+ Refresher in inferential statistics Tim.bates@ed.ac.uk http://www.psy.ed.ac.uk/events/research_seminars/psych stats + Resources http://www.statmethods.net + Our basic question… Did something occur? Importantly, did what we predicted would occur, transpire?, i.e., is the world as we predicted? Why does this require statistics? + Is Breastfeeding good for Baby’s brains? The association between breastfeeding and IQ is moderated by a genetic polymorphism (rs174575) in the FADS2 gene Caspi A et al. PNAS 2007;104:18860-18865 ©2007 by National Academy of Sciences + Overview Hypothesis testing p-values Type I vs. Type II errors Power Correlation Fisher’s exact test T-test Linear regression Non-parametric statistics (mostly for you to go over in your own time) + Hypothesis testing 1. Propose a null and an experimental hypothesis. Mistakes here may make the experiment un-analysable 2. Consider the assumptions of the test: Are they met? Statistical independence of observations Distributions of the observations. Student's t distribution, normal distribution etc. 3. Compute the relevant test statistic. 1. Student’s t-test-> t ; ANOVA F; Chi2 4. Compute likelihood of the test-statistic: 1. Does it exceed your chosen threshold? 2. Either reject (or fail to reject) the null hypothesis + What mistakes can we make? “The World” Yes Yes correct detection Your Decision No false negative No false positive correct rejection + Starting to make inferences…the Binomial Toss a coin + Dropping lots of coins... Pachinko + Normal compared to Binomial n=6 p = .5 + Distributions normal (µ, ∂) binomial (p, n) + Distributions Poisson (lambda) Accidents in a period of time; Power Publication rates + Testing what distribution you have + Why are things normal? + Central limit theorem The mean of a large number of independent random variables is distributed approximately normally. + Hypothesis testing Making statistical decisions using experimental data. Need to form a null hypothesis (we can reject, but not confirm hypotheses) A result is “significant” if it is unlikely to have occurred by chance. Ronald Fisher “We may discover whether a second sample is or is not significantly different from the first”. + What mistakes can we make? “The World” Yes Yes correct detection Your Decision No false negative No false positive correct rejection + Error Type-I error: False Alarm, a bogus effect reject the null hypothesis when it is really true Much of published science is Type-I error (Ioannides, 2008) Type-II error: Miss a real effect Fail to reject our null hypothesis when it is false Many small projects have this problem Type-III error: :-) lazy, incompetent, or willful ignorance of the truth + p-values Almost any difference (a count, a difference in means, a difference in variances) can be found with some probability, irrespective of the true situation. All we can do is to set a threshold likelihood for deciding that an event occurred by chance. p=.05 = 1 time in 20, the result would be as large by chance when the null hypothesis is true. + Type I vs. Type II errors Type I: False positive Likelihood of type 1 = α p=.05 = setting α to .05 World Type II: False negative Likelihood of type 2 = β Power = 1-β Yes You No Yes Correct detection Type I (α) (power) No Type II (β) Correct rejection + P-values p-value is the likelihood of mean differences as large or larger than those observed in the data occurring by chance p-value criteria (alpha ) allow us a binary answer to our questions Questions – is a smaller p-value: “More” significant? Indicate a “Bigger” effect? (if so when?) and how could we measure” effect”? + Compare these two statements It’s ‘significant’, but how big is the effect? I can see it’s big: but what is the p-value? + Confidence Intervals Range of values within a given likelihood threshold (for instance 95%) Closely related to p-values. p = 1-CI i.e., if p<.05, 95% CI will not include 0 (no difference) Would you rather have a CI or a p-value? Why? What is an effect size? + P and CI You can’t go from p to CI! You can go from CI to p At a p=.05, 95%CIs will overlap less than 25% At p= .01, the 95% CI bars just touch + Units of a Confidence Interval Unlike p, CIs are given in the units of the DV Cumming and Finch (2005) BMI in people on a low carb diet might be19-23 kg/m2 Cumming, G. and Finch S.(2005). Inference by eye: confidence intervals and how to read pictures of data. American Psychologist. 60:170-80. PMID: 15740449 + Standard Errors and Standard Deviations SE is (typically) the standard error of the mean The precision with which we have estimated the population mean based on our sample Computationally, it is ∂/sqrt(n) A 95% confidence interval is ± 1.96 SE + Example: coin toss Random sample of 100 coin tosses, of a coin believed to be fair We observed number of 45 heads, and 55 tails: Is the coin fair? + Binomial test binom.test(x=45, n=100, p=.5, alternative="two.sided”) number of successes = 45, number of trials = 100 p-value = 0.3682 alternative hypothesis: true probability of success != 0.5 95 percent confidence interval: 0.3503 0.5527 sample estimates: probability of success: 0.45 + Categorical Data Fisher’s Exact Test Categorical data resulting from classifying objects in one of two ways Tests significance of the observed "contingency" of the two outcomes. Fisher, R. A. (1922). On the interpretation of χ2 from contingency tables, and the calculation of P. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 85(1), 87-94. + The Lady Drinking Tea Question: Does Tea taste better if the milk is added to the tea, or vice versa? Null Hypothesis: The drinker cannot tell Subjects: Ms Bristol Experiment: 8 "trials" (cups): 4 in each way, in random order DV: Milk versus Milk second discrimination Enter data into 2 x 2 contingency table + Fisher Contingency Table Guess Milk Tea Truth Milk Tea 3 1 1 3 A = c(1, 1, 1, 0, 1, 0, 0, 0) # vector of guesses B = c(1, 1, 1, 1, 0, 0, 0, 0) # vector of Teas guessTable <- table(A,B) # contingency table labels = list(Guess = c("Milk", "Tea"), Truth = c("Milk", "Tea")) # make labels dimnames(guessTable)= labels # add label fisher.test(guessTable, alternative = "greater") # test + Can she tell? Fisher's Exact Test for Count Data p-value = 0.24 # association could not be established Alternative hypothesis: true odds ratio is greater than 1 95% confidence interval: 0.313 – Inf Sample odds ratio: 6.40 + What if we have two continuous variables? Are they related Q: If you have continuous depression scores and cut-off scores, which is more powerful? + Correlation of two continuous variables: Pearson’s r All variables continuous Pearson + Correlation: what are the maximum and minimum correlations? + Power (1-β) Probability that a test will correctly reject the null hypothesis. Complement of the false negative rate, β False negative = missing a real effect 1-β = p (correctly reject a false null hypothesis) + Power and how to get it Probability of rejecting the null hypothesis when it is false Whence comes power? + Power applied to a correlation Samples of n=30 from a population in which two normal traits correlate 0.3 r=0.3 xy = mvrnorm (n=30, mu=rep(0,2), Sigma= matrix(c(1,r,r,1) ,nrow=2, ncol=2)); xy = data.frame(xy); names(xy) <- c("x", "y"); qplot(x, y, data = xy, geom = c("point" , "smooth"), method=lm) + Power of a correlation test library(pwr) pwr.r.test(n = 30, r = .3, sig.level = 0.05) n = 30 r = 0.3 sig.level = 0.05 power = 0.359 alternative = two.sided + Power: r = .3 + t-test When we wish to compare means in a sample, we must estimate the standard deviation from the sample Student's t-distribution is the distribution of small samples from normally varying populations + t-distribution function t is defined as the ratio: Z/sqrt(V/v) Z is normally distributed with expected value 0 and variance 1; V has a chi-square distribution with ν degrees of freedom; + Normal and t-distributions Normal is in blue Green = t with df = 1 Red = t with df = 3 (far right = df increasing to 30) + Power of t-test power.t.test(n=15, delta=.5) Two-sample t test power calculation n = 15 ; delta = 0.5 ; sd = 1; sig.level = 0.05 power = 0.26 alternative = two.sided NOTE: n is number in *each* group + Linear regression + Linear regression fit = lm(y ~ x1 + x2 + x3, data=mydata) summary(fit) anova(fit) # show results # anova table coefficients(fit) # model coefficients confint(fit, level=0.95) fitted(fit) # CIs for model parameters # predicted values residuals(fit) # residuals influence(fit) # regression diagnostics + Nonparametric Statistics Timothy C. Bates tim.bates@ed.ac.uk + Bootstrapping: Kurtosis differences kurtosisDiff <- function(x, y, B = 1000){ kx <- replicate(B, kurtosi(sample(x, replace = TRUE))) ky <- replicate(B, kurtosi(sample(y, replace = TRUE))) return(kx - ky) } kurtDiff <- kurtosisDiff(x, y, B = 10000); mean(kurtDiff > 0) # p= 0.205 NS + Parametric Statistics 1 Assume data are drawn from samples with a certain distribution (usually normal) Compute the likelihood that groups are related/unrelated or same/different given that underlying model t-test, Pearson’s correlation, ANOVA… + Parametric Statistics 2 Assumptions of Parametric statistics 1. Observations are independent 2. Your data are normally distributed 3. Variances are equal across groups Can be modified to cope with unequal ∂2 + Non-parametric Statistics? Non-parametric statistics do not assume any underlying distribution They compute the likelihood that your groups are the same or different by comparing the ranks of subjects across the range of scores. + Non-parametric Statistics Assumptions of non-parametric statistics 1. Observations are independent + Non-parametric Statistics? Non-parametric statistics do not assume any underlying distribution Estimating or modeling this distribution reduces their power to detect effects… So don’t use them unless you have to + Why use a Non-parametric Statistic? Very small samples Leads to Type-1 (false alarm) errors Outliers more often lead to spurious Type-1 (false alarm) errors in parametric statistics. Nonparametric statistics reduce data to an ordinal rank, which reduces the impact or leverage of outliers. + Non-parametric Choices Data type? continuous discret e Question? association Spearman’s Rank χ2 Different central value BrownForsythe Number of groups? two-groups Mann-Whitney U Wilcoxon’s Rank Sums Difference in ∂2 more than 2 Kruskal-Wallis test + Non-parametric Choices Data type? continuous discret e Question? Like a Pearson’s R association Spearman’s Rank Like Student’s t No alternative χ2 Different central value Difference in ∂2 BrownForsythe Number of groups? two-groups Mann-Whitney U Wilcoxon’s Rank Sums more than 2 Kruskal-Wallis test Like F-test Like ANOVA + Binomial test binom.test(45, 100, .5, alternative="two.sided”) number of successes = 45, number of trials = 100, p-value = 0.3682 alternative hypothesis: true probability of success is not equal to 0.5 95 percent confidence interval: 0.350 0.5527 Sample estimates: probability of success 0.45 binom.test(51,235,(1/6),alternative="greater") + Spearman Rank test (ρ (rho)) Named after Charles Spearman, Non-parametric measure of correlation Assesses how well an arbitrary monotonic function describes the relationship between two variables, Does not require the relationship be linear Does not require interval measurement + Spearman Rank (ρ rho) d = difference in rank of a given pair n = number of pairs Alternative test = Kendall's Tau (Kendall's τ) + Mann-Whitney U AKA: “Wilcoxon rank-sum test Mann & Whitney, 1947; Wilcoxon, 1945 Non-parametric test for difference in the medians of two independent samples Assumptions: Samples are independent Observations can be ranked (ordinal or better) + Mann-Whitney U U tests the difference in the medians of two independent samples n1 = number of obs in sample 1 n2 = number of obs in sample 2 R = sum of ranks of the lower-ranked sample + Mann-Whitney U or t? Should you use it over the t-test? Yes if you have a very small sample (<20) (central limit assumptions not met) If your data are really ordinal Otherwise, probably not. It is less prone to type-I error (spurious significance) due to outliers. But does not in fact handle comparisons of samples whose variances differ very well (Use unequal variance t-test with rank data) + Wilcoxon signed-rank test (related samples) Same idea as Mann-U, generalized to matched samples Equivalent to non-independent sample t-test + Kruskall-Wallis Non-parametric one-way analysis of variance by ranks (named after William Kruskal and W. Allen Wallis) tests equality of medians across groups. It is an extension of the Mann-Whitney U test to 3 or more groups. Does not assume a normal population, Assumes population variances among groups are equal. + Aesop: Mann-Whitney U Example Suppose that Aesop is dissatisfied with his classic experiment in which one tortoise was found to beat one hare in a race. He decides to carry out a significance test to discover whether the results could be extended to tortoises and hares in general… + Aesop 2: Mann-Whitney U He collects a sample of 6 tortoises and 6 hares, and makes them all run his race. The order in which they reach the finishing post (their rank order) is as follows: tort = c(1, 7, 8, 9, 10,11) hare = c(2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 12) Original tortoise still goes at warp speed, original hare is still lazy, but the others run truer to stereotype. + Aesop 3: Mann-Whitney U wilcox.test(tort, hare) Wilcoxon = W = 25, p-value = 0.31 Tortoises and hares do not differ tort = c(1, 7, 8, 9, 10,11) (n2 = 6) hare = c(2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 12) (n1 = 6, R1 =32)