The Syntactic Development of Child Language Acquisition 1. INTRODUCTION The study of a child’s acquisition of the English language over his/her early years involves the research of a methodology to understand and predict the structural development of a child’s language development. People who are already interested in children, be it natural parents or pre-school teachers, are often said to take an interest in this subject, due to their already acquired friendliness and familiarity with children in their early years of life. It is to be understood that syntactic development includes the structural analysis of child language only, and not foreign subjects like the clarity of speech or pronunciation. While the latter studies often accompany the former, the two aren’t overlapping in any manner. The structural analysis consists of, among other things, the phonetic assessment of the different vocal sounds a child can produce, and the phonological assessment of these sounds being combined into more complex speech patterns. In order to understand syntactic development, the understanding of three basic terms is necessary: Morphology: The branch of grammar that dedicates itself to the study of the structure and form of words, through the use of morpheme construction. Syntax: The study of the various rules implied in the combination of several words to form full sentences. Morpheme: A unit of meaning in morphology, not necessarily related to syllable or word counts. E.g. Happy (One Morpheme), Happier (Two Morphemes), Unhappily (Three Morphemes). In this paper, we are going to study the syntactic development of child language acquisition amongst children aged 18 months to five years. We are going to learn how these children familiarize themselves with the various words and sentences in the English language, while structurally analyzing the same. Table 1: Counting Morphemes The girl s mother slow ly fill ed the bucket with water 1 2 3 4 6 7 8 9 10 12 5 11 2. LITERATURE REVIEW First language acquisition is a subject that has been studied upon for years in all parts of the world. A mother is excited to hear her child speak for the first time, she observes patiently and proudly as her child slowly develops the ability to utter meaningless noises, then simple words and finally, full blown phrases and even sentences. Watching a child progress in his or her acquisition of the English Language is both thrilling and exciting. The role played by a child’s mother, and in later years, the pre-school teacher, is absolutely crucial to this subject. Depending on a child’s interaction with their parents/teachers, they learn new words, their meanings and how to use them successfully in normal conversation. It is a rather interesting spectacle to observe a child develop from meaningless babbling to forming their very first meaningful chunks of words, morphemes and phrases to communicate with those that surround (Foster-Cohen 2009). The relationship between the child and its mother is very crucial in the acquisition of first language. Lieven says that the conversations held between mother and child happen differently for every child-mother pair and has significant effects on how the acquisition varies (1978). An entirely different perspective, however, states that a child’s language acquisition betters as their organs develop, as first acquisition is a biological procedure more than a social one (Yule 1996). Before the 1950’s, all study on child acquisition was confined to a few linguistic scholars studying their own children. “At one time, it was believed that children learned language by simply imitating the speech of those around them” (O’Grady, Dobrovolsky and Aronoff). Since then, the learning of child language acquisition changed rapidly, especially after Chomsky presented his theory of there being an innate biological system of learning language in every child. Linguists now began to see their children as “creators of interesting and systematic language, rather than as imperfect imitators of adult speech”. With the introduction of tape-recording, more linguists began to obtain and study records of children speaking language, and the study of child acquisition received a huge boost. The learnings of the acquisition of language in a child has improved greatly in the last 50 years. Dr. Barbara Lust from the Cornell University’s Cornell’s Language Acquisition Lab (CLAL) recently conducted a study in 2006; her findings were captured in her book Child Language: Acquisition and Growth. Some of the major findings in her study have been summarized below: Soon after birth, even before the acquisition of language in any form, children begin to process a speech stream in order to study phonology and syntax around them. Soon after, they begin to demonstrate some form of semantics or understanding of the meanings of the language spoken around them. As children learn their first words and begin to construct simple sentences, they will demonstrate an agility in understanding the complex knowledge of semantics. They will avoid random errors and even be able to understand possible multiple meanings within the same words and sentences in language. By the time they reach three, children will have learned most of the basics of language acquisition and be able to understand language quite fluently. Children sometimes show a tendency to make some common errors in language, for example, calling all fruits and vegetables “apples”, instead of calling them by their specific names. However, these errors only proceed to demonstrate their creativity of mind and their ability to be abstract and categorical in the learning of their first language. Through these studies, it is clearly observable the child acquisition is a sensitive and delicate procedure. It is also rather difficult to study, as children need to be observed in their natural state, rather than being interrogated out front on what they know and what they don’t. Experiments and studies on child acquisition are generally conducted on a longitudinal basis and a cross-sectional basis. Longitudinally, each child is observed over a period of several years to see how they acquire language. Crosssectionally, a group of children are observed at a certain point of time to see how they interact through the English language. This way, each child is studied singularly and in groups, both in short and longterm bases. 3. THE ACQUISTION OF LANGUAGE IN A CHILD Child Language Acquisition is a rather interesting subject, applicable not only in linguistics but also in early childhood education programs. Teachers have made good use of child acquisition to help children better learn the English Language. As the understanding of child acquisition has improved over the years, more and more attention has been allotted to the subject. There are a large number of theoretical perspectives as to how a child acquires their first language. Broadly, these can be categorized into behavioristic, innatist and interactional perspectives. Behavioristic Perspective: This perspective states that language is learned through imitation, practice and “positive reinforcement” from the environment. According to this theory, children learn language as they attempt to imitate the adults in vocal mechanics and receive encouragement from their environment for the same. Thus begins the correlation of words with meanings and the understanding of language. This theory, however, has a few loopholes. First, children only imitate what they have previously heard, “input”. Further, they often utter words and phrases on their own, something which they have not heard any adults around them use. Innatist Perspective: According to Chomsky, language is an innate ability that grows as a biological organ, and requires only a little bit of environmental interaction to develop. This theory basically attempts to fill the gap in the behavioristic perspective, by announcing that a child learns language as an innate ability rather than an environmental result, independent of the input they receive. Even innatists, however, must admit that some form of interaction with the outer world is required for the children to acquire language in any form or shape. Interactional/Developmental Perspective: Piaget and Vygotsky state that children acquire language through interaction with people and objects, as a cognitive development procedure. This is the interactional perspective. The importance of interaction in language acquisition has been highlighted in study after study. In one such case, a boy with deaf parents was left with nothing but a television as a source to learn English. Obviously, his language skills were poor, and only after he began interacting with another person in the real world did he acquire English as well as he should have (Sachs, Bard & Johnson 1981). Child acquisition begins at the very first point a child learns to create their first sound, crying. It starts at birth and continues all the way through his growing years till he has been accustomed with the basics of their first language. There are several stages in this, starting with crying and emitting other physical noises like breathing, burping, etc., followed by a lot of babbling, then comes the first words, the ability to associate two words at a time, being able to pair multiple words meaningful into a semi-sentence, and, finally, full blown understanding of the basics of language. Despite the several perspectives on first language acquisition, all experts can agree that the acquisition of first language occurs through four primary stages: The Pre-Language State: This phase is marked by a lot of cooing and babbling and signifies the beginning of the acquisition of language in a child. Around three months of age, a child begins cooing, uttering velar consonants and high vowels. This is followed by meaningless babbling from around six months of age. The Holophrastic Stage: In between the ages of twelve and eighteen months begins the oneword of holophrastic stage. During this phase, a child learns to utter simple one or two-syllable words such as “milk” and “eat”. The Two-Word Stage: This begins around eighteen and twenty months once the child’s vocabulary has superseded 50 words. The child learns to pair words together into meaningful phrases, such as “daddy eat”, “mommy sing” and so forth. The Telegraphic Speech Stage: This stage occurs before a child turns three, where the child learns to link words together into neo-sentences in the right order. This stage proceeds into the final stage where a child fully acquires language and learns to speak in an orderly manner. As the child acquires language for the first time, different stages of acquisition focus on various different areas of language. As a scholar, one may choose to focus on a particular language area for study and leave out the rest: Semantics: Semantics refers to a child’s usage of the right words at the right time for them to make sense and be meaningful beyond just an utterance. When focusing on semantics, scholar determines which words can be associated or linked together and which ones can’t. For example, “bread” can be linked with “taste” and “knife can be linked with “sharp”. Phonology: Phonology refers to the sound patterns that help diversify a language. Each human is provided with a vocal tract based on their gender and body structure, hence there can be different ways to pronounce the same word. Phonology helps us study and understand the diversity in sound patterns in terms of articulation in a child’s language acquisition procedure. Syntax: Syntax refers to the organization and structural disposition of the various components of a sentence. The literal meaning of the word “syntax” is “arrangement”. The study of syntax ensues when a child learns to speak its first words and continues up till when they can construct entire sentences through the weaving of different words and phrases. Table 2: Child Language Acquisition Operations Instances Intents Nomination That car That is a car. Recurrence More juice Give me more juice. Negation No wee wee I did not do a wee wee. Negation No more I don't want any more. Negation Birdie go The bird has gone. Once infants are born, their body parts are highly fragile, delicate and underdeveloped. While human children fully develop their hearing only halfway through pregnancy, the eyesight and control over facial muscles come with maturation. Once born, the only form of sound children know to produce is crying. Through crying, they seek attention, make known their needs, and express discomfort and so forth. Other sounds they produce during this stage are largely vegetative, such as breathing, gasping, yawning and so forth. At around three months of age, children gain significant control over their vocal cords and learn to produce their first few sounds. Most of their utterances during this period is through cooing and laughing. They now have enough control over their facial muscles and can smile as well as laugh. They can also emit high pitched noises and minor screams as a show of enthusiasm as well as discomfort. Next, a child learns to speak their first words. This happens at about twelve months of age. The child can now utter simple words like “mamma” and “papa”. They learn to single out words of importance and use just those in order to communicate their feelings. They are able to do this despite the fact that adults around them, however, speak in full sentences. It is said that the children learn to single out words depending on the effort an adult around them puts on a single word. For example, when a child wants to say “I want food”, all he says is “food”, thereby communicating his need without the use of a complex linguistic mechanism. Learning new words is a rather slow procedure, where a child learns a new word only about once a week. But after a child reaches eighteen months, the vocabulary starts multiplying rapidly. The child acquires more and more words within a shorter span of time, speeding up the procedure of language acquisition. The speed now increases to ten-twelve words a day (O'Grady, Dobrovolsky and Aronoff 371). This is called a “vocabulary spurt”. During this period, children understand more complex sounds then they are able to produce. While they may limit themselves to one-word utterances only, they already understand full sentences. Their comprehension of words is thus much greater than their utterance of the same. A few months after they turn eighteen, children first learn to link two words into a neo-phrase, creating utterances such as “mommy cookie” or “daddy go”. While their pronunciation is not as improved as others, and they can often make obvious mistakes, such as saying “doggie” while pointing at a horse and so forth, this is one of the most crucial phases of first language acquisition, as a child learns what a language is for the first time in its life. By the time children reach the age of two, their way of speaking resembles that of a telegraph, i.e., “send money car broke”, etc. Children can now associate multiple words together, much more than just two, and can more or less grasp the concept of sentences. Although they may not be perfect in their use of articles, prepositions, etc., they do learn to communicate in simple language quite well. This is followed by the final stage of first language acquisition. When a child is about 3 years old, it learns to speak full sentences in a way similar to an adult. It learns to speak simple sentences, such as “Give me a cookie“ or “Daddy went home” , with ease. Thus end the procedure of child language acquisition, as the child now masters their mother tongue similar to the way any adult would, learning more complex words, sentences, prepositions, pronouns, articles and so forth. 4. CONCLUSION The aim of this study was to study the procedure of first language acquisition in children aged 0-3 years. In that course we learned of the various perspectives to first language acquisition, behaviorist, innatist and interactional. We learned how each of these perspectives differ, and how each of them is correct in its own way. Then we went through the four stages of first language acquisition in children, the prelanguage state, the holophrastic state, the two-word stage and the telegraphic speech stage. We learned how each of these stages help in a child’s acquisition of its first language, and we learned how the procedure of language acquisition occurs in a child stage after stage. We then learned the different areas of language explored in the study of first language acquisition, namely semantics, phonology and syntax. We learned how each stage of child language acquisition focuses on each of these areas of language, and how a linguist can selectively study only one of these areas of language acquisition while effectively omitting the rest. We then proceeded to study the actual procedure of first language acquisition as it occurs in a child, from its first cry to the age of three years. We studied how a child develops from the stage of crying to cooing, to babbling, to first words, to two-word utterances to telegraphic speech and finally to full-blown sentence construction. We also learned how each off these phases are vital to the acquisition of first language, and how the procedure of child language acquisition takes place differently in different children. Indeed, the study of first language acquisition is a rather delicate procedure. Not only is it difficult to study as a child is observed through his or her growing ages and studied based on how he or she masters language, it is also a procedure that differs largely from child to child. No wonder, a large number of theories and perspectives exist pertaining to first language acquisition, each trying its best to cover a certain angle in child acquisition. Child language acquisition is not only delicate, unique and ever so adventurous, it is also very much practically applicable and useful when it comes to early childhood education. The understanding of child language acquisition enables linguists to develop a course of study in language suited to the needs of each child, helping it get better in English language through its growing phases. The understanding of first language acquisition helps cater to a child’s needs in learning a new language, and helping it get better at it without effort and hard work. The understanding of this subject is going to serve very well in educating children in their earlier ages, no doubt it has been so widely researched. 5. REFERENCES BOWEN, CAROLINE. 2011. Brown's Stages of Syntactic and Morphological Development. Online: http://www.speech-languagetherapy.com/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=33:brown&catid=2:uncategorised&Item id=117. THURESSON, JANI. 2011. The syntactic development in the earlier stages of children’s first language acquisition. Online: http://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:480083/fulltext01. GALASSO, JOSEPH. 2015. Remarks on a Minimalist Approach to Early Child Syntax. Online: https://www.academia.edu/15157172/Remarks_on_a_Minimalist_Approach_to_Early_Child_Syntax. WHELPTON, MATTHEW. 2014. First Language Acquisition: The rate and style of vocabulary growth in the first years. Online: file:///C:/Users/princepetropia/Downloads/Documents/BA_Essay.pdf.