying warm, remaining alert in case of danger, and tending to young ones. However, over time humans have been able to create living conditions that allow for long and interrupted focus on tasks or activities (188). As a proponent of evolution by natural selection, it is easy for me to understand that our brains can be, and are being rewired as a result of increased exposure to technology and the experiences paired with hyper attention, and could also help try to explain why we so often utilize technology. I find myself often simultaneously occupied with various applications on the computer, especially when work needs to be done. When I have a paper to complete, or a reading assignment, it is very difficult to allot all of my attention to a single medium. As I said before, I will usually have several application windows or browser tabs open Shank 3 while attempting to complete any work. My work always gets completed, and I am usually satisfied with the quality, but it usually takes a long time from start to finish because I tend to get bored or distracted when only focusing on a single thing. So I cannot say with certainty that it would be more efficient to complete each task one at a time. Speaking of, Parke and Gauvain mention that a primary characteristic of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the inability to remain focused on a particular task for long periods of time, especially a task requiring concentration (565). One could say that we are inadvertently putting ourselves in environments that promote behaviors that are associated with symptoms of ADHD. According to Nicholas Carr, part of what keeps us using technology is the instant social or informational reinforcement or reward that follows a simple response, such as a click of a mouse, or sending a text message. These little rewards often occur immediately after the response (117). Therefore, it may be easy to understand why people use computers and the internet so frequently, and for such long periods of time. During the moments we are interacting with technology, our senses are being occupied with substantial amounts information. This may also provide concern for some educators regarding whether or not to employ heavy use of technology in the classroom environment. This could relate to a psychological process known as instrumental or operant conditioning which, in simple terms, means if you do something and are either rewarded punished for it, then you will be more, or less likely to do it again in the future, respectively. (Cooper, Heron and Heward 41-42). This process of conditioning involves what is known as a three term contingency. The first of the three is called a discriminative stimulus. All that means is that the presence of this object or thing – the stimulus- evokes a specific type of Shank 4 response, or reaction (Cooper, Heron and Heward 41-42). For example, if you tell your friend a joke and he laughs, the presence of your friend will most likely encourage you to tell other jokes in order to be rewarded with laughter again. In this case, a computer will act as our discriminative stimulus. By clicking an icon with a mouse, typing words into search engines, and checking Facebook, one is acting or responding in a way to get the desired outcome. In this situation, the response is the second term in the three-term contingency. Finally, the third is the reason one would do things like typing and clicking – for the immediate reinforcer, or the reward of instantly finding information or confirming a purchase from a website among other things. So we have the computer that evokes the response of clicking and typing, thus resulting in the reward of information (Cooper, Heron and Heward 41-42). Repeated pairings of computer use with the rewards obtained from performing leisure activities could very well condition an individual to associate that machine with mostly pleasurable experiences. A comparable situation could relate to insomnia. Doing things in bed other than sleeping or sex can cause an individual to associate being in bed with the rewards or pleasures of those other activities, making it more difficult to fall asleep due to those other repeated associations (Mazur 248). In the case of incorporating technology into the learning environment, it allows for the probability that deep attention would be difficult to accomplish when, for example, substituting physical paper books with documents on the computer. This is because of all the other activities requiring hyper attention that have over time been associated with the use of computers and other technology. It seems to me this is where those who favor deep attention and media like books, and those who favor hyper attention and technology must, at least temporarily, reach an Shank 5 impasse. Deep attention has its advantages of being able to focus on specific tasks for long periods of time, but technology is becoming such an essential cog in the inner workings of society that removing it would cause more problems than it would solve. Although I am a part of this generation of technology, I cannot completely side with one or the other. There may certainly be solutions to this dilemma that we have not yet conceived or researched enough to solve this situation. Perhaps it is the exponentially improving technology itself that will allow us to discover and invent the new processes and procedures we must take to put an end to this societal concern. Shank 6 Works Cited Carr, Nicholas G. The shallows: what the Internet is doing to our brains. New York: W.W. Norton, 2010. 117. Print. Cooper, John O., Timothy E. Heron, and William L. Heward. Applied Behavior Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Merrill-Prentice Hall, 2007. 41-42. Print. Hayles, N. K. "Hyper and Deep Attention: the Generational Divide in Cognitive Modes." Profession (2007). 187-188. Print. Mazur, James E. Learning and Behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall, 2006. 132+. Print. Parke, Ross D., and Mary Gauvain. Child Psychology: a Contemporary Viewpoint. Boston: McGraw Hill, 2009. 565-66. Print. Rosen, Christine. "The Myth of Multitasking." The New Atlantis (2008): 1. Print.