Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT The Painful Truth: A Comprehensive Review of Pain and Pain Management Cameron S. Wasson Psychology 3227B, University of Western Ontario 1 Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 2 The Painful Truth: A Comprehensive Review of Pain and Pain Management “The greatest evil is physical pain” (St. Augustine, n.d.). These words, spoken by Saint Augustine, a fifth century philosopher, converge on the idea that the most malevolent things plaguing humanity take root in the existence of physical pain. To elaborate further, “a recent market research report indicates that more than 1.5 billion people worldwide suffer from chronic pain” (Pleis, Ward, & Lucas, 2010). It is safe to say that pain is an unfortunate vicissitude all humanity must face; it is universal and inevitable. Because of the ubiquitous nature of pain, researchers and psychologists wish to describe, explain, predict, and control the very nature of it; however, there are a multitude of dilemmas with doing so. Firstly, pain has many ethical constraints for human testing. Most experiments of pain are restricted to animal models, which begs the question: How can researchers quantify data on those unable to convey what they are feeling? Secondly, pain seems to have variation from person-to-person. Thus, it is hard to operationalize and not easy to apply to multiple models using the same definition of pain. Aside from the two major dilemmas, researchers are making substantial progress in the study of pain and pain management. They are also using inductive observation, and applying a variety of treatments to converge on a universal pain theory. Because the theories of physiological mechanisms of pain are still relatively new, it is not always clear why these treatments work the way they do. This literature review hopes to provide a comprehensive understanding of pain by providing a brief history of theories, describing the physiological circuits involved, reviewing clinical treatments of pain, and examining neural manifestations of pain. Theories of Pain Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 3 Much like most psychological postulates, the first theories of pain root themselves in ancient Greek philosophy, where prominent figures such as Aristotle, believed pain was caused by evil spirits entering ones body and inflicting harm (Vertosick, 2001, p. 72). At the turn of the 16th century, French philosopher René Descartes proposed the idea that pain was due to an interaction between the body and the soul. He hypothesized that nerves were hollow tubes, which acted like ropes, and that when a noxious stimuli were encountered the nerves would ‘pull’ and cause the brain to produce the perception of pain (Vertosick, 2001, p. 109). While Descartes theory may be viewed as primitive, some of his ideas are carried onto many modern theories. Today there are three primary theories of pain which scientists use as the basis of their research: the specificity theory, the peripheral pattern theory, and the gate control theory of pain. Von Frey brought the specificity theory to light during the 19 th century, where he hypothesized that specialized peripheral receptors in the tissue sent impulses to the brains pain region in the brain (Helms & Barone, 2008). The peripheral pattern theory suggests that pain is perceived in a pattern-like fashion, where different firing patterns of nerves essentially summate and create the sensation of pain. It is important to note that in the pattern theory supporters do not believe in specific pain receptors, but rather a integration of various types of touch receptors that cause one to perceive pain. The gate control theory suggests more detailed physiological interactions than the latter two theories. Melzak and Wall (1965) suggest that painful stimuli activate two different nerve fibers; myelinated Aδ-fibers, which are the first to respond and fire to more discriminative pain sensations; and unmyelinated C-fibers, which respond slowly Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 4 and transmit a more diffuse pain sensation (Dubuc, n.d.). Both Aδ-fibers and C-fibers feed into transmission cells (T-cells), which transmit the painful stimulus to brain; however, modulating this event of pain transmission occurs via the substantia gelatinosa (SG), a group of cells in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord. When experiencing a painful interaction, Aδ-fibers inhibit the SG and produce a sharp fast pain. After this, the C-fibers slowly fire and produce the dull aching pain. The process known as the gate control theory because the SG acts as a gateway, which the noxious stimulus must override to cause the perception of pain. One important fiber not mentioned is the Aβ- fiber, an A-fiber subtype that transmits non-noxious mechanical stimuli. According to the gate control theory of pain, “if touch receptors (Aβ fibers) are stimulated, they dominate and close the gate” (Helms & Barone, 2008). This theory suggests that massage-type therapies can be used in the treatment of chronic pain in patients (Adams, White, & Beckett, 2010). Afferent Pathway of Pain The modern theory of pain is an integrated model of the specificity theory and gate control theory. There are three major components to the experience of pain: transduction, transmission, and perception. Transduction begins with the introduction of a noxious stimuli and the conversion of the stimuli into an impulse by a specific receptor. These receptors are coined nociceptors (Sherrington, 1903). When one becomes injured, the synthesis of arachidonic acid takes place at the site of injury, where it is converted to prostaglandins by the cyclooxygenase enzyme (COX). This then acts on C-fibers, which lowers the pain threshold. This is why just by lightly touching an injured area can cause severe acute pain. From the site of injury, a Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 5 nociceptor is activated and fires and transmits an action potential along Aδ-fibers and Cfibers, where they synapse onto the substantia gelatinosa in the dorsal horn of the spinal column (Dubuc, n.d.). The fibers then cross over at the anterior white commissure and ascend via the anterior spinothalamic tract. The anterior spinothalamic tract then splits into two distinct tracts; the neospinothalamic tract, which projects fast pain (Aδ-fibers) to the ventrobasal complex of the thalamus; and the paleospinalthalamic tract, which joins the fast tract then terminates in the hypothalamus and limbic structures (Mohr-Catalano, 1997). The neospinothalamic tract relays information to the somatosensory cortex; where pain is perceived, while the paleospinalthalamic tract projects to the periaqueductal gray (PAG), the raphe nucleus (RN), the hypothalamus, and other limbic structures; this where emotional pain is integrated (Kozlov et al., 2012). This afferent model of pain is sometimes referred to as bottom-up processing since there is no central input until the latter stages of the pathway. It is interesting to note that this model, although more detailed, parallels with Descartes theory he proposed back in the 16th century. Simplified, this pathway is essentially a string that connects to the brain and when a noxious stimulus is introduced, it projects to the control center. This theory is important because it is how most pharmaceutical companies look to target pain. Therapeutic Models for Bottom-Up Processing Drug companies and alternative medicines such as acupuncture attempt to target or cut off the afferent pain fibers by intervening at different levels of the pathway. Aspirin and other non-steroidal anti-inflammatory (NSAIDS) drugs work by inhibiting the COX enzyme (Derry, Moore, & Rabbie, 2012). By shutting off the enzyme arachidonic acid Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 6 never gets converted into prostaglandins therefore, the threshold of pain fibers is not reduced and allodynia, pain to non-noxious stimuli, does not occur. Alternative medicines like acupuncture and transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation are believed to cut off the ascending pathways by activating Aβ-fibers, which turn on the SG inhibiting T-cells (Chang, 2013). This can be seen through a common reflex. As one is injured the individual tends to pull away or either shake or rub the affected area. It is thought that this again stimulates large Aβ-fibers and inhibits the transmission of pain. Though this model seems to be rather comprehensive there are some flaws. For example, the cutting of small fibers (C-fibers) seems to alleviate pain in some instances and does not in others (Pritchard & Alloway, 1999, p. 107). As well, there seems to be another major loophole in the modern pain theory, and that is the existence of phantom limb pain. The amputee experiencing phantom limb pain does not have nociceptors in the afflicted area, so how could there possibly be pain coming from that region? How can physicians cut or block a pathway that is no longer present? This alludes to the concept that pain is nothing more than a cognitive process and perhaps is only a product of the brain. This idea leads to the existence of another processing pathway. Efferent Pathway of Pain This idea that pain is nothing more than a cognitive illusion begins with the efferent route of pain. Of the two, the efferent pathway is the most poorly understood and an area where much research is being done. The purpose of this pathway is to inhibit the afferent pain information by responding to incoming nociceptor information from the peripheral nervous system. It was found that noxious stimulation excites neurons in the nucleus reticularis gigantocellularis (RGC), which in turn stimulates the Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 7 PAG and the RN (Braz, Enquist, & Basbaum, 2010). Next, the PAG sends its fibers to the RN where: Serotonin is released from the raphe nuclei and descends to the dorsal horn of the spinal cord where it forms excitatory connections with the "inhibitory interneurons” the substantia gelatinosa. When activated, these interneurons release endogenous opioid neurotransmitters, which bind to mu-opioid receptors on the axons of incoming C and A-delta fibers carrying pain signals from nociceptors activated in the periphery. This inhibits [action potential] from these incoming first-order neurons and, in turn, inhibits [the pain signal]. (Basbaum & Fields, 1978) This pathway is sometimes referred to as the top-down model of pain because of the efferent output to the spinal cord. This top-down model also seems to have a cognitive interaction at some conscious level, and perhaps can be controlled. This pathway is also body’s main response to noxious stimuli, as it produces pain-reducing molecules termed endorphins. This model of pain is held in high regards, especially in the clinical practice. Analgesics such as morphine monopolize on this system as exogenous endorphins to completely alleviate or remove the sensation of pain Therapeutic Models for Top-Down Processing The therapeutic drugs that act on this model interact with the opioid receptors found on the axons of the incoming Aδ-fibers and C-fibers. Drugs such as morphine and heroin reduce pain by acting on these systems. A study by Bood, Kjellgren, and Norlander (2009) showed that floating restricted isolated stimulation to be an effective form of pain treatment because of the release of endorphins this intervention seems to Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 8 promote. Other pain treatments such as deep brain stimulation have been shown to be an effective way to reduce pain in chronic cancer patients (Young & Brechner, 1986). One of the best methodologies for studying the effectiveness for treatment is to introduce the drug Naloxone, an opioid antagonist, which inhibits the effectiveness of endorphins. The pain reveling effects of eletroacupuncture in rats have been shown to reverse when Naloxone is subcutaneously introduced (Pomeranz & Chiu, 1976). This suggests that acupuncture indeed exhibits its affects by acting on the top-down model of pain, which is contrary to what was previously stated. Some of the most interesting treatments administered are placebos. Because there is an emotional and cognitive component to pain, placebos seem to play a role in pain modulation. Grevert, Albert, and Goldstein (1983) showed that naloxone diminished the analgesic effectiveness of the placebo. This suggests that the placebo effect actually causes the release of opioids and endorphins. Neuropsychological Representations of Pain Research implies there is no specific part of the cortex responsible for the perception of pain; it is widely distributed throughout the brain (Talbot et al., 1991). Although pain perception seems to lack a locus, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) seems to play a role in pain interpretation. Davis et al. (1997), using fMRI, showed there is a significant correlation between pain intensity and the amount of activation. Interestingly enough, the anterior cingulate cortex is a major player of the limbic system; therefore, perhaps all pain really only dependent on the emotional context of it. Sawamoto et al. (2000) conducted an experiment where they evaluated the expectation of pain in relation to the activation of the ACC. The participants were told the stimulus Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 9 would be warm; they showed little activity; were told it was going to hurt; they showed substantial activity; then presented the warm stimulus and were simultaneously told it would hurt; they again showed substantial activation. Because the participants were told the stimulus was going to hurt they reacted in a way to the warm stimulus differently than they initially did when they were told it was warm. This goes to show that pain is context specific and that it can be experienced differently with individual mindsets. As well, pain also seems to be attenuated by external cues. A study by Miron, Duncan, and Bushnell (1999) showed attention to pain to be an important component of pain perception. Researchers introduced a noxious-thermal stimulus to participants and monitored cortical activation, which showed a substantial response. The participants where then given headphones and asked to attend to the music; this almost seemed to relocate activation to the auditory cortex and minimized the activation in “pain areas.” These aforementioned studies suggest that pain is nothing more than an illusion created by the brain and that it may be controlled on some conscious level. Future Directions More conclusive studies need to be done to truly understand pain. Researchers use inductive reasoning for the study of pain, and this is why there are so many findings that are unequivocal and varied. The better scientists can understand both the top-down and bottom up models of pain the more efficient and effective the treatments will become at targeting and alleviating chronic pain. But for now pain-specific therapies must wait, and that is the painful truth. Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 10 References Adams, R., White, B., & Beckett, C. (2010). The effects of massage therapy on pain management in the acute care setting. International Journal of Therapeutic Massage & Bodywork, 3(1), 4-11. Basbaum, A., & Fields, H. (1978). Endogenous pain control mechanisms: review and hypothesis. Ann Neurol, 4(5), 451-62. Bood, S., Kjellgren, A., & Norlander, T. (2009). Treating stress-related pain with the flotation restricted environmental stimulation technique: Are there differences between women and men?. Pain Res Manag, 14(4), 293-298. Braz, J., Enquist, L., & Basbaum, A. (2010). Inputs to serotonergic neurons revealed by conditional viral transneuronal tracing . J Comp Neurol, 514(2), 145-160. Chang, S. (2013). The meridian system and mechanism of acupuncture-A comparative review. Part 2: Mechanism of acupuncture analgesia. Taiwan J Obstet Gynecol, 52(1), 14-24. Davis, K., Taylor, S., Crawley, A., Wood, M., & Mikulis, D. (1997). Functional MRI of pain- and attention- related activations in the human cingulate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 3370-3380. Derry, S., Moore, R., & Rabbie, R. (2012). Topical NSAIDs for chronic musculoskeletal pain in adults. Cochrane Datebase Syst Rev, 9, 255-62. Dubuc, B. (n.d.). Ascending brain pathways. The brain from top to bottom. Retrieved April 8, 2013, from http://thebrain.mcgill.ca/flash/d/d_03/d_03_cl/d_03_cl_dou/d_03_cl_dou.html Grevert, P., Albert, L., & Goldstein, A. (1983). Partial antagonism of placebo analgesia Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 11 by naloxone. Pain, 16(2), 129-43. Helms, J., & Barone, C. (2008). Physiology and treatment of pain. Crit Care Nurse, 28(6), 38-49. Kozlov, A., Abramova, A., Nikenina, E., & Mezentseva, L. (2012). Nociceptive thresholds of the response to lipopolysaccharide injection into the limbic structures of the brain in rats. Bull Exp Biol Med, 153(5), 730-3. Melzack, R., & Wall, P. (1965). Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science, 150(3699), 971-9. Miron D., Duncan G.H., Bushnell, M.C. (1989). Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain. Pain, 39:345–352. Mohr-Catalano, E., Tupper, S., & Carron, H. (1997). The chronic pain control workbook. New York : Fine Communications. Pleis J.R., Ward B.W., Lucas J.W. (2010). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: National Health Interview Survey. Vital Health Stat, 10(249). Pomeranz, B., & Chiu, D. (1976). Naloxone blockade of acupuncture analgesia: Endorphin implicated. Life Sciences, 19(11), 1757-1762. Pritchard, T., & Alloway, K. (1999). Medical Neuroscience. Malden, MA: Fence Creek Publishing. Sawamoto, N., Honda, M., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Kanda, M., Fukuyama, H., et al. (2000). Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.. J Neurosci, 20(19), 7438-45. Running head: PAIN MANAGEMENT 12 Sherrington, C.S. (1903). Qualitative difference of spinal reflex corresponding with qualitative difference of cutaneous stimulus. J Physiol, 30:39–46. Talbot, J., Marrett, S., Evans, A., Meyer, E., Bushnell, M., & Duncan, G. (1991). Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex. Science, 251(4999), 1355-8. Vertosick, F. T. (2001). Why we hurt. Orlando : Harcourt Inc.. Young, R., & Brechner, T. (1986). Electrical stimulation of the brain for relief of intractable pain due to cancer. Cancer, 57(6), 1266-72.