Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 1 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Ali (1993) Anakwe et al. (1999) Arunachalam et al. (1998) Bennett (1999) Countries/ Sample 117 managers in Saudi Arabia Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models noneb Survey Individualism (Ali, 1987)a; decision making styles 424 U.S. undergraduate and graduate students A total of over 600 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S. Survey Collectivism (Wagner, 1995); receptivity to distance learning technology none Experiment Individualism; Collectivism (Triandis, 1995)c; negotiation outcome quality none 64 U.S. and 47 People’s Republic of China (PRC) employees Survey All five dimensions of Hofstede’s cultural values (Dorfman & Howell, 1988); work-related attitudes and beliefs Social information processing; dissonance theory Key Findings Individualism was positively related to consultative, participative, and autocratic decision making styles and attitudes toward risk. Collectivism was negatively related to distance learning technology. Hong Kong subjects were significantly more collectivistic and less individualistic than U.S.; H.K. negotiators obtained higher joint outcomes than U.S. negotiators; negotiators with a high, rather than low, best alternative to a negotiated agreement (BATNA) obtained larger individual outcomes in both countries; mediation had a stronger effect on outcomes in the U.S. U.S. was significantly lower on collectivism and higher on masculinity than the PRC; in both countries, collectivism was positively related to favorable attitudes towards group activities and cooperation; in the U.S., masculinity was negatively related to positive attitudes toward human development. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 2 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Bochner & Hesketh (1994)d Brett & Okumura (1998) Countries/ Sample 263 Australian bank employees representing 28 nationalities Method Survey Managers from Japan and the U.S. making up 30 intercultural dyads and 65 intracultural dyads Experiment Cable & Judge 171 U.S. (1994) undergraduates Experiment Variables and Measures Individualism-Collectivism; Power Distance (both assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980 and study specific items used to validate the use of country scores); contact with coworkers; knowledge of staff; extent of working alone; openness and contact with superiors; task-orientation; beliefs in Theory X Individualism (Schwartz, 1994); Hierarchy (i.e. power distance) (Schwartz, 1994); negotiation tactics Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1987; Wagner & Moch, 1986)e; preferences for individual-based pay Theories/ Models Social categorization theory Social cognition Expectancy theory; agency theory Key Findings Collectivists had more informal contact with coworkers, knew staff better, and were more likely to work on a team than alone than were individualists; high power distance respondents were less open with superiors, had more contact with them, described their supervision as being closer and direct, were more task-oriented, and had greater beliefs in Theory X management than those low in power distance. U.S. managers were significantly more individualistic but less hierarchical than Japanese managers; individualists endorsed self-interest in negotiations more than collectivists; those with hierarchical values endorsed distributive tactics more; in inter- (rather than intra-) cultural negotiations, significant misunderstandings occurred about negotiator priorities; and individualism-collectivism failed to fully mediate country effects. Collectivism was negatively related to preferences for individual-based pay. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 3 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Carpenter & Radhakrishnan (2000) Casimir & Keats (1996) Chan & Drasgow (2001) Chen et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample 34 EuropeanAmerican undergraduates and 42 MexicanAmerican undergraduates Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Social categorization theory Key Findings Survey Individualism-Collectivism (independent-interdependent self-construal; Singelis et al., 1995); friendship descriptions European-Americans scored significantly higher on individualism than did Mexican-Americans, but their scores did not significantly differ on collectivism; collectivists represent more social connections (i.e., social identities, social traits, and other persons) in their concepts of people (i.e., self, other individuals, and in-groups) than do individualists. Anglo- and Chinese-Australians did not significantly differ on individualism-collectivism; respondents in both cultures preferred leaders who expressed high concern for both performance and group relations. A total of 120 Anglo- and ChineseAustralian middle managers 1,594 military recruits and 274 junior college students in Singapore; 239 U.S. undergraduates Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Bontempo, 1993)f; preferences for leadership style PM theory of leadership Survey Social cognition; theory of reasoned action; selfefficacy theory Collectivism (both types) was positively related, and horizontal individualism was negatively related, to the noncalculative and social-normative aspects of motivation to lead; vertical individualism was positively related to the affective identity and social-normative aspects of motivation to lead and was negatively related to the noncalculative aspect. 300 managers in the PRC In-basket exercise Horizontal Collectivism; Vertical Collectivism; Horizontal Individualism; Vertical Individualism (all assessed using Singelis et al., 1995); general cognitive ability; the Big Five traits; leadership self-efficacy; motivation to lead Horizontal Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1986); Vertical Collectivism (Wagner and Moch, 1986; Hui, 1988)g; reward allocation reform Reward allocation preferences Vertical collectivism was positively related to reward allocation reform; horizontal collectivism was negatively related to reward allocation reform and differential allocation preferences (but only under the condition of low vertical collectivism). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 4 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Chen et al. (1998a) Chiu & Kosinki (1999) Countries/ Sample A total of 241 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S. A total of 626 registered nurses in Australia, Hong Kong, Singapore, and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Experiment Collectivism (Perloe, 1967, Hui, 1988)h; equity versus equality norms; task interdependence; goals; achievement motivation Survey Individualism-Collectivism; Confucian dynamism (Chinese Culture Connection, 1987); affective disposition (i.e., positive and negative affect); job satisfaction; work strain Theories/ Models Distributive justice; allocation preferences; need for achievement none Key Findings In both countries, reward allocation was more differential when task interdependence was low, rather than high, and when the goal was productivity or fairness rather than solidarity; collectivism was negatively related to reward differential in Hong Kong but not in the U.S.; in both countries, achievement motivation was negatively related to differential allocation when task interdependence was high, however, when low, achievement motivation was positively related to differential allocation in the U.S. (but stays negative for Hong Kong). Singaporean and Hong Kong nurses were significantly more collectivistic and less individualistic than Australian and U.S. nurses; individualists scored higher on positive affect (no difference on negative affect) and job satisfaction and lower on work strain than collectivists; individualist subjects high in positive affect scored higher on job satisfaction and lower on work strain than collectivist subjects; individualist subjects high in negative affect scored lower on job satisfaction and higher on work strain than collectivists. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 5 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Clugston et al. (2000) Countries/ Sample 156 employees in a U.S. public agency Method Survey Cocroft & Ting-Toomey (1994) 200 undergraduates in both Japan and the U.S. Experiment Earley (1986) 36 managerial trainees in both England and the U.S. Experiment Variables and Measures Individualism-Collectivism; Power Distance; Uncertainty Avoidance; MasculinityFemininity (all assessed with Dorfman & Howell, 1988); affective, normative, and continuance commitment to organization, supervisor and work group (i.e., three foci) Individualism; Collectivism (Befu, 1990, Triandis, 1986)j; Modified version of Hamaguchi’s (cited and translated into English by Befu, 1990) scale and three items from Triandis et al.’s (1986) scale; presentation strategies in conflict Collectivism; Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, MasculinityFemininity (all assessed with Hofstede, 1980); positive and negative feedback; individual performance; importance of feedback; trust in supervisor Theories/ Models Lewin’s (1943)i field theory; Theory X; social identity theory Key Findings Collectivism was related to affective commitment to supervisors and work groups, continuance commitment to work groups, and normative commitment to all foci; power distance was related to affective commitment to organizations and both continuance and normative commitment to all foci; uncertainty avoidance was related to affective commitment to organizations and continuance commitment to all foci. Theory of facework; faceworknegotiation Japanese scored significantly higher on both individualism and collectivism; U.S. subjects used more anti-social, self-attribution, hint, and selfpresentation strategies than Japanese subjects who used more indirect face strategies. Theory of reasoned action; Ilgen et al.’s (1979)k model of feedback English subjects scored significantly higher on collectivism and power distance than did U.S. subjects; U.S. subjects’ performance increased as a result of both positive and negative feedback, whereas English subjects’ performance improved only as a result of positive feedback; importance of feedback and trust in supervisor partially mediated the main effects of positive and negative feedback and culture on performance. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 6 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Earley et al. (1999) Countries/ Sample 228 managerial trainees in the Czech Republic, the PRC, and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Managerial simulation Individualism-Collectivism (Earley, 1994); individual and group level feedback; self-efficacy; individualbased performance beliefs; and satisfaction with performance Collectivism (Wagner and Moch, 1986); self-efficacy for teamwork; need for social approval; positive past experience working in teams Collectivism (Wagner & Moch, 1986); individual perceptions of organizational readiness to change Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); skill utilization; opportunities to learn new skills; internship satisfaction; organizational commitment; in-role performance Eby & Dobbins (1997) 148 U.S. undergraduates Survey Eby et al. (2000) 117 employees and managers in a U.S. sales organization 125 U.S. MBAs participating in overseas internships Survey Feldman & Bolino (2000) Archival and Survey Theories/ Models Information processing view of self Expectancy theory; perceived control; need for affiliation Chaos theory; complexity theory Relative deprivation theory; Job Characteristics Model Key Findings U.S. subjects were significantly more individualistic than Czech or PRC subjects (who did not differ); collectivism was positively related to performance beliefs and satisfaction; collectivists had the highest efficacy in the high group-high individual performance information condition; individual feedback was important for both groups but group feedback was important only for collectivists. Collectivism was positively related to selfefficacy for teamwork, need for social approval, and positive past experiences working in teams. Collectivism was positively related to individual perceptions of an organization’s readiness to change to team-based selling. Cultural distance was not significantly related to skill utilization, opportunities to learn or further develop skills, internship satisfaction, organizational commitment, or in-role performance. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 7 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Gabrielidis et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample A total of 200 undergraduates in Mexico and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Survey Individualism; Collectivism (Kitayama, Markus, Tummala, Kurokawa, & Kato, 1991)l; MasculinityFemininity (Spence & Helmreich, 1978)m; styles of conflict resolution Theories/ Models Dual- concern model of conflict Geletkanycz (1997)d 1,500 top managers in 20 countries Archival and Survey All five Hofstede values (assessed using country scores, Hofstede, 1980); commitment to the status quo regarding organizational leadership and strategy Upper echelons perspective Gibson & ZellmerBruhn (2001) 107 members of 52 teams in French, Puerto Rican, Filipino and U.S. subsidiary locations of five multinational firms A total of 330 MBA students in Mexico and U.S. Interview and Archival Individualism and Power Distance (captured in interviews based on search terms derived from Hofstede, 1980a, and others) Linguistic theory, social cognitive theory, teamwork theory Experiment Collectivism (Wagner, 1995); evaluation generosity; ingroup-outgroup distinction; task and maintenance inputs Social identity theory; allocation rules Gomez et al. (2000) Key Findings Mexicans were significantly more collectivistic, individualistic, and masculine than U.S. subjects; Mexican scores on accommodation and collaboration were higher than U.S.; within Mexico, collectivism was positively correlated with collaboration and masculinity with accommodation and avoidance; in the U.S., collectivism was positively, and individualism negatively, correlated with avoidance. Managers were more likely to believe that the future CEO of their company should have the same leadership expertise as the current CEO and that the company’s future corporate strategy should be the same as the existing corporate strategy when they were high on individualism, low on uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and long-term orientation. Power distance was negatively related to the use of teamwork metaphors containing clear role content (e.g., family or military metaphors); individualism was negatively related to respondents’ use of teamwork metaphors broad in scope (e.g., community metaphor); teamwork metaphors carry with them expectations for the manner in which teams will be managed and the way in which team processes will unfold. Mexicans were significantly more collectivistic than U.S. Americans; collectivists gave more generous evaluations to ingroup, rather than outgroup, members; individualists valued task inputs in determining evaluations more than collectivists; and individualism-collectivism failed to fully mediate country effects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 8 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Grimm et al. (1999) Harpaz et al. (2002)d Helgstrand & Stuhlmacher (1999) Hui et al. (1991) Countries/ Sample A total of 1,286 undergraduates and high school students in the Philippines and the U.S. A total of 1,491 new work entrants from eight countries participating in the Work Socialization of Youth study (WOSY) 167 high school students in Denmark and the U.S. A total of 160 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models none Key Findings Survey Individualism (Hui, 1988); valence; agreeableness; conscientiousness; civility; refinement; gregariousness Longitudinal survey and Archival Individualism-Collectivism; Uncertainty Avoidance; Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); work centrality; work goal importance Meaning of Work modeln; social exchange theory Survey Individualism-Collectivism; Power Distance; Uncertainty Avoidance; MasculinityFemininity (all assessed using Hofstede, 1980); leadership preferences Collectivism (Hui 1988; Hui & Villareal, 1989); allocator generosity; equity vs. equality norms; friends vs. strangers plentiful vs. fixed resource amounts Implicit leadership theories Danish and U.S. subjects differed significantly only on power distance (Danes lower); respondents in both countries rated leaders who were both feminine and individualistic as most effective. Equity theory; reward allocation rules Hong Kong subjects were significantly more collectivistic than U.S. subjects; when resources were plentiful, HK subjects put greater emphasis on equal allocation than U.S. subjects; when resources were fixed, HK subjects were more generous than U.S. subjects and differentiated between close friends and co-workers; and individualism-collectivism failed to fully mediate country effects. Experiment Filipinos and U.S. respondents did not significantly differ on individualism; in the U.S., individualism was negatively related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, civility, and refinement; in the Philippines, individualism was negatively correlated with gregariousness. Over time, new work entrants from low uncertainty avoidance countries had increased work centrality; new work entrants from highly masculine countries began their careers with higher work centrality than those from more feminine countries (although this hypothesis was only partially supported). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 9 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Hui & Villareal (1989) Countries/ Sample 49 and 160 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S., respectively Method Variables and Measures Survey Individualism-Collectivism; (Hui, 1988); preference for autonomy, deference, affiliation, succorance, and nurturance; need for abasement; social recognition Theories/ Models none James (1993) 50 U.S. undergraduates Survey responses to reading essay Individualism-Collectivism, (Scott, 1965)o; attitudes toward new technology; information retention Theory of reasoned action; selfsystem theory Kirkman & Shapiro (2000) 618 line-level U.S. insurance company employees 491 selfmanaging work team employees in Belgium, Finland, the Philippines and the U.S. Survey Collectivism (Maznevski et al., 2003)p; receptivity to team-based rewards none Survey Collectivism; Power Distance (Maznevski et al., 2003); job satisfaction; organizational commitment; resistance to teams and selfmanagement none Kirkman & Shapiro (2001a) Key Findings In both countries, collectivism was positively related to preferences for affiliation, succorance, and nurturance and low need for autonomy. In Hong Kong, collectivism was negatively related to preference for autonomy, and deference. In the U.S., individualism was positively related to a high need for social recognition and a low need for abasement. Collectivists showed more positive attitudes toward new technology and retained more information about it when a presentation contained a group-relevance theme; individualists showed more positive individual attitudes when presentation contained a self-relevance theme. Collectivism was positively related to receptivity to team-based rewards. Resistance to teams fully mediated the relationship that collectivism had with job satisfaction and organizational commitment; resistance to self-management partially mediated the relationship between power distance and organizational commitment; and individualismcollectivism failed to fully mediate country effects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 10 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Kwan et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample A total of 378 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Survey Individualism and Collectivism (independent and interdependent selfconstrual; Singelis, 1994); self-esteem; relationship harmony; life satisfaction Theories/ Models none Lee et al. (2000a) A total of 540 U.S. and 512 Hong Kong undergraduates Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (independent and interdependent selfconstrual; Singelis, 1994); promotion versus prevention focus Regulatory focus theory Leung (1987) 96 undergraduates and 72 nonstudents in both Hong Kong and the U.S. Experiment Collectivism (Hui, 1984)q; negotiation preferences; rationales behind preferences for certain negotiation strategies none Key Findings U.S. undergraduates were significantly more individualistic (i.e., higher independent selfconstrual) and less collectivistic (i.e., lower interdependent self-construal) than Hong Kong undergraduates; self-esteem mediated the relationship that individualism had with life satisfaction and relationship harmony mediated the relationship between collectivism and life satisfaction (found in both Hong Kong and U.S. samples). U.S. subjects were significantly higher on individualism (i.e., independent self-construal) and lower on collectivism (i.e., interdependent self-construal) than Hong Kong subjects; individualists were attuned toward a promotion focus, seeing potential gains as important and responding to events with relatively greater happiness or dejection, whereas collectivists were attuned toward a more prevention focus, seeing potential losses as more important and responding to events with relatively greater relaxation or anxiety. Hong Kong subjects were significantly more collectivistic than U.S. subjects; HK subjects preferred bargaining and mediation more than U.S. subjects due their beliefs that these strategies helped reduce animosity between disputants. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 11 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Leung (1988) Leung & Iwawaki (1988) Mann et al. (1985) McCleanParks et al. (1999) Countries/ Sample Theories/ Models Procedural justice; expectancy theory Method Variables and Measures 96 undergraduates and 72 nonstudents in both Hong Kong and the U.S. A total of 500 undergraduates in Japan, South Korea, and the U.S. Experiment Collectivism (Hui, 1984); likelihood of suing; stranger versus friend Experiment Collectivism (Hui, 1984); allocator generosity; equity versus equality norms; low versus high inputs; friends versus strangers Equity theory A total of over 300 primary schoolchildren in Australia and Japan A total of 63 MBAs in Singapore and the U.S. Experiment Collectivism (study specific measure); reward allocation decision rules Decision making rules Experiment Individualism (Triandis et al., 1988); resource allocation; resource recovery; time duration of allocation and recovery; equity versus equality norms Prospect theory; social exchange theory; resource recovery rules Key Findings Hong Kong subjects were more likely to sue a stranger than were U.S. subjects; collectivism was associated with higher likelihood of suing when dispute was between strangers but not between friends. Japanese and U.S. subjects did not significantly differ on collectivism; generous allocators were better liked in all three countries and were rated as fairer than less generous allocators in Japan and the U.S.; low-input Japanese and Korean subjects did not follow the equity norm more closely than did U.S. subjects nor did they allocate more equally with friends and more equitably with strangers; collectivism was positively related to the use of the equality rule and negatively to the use of the equity rule. Japanese subjects were significantly more collectivistic than Australian subjects; Japanese subjects were more likely to follow “equal-say” rules while Australians used more “self-interest” rules in actual game experiments. U.S. subjects were, on average, more individualistic than Singaporean subjects; individualists took more time to recover resources than collectivists while the opposite was true for resource allocation; in both cultures, equity rules were used more often when allocating a resource while equality rules were used more often when recovering resources. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 12 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Mitchell et al. (2000)d Moorman & Blakely (1995) Morris et al. (1998) Countries/ Sample 677 business professionals and new venture creators in Canada, the U.S., Mexico, China, Japan, Australia, and Chile 155 employees in a U.S. financial services organization 132 U.S., 100 PRC, 160 Indian, 44 Filipino MBA students Theories/ Models Social cognition theory; expert information processing theory Method Variables and Measures Archival and Survey Individualism; Power Distance (both assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); willingness scripts; ability scripts; arrangements scripts; new venture creation decisions Survey Collectivism (values, norms, beliefs; Wagner & Moch, 1986); organizational citizenship behavior none Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1988); cultural value inventory (Schwartz, 1994); conflict handling styles Thomas & Kilmann’s (1974)r conflict mode framework; need for achievement Key Findings Individualism and power distance were positively related to “ability” scripts (i.e., given resources, the person is able to carry out a goal); individualism was positively related to “willingness” scripts (i.e., given resources, the person will want to carry out a goal); and the relationship between “arrangements” scripts (i.e., access to required materials) and the venture creation decision was stronger for individualists rather than collectivists. Collectivism was positively related to multiple dimensions of organizational citizenship behavior when controlling for procedural justice. The individualism-collectivism measure was unreliable and could not be used in analyses; Chinese managers preferred an avoiding style, while U.S. managers preferred a competing style of handling conflict; self-enhancement (achievement) fully mediated the effects of country on conflict style for U.S. managers; societal conservatism (conformity) fully mediated the effects of country on conflict style for Chinese managers. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 13 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Nooteboom et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample 97 transaction partners in firmsupplier alliances in the Netherlands Method Variables and Measures Survey Uncertainty Avoidance (study specific measure); perceived probability of potential loss by transaction partners Theories/ Models Transaction cost economics; social exchange theory; resource dependency theory; relational contract theory Social identity theory Oyserman (1993) A total of 1,021 undergraduates of various religious backgrounds in Israel Survey Individualism; Collectivism (both from study specific measures); focus on private or public aspects of self; selfconcept; centrality of social identities to self-definition; perceptions of intergroup conflict Oyserman et al. (1998) 93 Jewish American and 80 Asian and Asian American undergraduates Survey Collectivism; Individualism (both assessed from Oyserman, 1993); social obligation; cultural identity; personal goal importance Social identity theory; social categorization theory Ozawa et al. (1996) 58 Japanese and 53 U.S. undergraduates Survey Individualism (self-construal; Singelis et al., 1995); change to an affirmative action program none Key Findings Uncertainty avoidance was negatively related to the probability of a potential loss by transaction partners but unrelated to the actual size of loss. Arabs were significantly higher in both individualism and collectivism compared to Israelis; individualism was positively related to focusing on private aspects of the self and conceptualizing the self in terms of distinctions between the self and others; collectivism (and individualism) was positively related to centrality of social identities to self-definition, a focus on public aspects of the self, and heightened perception of intergroup conflict. Collectivism was positively associated with social obligation; individualism reduced social obligation when participants’ cultural identity was salient and when social obligation competed with important personal goals; collectivism increased obligation to ingroups when cultural identity was salient. U.S. subjects were significantly more individualistic than Japanese; U.S. subjects perceived a change to affirmative action less favorably and as less fair than Japanese subjects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 14 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Pearson & Stephan (1998) Countries/ Sample A total of 419 undergraduates in Brazil and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Dual-concern model Experiment Individualism (Triandis et al., 1988); negotiation tactics Collectivism (Earley, 1989; Wagner & Moch, 1986); charismatic leadership; work unit performance; job satisfaction; satisfaction with leader; leader effectiveness Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism (Singelis et al., 1995); cooperation; single group and intergroup dilemma Theory of mechanistic and organic structures; charismatic leadership Fiske’s (1990)s universal social relationship framework Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1993; Wagner, 1995)t; need for achievement; need for security; human resource practice preferences none Pillai & Meindl (1998) 463 subordinates and 133 supervisors in the U.S. Survey Probst et al. (1999) 165 U.S. undergraduates Experiment Ramamoorthy & Carroll (1998) 342 U.S. undergraduates Survey Key Findings Brazilians scored significantly lower on individualism than did U.S. subjects; Brazilians preferred accommodation, collaboration, and withdrawal tactics more than did U.S. subjects; U.S. subjects preferred competition more than Brazilians; U.S. subjects preferred “high concern for self” tactics while Brazilians preferred “high concern for others” tactics; Brazilians made ingroup-outgroup distinctions while U.S. subjects did not. Collectivism was positively associated with the level of charismatic leadership, which in turn, was associated with higher supervisory ratings of work unit performance, job satisfaction, satisfaction with the leader, and leader effectiveness. Type of social dilemma (i.e., single group vs. intergroup) moderated the main effect relationship between individualism-collectivism and cooperation such that vertical individualists were least cooperative in the single-group dilemma but were more cooperative in the intergroup dilemma; vertical collectivists were most cooperative in the single group dilemma but were less cooperative in the intergroup dilemma. Controlling for need for achievement and security, collectivism was negatively related to use of selection tests, formal appraisal practices, and desire for promotions based on merit and positively related to preference for equality-based rewards and job security. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 15 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Satterwhite et al. (2000) Steensma et al. (2000a) Tafarodi et al. (1999) Countries/ Sample 181 U.S., 291 Taiwanese, and 152 Japanese undergraduates 484 entrepreneurs from Australia, Finland, Greece, Indonesia, Mexico, Norway, and Sweden 94 British and 92 Malaysian undergraduates in Wales Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1988); similarity judgments; ingroup/out-group distinction Survey Individualism; Uncertainty Avoidance; Masculinity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); acceptance of cooperative strategies; importance of contractual safeguards and partner commonality Individualism-Collectivism (Hui, 1988); self-esteem Survey Theories/ Models Tversky’s (1977)u contrast model none none Key Findings U.S. subjects were significantly more individualistic than either Taiwanese or Japanese subjects (who did not differ); U.S. subjects viewed the in-group as more similar to themselves relative to the Taiwanese and Japanese subjects (who did not differ); U.S. and Taiwanese subjects (who did not differ) perceived the out-group as more similar to themselves relative to the Japanese subjects. Entrepreneurs viewed cooperative strategies with other firms as more acceptable when they were lower in individualism and masculinity, but higher in uncertainty avoidance; contractual safeguards were preferred more by those higher in individualism and uncertainty avoidance; partner commonality was positively linked to uncertainty avoidance. Factor analyzed four dimensions of individualismcollectivism: deference to the direction of relatives, connectedness to parents, confiding in others, and instrumental interdependence; in both countries, “deference” and “connectedness” dimensions were positively related to the selfliking dimension of self-esteem but negatively to the self-competence dimension; and individualism-collectivism fully mediated country of origin effects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 16 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Countries/ Sample Method Variables and Measures Collectivism (Maznevski et al., 1997)v; assessments of group processes; group receptiveness; cultural distance Theories/ Models Similarityattraction paradigm; social identity theory Thomas (1999) 77 undergraduates from 14 countries in New Zealand Experiment Thomas & Au (2002) 111 New Zealand and 107 Hong Kong executive education participants A total of 104 businesspeople from Japan, Germany, and the U.S. Survey Horizontal Individualism; Vertical Collectivism (Singelis et al., 1995); exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect; job satisfaction; quality of job alternatives Dyad conflict Individualism (Earley, 1993); simulation hierarchy (Erez & Earley, 1987); polychronicity; explicit contracting; various aspects of conflict management None 94 U.S. and 120 Hong Kong business students Dyad conflict Collectivism (i.e., selfsimulation transcendence; Schwartz, 1994); self-direction; hierarchy; tradition; various conflict management norms; conflict outcomes Social identity theory Tinsley (2001) Tinsley & Brett (2001) Conflict resolution strategies Key Findings Asian respondents were significantly higher on collectivism than New Zealanders and Europeans; collectivism was positively related to participants’ positive assessments of group process but not to receptiveness; participants’ cultural distance on collectivism was positively related to group receptiveness but not to assessments of group process. Horizontal individualism was positively related to voice; vertical collectivism was positively related to neglect and negatively related to loyalty. Japanese respondents were significantly lower on individualism than German or U.S. respondents (who did not differ); individualism was positively related to the use of interest strategies and negatively related to the use of power strategies; and individualism-collectivism and power distance fully mediated country of origin effects. U.S. participants were significantly more selfdirected, less hierarchical, and less traditionbound than Hong Kong subjects; U.S. participants placed greater emphasis on discussing interests and synthesizing multiple interests than did Hong Kong participants; American dyads were significantly more likely to reach an integrative outcome than were Hong Kong dyads; collectivism did not predict cultural differences in outcomes; and individualism-collectivism failed to fully mediate country effects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 17 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Tinsley & Pillutla (1998) Countries/ Sample A total of 231 undergraduates in Hong Kong and the U.S. Method Experiment Variables and Measures Individualism (i.e., selfenhancement; Schwartz, 1994); Collectivism (i.e., self-transcendence; Schwartz, 1994); problem solving strategies Collectivism (Hui, 1988; Hui & Villareal, 1989; Craig & Tower, 1993)w; allocator generosity; equity versus equality norms; low versus high inputs; friends versus strangers Theories/ Models Social comparison processes Tower et al. (1997) 80 students each in England and Russia Experiment Triandis et al. (1988) 300 U.S. undergraduates Survey Individualism (Hui & Triandis, 1986; Hui, 1984)x Social categorization theory; need for achievement 353 student and non-student subjects in Japan, Puerto Rico, and the U.S Survey Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1985)y; conformity; ingroup influence; concern for ingroup members; extent of subordination of own needs Greenwald’s (1982)z model of egoinvolvement and selfawareness Study 1 Study 2 Equity theory; reward allocation rules Key Findings Hong Kong subjects were significantly more collectivistic (self-transcendent) and less individualistic (self-enhanced) than U.S. subjects; U.S. subjects rated self-interested and joint problem solving as more appropriate than Hong Kong subjects. Russian subjects were significantly more collectivistic than English; English subjects allocated more reward to themselves than did Russians; English low performers allocated more to themselves than did Russian low performers; Russians were more generous when their coworker was a friend while Russian high performers allocated more to themselves when their co-worker was a stranger; friendship status was irrelevant to the British. A measure of individualism was factor analyzed and generated six dimensions: more concern for one’s own goals than the ingroup’s goals; less attention to the views of ingroups; self-reliance with competition; detachment from ingroups; deciding on one’s own rather than asking for others’ views; and less general concern for an ingroup. Contrary to expectations, conventional attitudes associated with collectivists (e.g., they conform more, they see themselves as always under the influence of ingroups, they show more concern for their ingroup members, and they subordinate their needs to their ingroups) were not true of Japanese or Puerto Rican respondents. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 18 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Study 3 Countries/ Sample 100 subjects each in Puerto Rico and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Survey Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1985); social support; loneliness; competition; ingroup versus outgroup Van Dyne et al. (2000) 183 matchedpair responses of cooperative housing residents in the U.S. Survey Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1987); organizational citizenship behavior; organization-based selfesteem WadeBenzoni et al. (2002) 256 U.S. MBAs and 128 Japanese undergraduates Four-party negotiation simulation Wojciszke (1997) 89 undergraduate students in Poland Survey Individualism-Collectivism (independent and interdependent selfconstrual; Markus & Kitayama, 1994; Schwartz, 1994)bb; cooperation; equality; egocentrism; and expectations of others’ behavior Rokeach Value Survey (Rokeach, 1973)cc; competence, morality Theories/ Models Kagan and Levi’s (1974)aa cybernetic model Social exchange theory; cognitive consistency theory; cultural selfrepresentation theory Social identity theory Social cognition; social values orientation Key Findings In both countries, collectivism correlated positively with social support and negatively with loneliness; U.S. respondents see competition as occurring between individuals, while Puerto Rican respondents view competition as occurring more between in-/out-groups. Collectivism was positively related to organizational citizenship behavior but organization-based self-esteem fully mediated this relationship. Japanese participants were significantly less individualistic than U.S. participants; U.S. participants were less cooperative, reached fewer equal solutions, and expected others to be less cooperative than their Japanese counterparts. The eight individualistic values on the survey were more strongly related to ratings of competence than to morality and were more strongly related to ratings of self-profitability than other-profitability (an opposite pattern emerged for the eight collectivistic values). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 19 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Yamaguchi et al. (1995) Countries/ Sample A total of 929 undergraduates in Japan, South Korea, and the U.S. Method Survey Variables and Measures Collectivism (Yamaguchi, 1994)dd; affiliative tendency; sensitivity to rejection; need for uniqueness Theories/ Models none Key Findings South Korea was significantly more collectivistic than Japan which was, in turn, significantly more collectivistic than the U.S.; in all three countries, collectivism was positively related to affiliative tendency and sensitivity to rejection and negatively related to need for uniqueness. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 20 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Group/Organization Level of Analysis) Author(s) Cox et al. (1991) Eby & Dobbins (1997)ff Elron (1997) Kirkman & Shapiro (2001b) Countries/ Sample 136 undergraduates in 17 ethnically diverse groups and 16 allAnglo groups in the U.S. 148 undergraduate students in 33 teams in the U.S. 121 intact top management teams in different foreign subsidiaries of U.S. companies 491 employees in 81 selfmanaging work teams in Belgium, Finland, the Philippines, and the U.S. Method Experiment Variables and Measures Collectivism (Triandis et al., 1986; Hui, 1988)ee; cooperative behavior Theories/ Models none Key Findings Ethnically diverse groups (i.e., those higher in collectivism) behaved more cooperatively than all- Anglo groups (i.e., those lower in collectivism). These differences increased when situational cues favored cooperation. ComputerCollectivism (Wagner and simulated Moch, 1986); team strategy game cooperation; team performance Expectancy theory; need for affiliation Team collectivistic orientation was positively related to team cooperation, which mediated the relationship between collectivism and team performance. Survey Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and MasculinityFemininity (all assessed from country scores in Hofstede, 1980); team and subsidiary performance; issue-based conflict Upper echelons paradigm Survey Collectivism (Maznevski et al., 2003); resistance to teams; team cooperation, empowerment, and productivity none Overall top management team heterogeneity on culture was positively related to team performance and issue-based conflict; issue based conflict was, in turn, negatively related to team performance; heterogeneity on both individualism-collectivism and masculinity-femininity was positively related to team performance; heterogeneity in uncertainty avoidance was positively related to issue-based conflict. Resistance to teams mediated the positive relationships that collectivism had with team cooperation, empowerment, and productivity. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 21 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Group/Organization Level of Analysis) Author(s) Morris et al. (1993) Morris et al. (1994) Oetzel (1998) Pillai & Meindl (1998)ii Countries/ Sample 252 senior marketing executives from 84 U.S. manufacturing firms 252 senior marketing executives from 84 U.S. manufacturing firms; 225 managers of various functions in South Africa; 25 senior HR executives in Portugal 10 EuropeanAmerican groups and 10 JapaneseAmerican groups 101 intact work groups in the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Social loafing phenomenon Key Findings Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Kilmann & Saxton, 1983; Hofstede, 1980; Earley, 1989)gg; firm-level entrepreneurial behavior Individualism-collectivism had a curvilinear relationship with entrepreneurial behavior such that at either extreme, entrepreneurial behavior suffers. Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Kilmann & Saxton, 1983; Hofstede, 1980; Earley, 1989); firm-level entrepreneurial behavior Social loafing phenomenon Individualism-collectivism had a curvilinear relationship with entrepreneurial behavior such that at either extreme, entrepreneurial behavior suffers (found in South African and U.S. data but not in Portuguese data). Experiment Individualism-Collectivism, (Gudykunst et al., 1996)hh; cooperation; competition; conflict Effective decision making theory The more individualistic European-American groups had a greater number of conflicts, fewer cooperating tactics, and more competing tactics than the more collectivistic Japanese-American groups. Survey Collectivism (Earley, 1989; Wagner & Moch, 1986); charismatic leadership Theory of mechanistic and organic structures; charismatic leadership Work group collectivism was positively associated with the emergence of charismatic leadership. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 22 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Anand & Delios (1997) Arora & Fosfuri (2000) Barkema et al. (1996) Barkema et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample 1,609 Japanese foreign direct investments from 1994 database 153 North American, Japanese, and Western European chemical firms engaging in licensing or wholly owned subsidiaries between 19811991 Foreign expansions of 13 non-financial Dutch firms between 1966 and 1988 International joint ventures in 25 large Dutch firms from 1966 to 1994 Method Archival Archival Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); choice of foreign entry mode; subsidiary performance Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); technology licensing or wholly owned subsidiaries Theories/ Models Uppsala stage model; transaction cost economics Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased (control variable), Japanese firms were less likely to initiate joint ventures versus greenfields. Transaction cost As the overall cultural distance between countries economics; increased, so did preferences for technology behavioral licensing rather than wholly owned subsidiaries. theory of the firm; “syncretic” theory of the entry mode choice Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); foreign venture longevity Behavioral theory of the firm; Uppsala stage model As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, foreign venture longevity decreased. Survey Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); foreign joint venture longevity Organizational learning theory; Uppsala stage model As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, foreign joint venture longevity decreased. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 23 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Barkema & Vermeulen (1997) Barkema & Vermeulen (1998) Benito & Gripsrud (1992) Brouthers & Brouthers (2000) Countries/ Sample 25 Dutch companies engaged in 828 international joint ventures from 1966 to 1994 25 Dutch companies engaged in 829 international joint ventures from 1966 to 1994 Norwegian companies foreign direct investments in manufacturing in 1970s until 1982 136 Japanese greenfield investments and acquisitions in Europe Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models none Key Findings Archival Overall cultural distance (Kogut & Singh, 1988) and on specific value dimensions based on country scores from Hofstede; survival rate of international joint ventures Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); mode of foreign entry (acquisition or start-up, partially or wholly-owned) Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); foreign direct investment location decisions Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988)jj; organizational learning Economic theory of the MNE; Uppsala stage model There was no effect of overall cultural distance on initial or subsequent foreign direct investment decisions. Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); country scores on Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions; choice between acquisitions or greenfields Transaction cost economics; institutional theory There was no effect of overall cultural distance on the choice between acquisitions or greenfields; uncertainty avoidance was positively related to preference for greenfield startup ventures rather than acquisitions. Archival Archival Overall cultural distance was negatively related to the survival rate of international joint ventures; cultural distance scores on uncertainty avoidance and long-term orientation (and, to a lesser extent, masculinity) had negative effects on survival rate of joint ventures (individualism and power distance were unrelated); effects of cultural distance were stable over time. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased (control variable), firms preferred partially owned start-ups or acquisitions versus wholly owned acquisitions. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 24 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Brouthers & Brouthers (2001) Chang & Rosenzweig (2001) Datta & Puia (1995) Diener & Diener (1995) Countries/ Sample 231 firms in the Netherlands, Germany, Britain, and the U.S. doing business in five central and eastern European countries 119 firms making foreign direct investments into the U.S. from 1975-1992 Completed U.S. cross-border acquisitions between 1978 and 1990 Over 13,000 undergraduates in 31 countries Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Transaction cost economics Survey Overall cultural distance (Morosini et al., 1998) and on specific value dimensions based on country scores from Hofstede; investment risk; entry mode choice Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988) supplemented by Ronen and Shenkar’s (1985)kk country clusters; entry mode choice Transaction cost economics; knowledgebased theory of the firm Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); shareholder wealth Individualism-Collectivism (country ratings by Triandis, personal communication, 1992); wealth; cultural heterogeneity Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988) Archival and Survey Maslow’s hierarchy of needs Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, firms preferred joint venture modes of entry rather than full control modes; when analyzed separately, cultural distance on individualism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity were positively related to the use of joint ventures; however, firms entering culturally distant, high risk investment markets prefer wholly owned entry modes. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, preferences for greenfield and joint venture entry increased (with joint ventures preferred over greenfield) and preference for acquisition entry decreased; the effects of cultural distance decreased after initial entry; country cluster analysis allowed for more fine-grained analyses. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, shareholder wealth in those firms making cross-border acquisitions decreased as a result of the acquisition. Individualism was positively correlated with national wealth but negatively with cultural heterogeneity. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 25 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Diener et al. (1995) Countries/ Sample Over 120,000 undergraduate students in 55 countries Survey Erramilli (1991) 151 U.S. service firms Archival and Survey Erramilli (1996) 337 subsidiaries belonging to U.S. and European advertising firms Archival Author(s) Method Variables and Measures Individualism-Collectivism (country scores, Hofstede; 1980; country ratings by Triandis, personal communication, 1992); wealth; human rights; growth of wealth; income social comparison; equality; cultural heterogeneity Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); entry mode (full versus shared control); pre-entry international experience; size Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); level of ownership in foreign subsidiaries Theories/ Models Maslow’s hierarchy of needs; social comparison After controlling for income, human rights, social equality, and heterogeneity, only individualism was positively related to the subjective well being of nations. Uppsala stage model; transaction cost economics As the pre-entry international experience of a firm increased, so did the frequency of entering markets that are cultural dissimilar from the host country. none While controlling for overall cultural distance, the greater the power distance and uncertainty avoidance characterizing a firm’s home country, the more that firm sought majority ownership in foreign subsidiaries; overall cultural distance was not related. Key Findings Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 26 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Erramilli et al. (1997) Countries/ Sample 177 Korean multinational company subsidiaries Method Archival Erramilli et al. (2002) 139 hotel managers representing 46 different countries Survey Franke et al. (1991) Data from 18 countries common to Hofstede (1980) and the Chinese Culture Connection (1987) 75 joint ventures formed by 94 British firms between 1980 and 1998 Archival Glaister & Buckley (1999) Archival and Survey Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); subsidiary ownership preferences (minority joint venture, equal joint venture, majority joint venture, sole venture) Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); franchising and management-service contracts Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); economic growth Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); subjective measures of performance Theories/ Models Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988) Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, Korean firms were more likely to choose higher equity ventures. Organizational capability perspective As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the propensity to choose a management-service contract, rather than franchising, increased. none Confucian dynamism was a strong predictor of economic growth for the periods 1965-1980 and 1980-1987; individualism was also a strong negative predictor of growth but only for the first time period. Transaction cost economics There was no effect of overall cultural distance on subjective assessments of joint venture performance. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 27 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Gomez-Mejia & Palich (1997) Gong et al. (2001) Habib & Zurawicki (2002) Hakanson & Nobel (2001) Countries/ Sample Culturallyrelated international diversification in Fortune 500 firms 265 CEOs of international joint ventures in the PRC Three-year bilaterial foreign direct investment data representing 89 different countries obtained from the International Monetary Fund data base in 2002 75 foreign research and development units based in Swedish multinationals Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Transaction costs economics; agency theory Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); firm performance Survey Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (1980); role ambiguity; role conflict; IJV performance Transaction cost economics; role theory Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); foreign direct investment Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988) Survey Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); reverse technology transfer; integration; local embeddedness; innovation Embeddedness; Institutionalizati on theory Key Findings There was no effect of overall cultural distance on firm performance. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the level of role conflict and role ambiguity decreased; cultural distance and role conflict were also positively related to CEO subjective ratings of international joint venture performance. There was no effect of overall cultural distance on amount of foreign direct investment. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, embeddedness and integration decreased especially for greenfield sites compared to acquisitions; the probability of reverse transfer was positively related to the innovativeness and level of integration. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 28 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Harzing (2002) Hennart & Larimo (1998) House et al. (1999) Husted (1999) Countries/ Sample 287 subsidiaries of multinational companies located in 22 countries Method Survey 28 joint ventures between two Japanese firms and 10 between two Finnish firms Middle managers in 54 countries Archival 40 countries Archival Survey Theories/ Models Overall cultural distance International based on country scores strategy models; from Hofstede (Kogut & institutional Singh, 1988); entry mode theory; (greenfield vs. acquisition); integration/ control mechanisms; responsiveness presence of expatriates; framework local responsiveness (Prahalad & Doz, 1987)ll Overall cultural distance “National based on country scores character” from Hofstede (Kogut & theory of Singh, 1988); preferences subsidiary for ownership ownership; transaction costs Collectivism; Power Implicit Distance; Uncertainty leadership Avoidance (GLOBE - study theory; specific measures); styles McClelland’s of leadership motivation theory; structural contingency theory of organizational form Hofstede’s cultural value Transaction cost dimensions (using country economics; scores from Hofstede, game theory 1980); corruption Variables and Measures Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased (control variable), acquisitions were less likely. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, shared-equity ventures were preferred over wholly-owned subsidiaries. Collectivism was positively associated with teamoriented leadership and both power distance and uncertainty avoidance were negatively associated with participative leadership. Power distance, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity were positively related to national levels of corruption. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 29 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Jones & Teegen (2001) Kallunki et al. (2001) Countries/ Sample U.S. multinational corporation R&D investment in 39 countries 79 foreign direct investments made by 19 Finnish firms from 1985-1996 Kashlak et al. Alliances and (1998) contracts between U.S. firms in the telecommunicati ons industry and firms in 109 other countries from 1982 to 1993 Kim & Hwang 96 U.S.-based (1992) senior-level managers of international operations Method Archival Archival Survey Survey Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); Individualism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, MasculinityFemininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); foreign R&D investment of U.S. firms Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); abnormal returns (stock price) Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); structural changes in alliances and contracts Perceived cultural distance (study specific measure); entry mode (licensing, joint venture, wholly owned subsidiary) Theories/ Models Theory of foreign direct investment Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, U.S. R&D investment abroad decreased; investment was also positively related to individualism and negatively related to power distance and uncertainty avoidance; there were no significant relationships involving masculinityfemininity; when considered together, only individualism was a significant predictor of U.S. R&D investment abroad. Transaction cost economics There were no effects of overall cultural distance on abnormal returns after foreign direct investments were made. Transaction cost economics; alliance theory As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, greater structural changes in alliances and contracts took place. Transaction cost economics As the perceived cultural distance between the U.S. and foreign country increased, firms tended to avoid wholly owned subsidiaries or joint venturing in pursuit of the lower resource commitment of licensing. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 30 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Kogut & Singh (1988) Krug & Nigh (1998) Li & Guisinger (1991) Li & Guisinger (1992) Countries/ Sample Data on U.S. joint ventures, acquisitions, and greenfields from 1981 to 1985 Method Archival 103 U.S. companies acquired by foreign firms between 1986 and 1989 Data on 85 foreigncontrolled business failures in the U.S. Archival Published data on 158 large service multinational enterprises (MNEs) from 1976-1980 and 168 MNEs from 1980-1986 (six countries) Archival Archival Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (study specific measure based on Hofstede, 1980); country scores on Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions (Hofstede, 1980); investment strategy choice Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); top management departure rates Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); foreign controlled business failures in the U.S. Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); U.S. foreign direct investment Theories/ Models Transaction cost economics; institutional theory Corporate governance theory; transaction cost economics Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988); transaction cost economics Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988); internationalizat ion theory; oligopolistic reaction Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the tendency to choose a joint venture over an acquisition increased; the level of uncertainty avoidance in the home country was positively related to the tendency to use joint ventures over greenfields or acquisitions. As the overall cultural distance between the U.S. and other countries increased, the greater were the level of post-acquisition top management departures; both foreign and U.S. company international experience moderated (weakened) the cultural distance-departure relationship. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the failure rate of U.S. affiliates whose partners came from culturally dissimilar countries was higher. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the amount of U.S. foreign direct investment decreased. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 31 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Loree & Guisinger (1995) Luo (2001a) Luo (2001b) Luo (2002) Countries/ Method Sample U.S. Department Archival of Commerce’s Benchmark Surveys in 1977 and 1982 282 Archival and international Survey cooperative ventures (ICVs) in the PRC 76 wholly owned subsidiaries and 92 foreigndominated equity joint ventures in the PRC 255 international equity manufacturing joint ventures originating in the PRC Survey Archival and Survey Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); U.S. FDI Theories/ Models none Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); personal attachment; ICV performance Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); local responsiveness; environmental complexity Embeddedness; transaction cost economics; social exchange theory Perceived cultural distance between PRC and other countries (study specific measure); IJV sales and return on investment Transaction cost economics; social capital; embeddedness Global integration-local responsiveness paradigm; transaction cost economics Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the amount of U.S. foreign direct investment decreased. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the level of personal attachment between ICV managers decreased; the level of personal attachment between ICV managers was positively related to ICV return on investment and process performance. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, local responsiveness decreased; environmental complexity was also positively related to local responsiveness. As the perceived cultural distance between the PRC and other countries increased (control variable), sales per asset resulting from the IJV decreased (return on investment is unaffected). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 32 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Luo & Park (2001) Luo & Peng (1999) Countries/ Sample 55 wholly owned subsidiaries, 49 equity joint ventures, and 9 contractual joint ventures in the PRC 108 upper managers of MNE subunits in the PRC Method Variables and Measures Archival and Survey Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); subsidiary strategic orientation; environmental dimensions; subsidiary performance Survey Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); international expansion performance Overall cultural distance (Euclidean distance measure using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); strength of both instrumental and expressive network ties Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); abnormal return (difference of stock market return associated with JV announcement and firm’s normal return) Manev & Stevenson (2001) 203 managers in a single UKbased multinational enterprise Survey Merchant & Schendel (2000) 101 U.S. firm international joint venture formation announcements Archival Theories/ Models Miles and Snow’s (1978)mm strategy typology; institutional theory Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased (control variable), subsidiary return on assets decreased; analyzer strategy was positively related to multiple performance indicators. Organizational learning; Uppsala model; eclectic theory; resource dependency Embeddedness; Social network theory As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, international expansion performance decreased. Transaction cost economics; institutional theory There was no effect of overall cultural distance on abnormal return as a result of U.S. international JV formation announcement. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, managers had stronger instrumental ties, but weaker expressive ties, in their organizational networks. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 33 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Morosini et al. (1998) Countries/ Sample 52 companies that had engaged in cross-border activity with Italy between 1987 and 1992 Method Survey Nachum (2003) 303 publicly traded service MNEs in 20 countries Archival and Survey Newman & Nollen (1996) 176 European and Asian work units of a U.S. manufacturing multinational located in 18 countries Survey Offerman & Hellmann (1997) Over 400 middle managers of a single multinational representing 39 nationalities Survey Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); uncertainty avoidance (country scores from Hofstede, 1980); the performance of crossborder acquisitions Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); greenfield and acquisition entry modes Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using dichotomies of hi/lo based on country scores from Hofstede, 1980); return on assets, sales, bonus levels Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Individualism-Collectivism, Masculinity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); leadership practices and behaviors Theories/ Models Institutional theory Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the performance of cross-border acquisitions increased. Eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988); Institutional theory none As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, preferences for greenfields decreased. none Power distance was negatively related to leader communication, approachability, delegation, and team building; uncertainty avoidance was positively associated with leader control and negatively associated with delegation and approachability. When managers fit their organizational practices to the values of a country’s culture, their units had higher returns on assets, higher returns on sales, and in some cases higher bonus levels than units in which fit was lacking. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 34 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Oyserman et al. (2002) Padmanabhan & Cho (1996) Pan (1996) Pan (2002) Countries/ Sample 83 empirical studies in the meta-analysis; 170 studies in the qualitative review 1,519 foreign direct investments made by 402 Japanese firms in 45 countries from 1969 to 1991 4,223 international equity joint ventures originating in the PRC from 1979 to 1992 8,078 international joint ventures in the PRC from 1979 to 1996 Theories/ Models none Method Variables and Measures Key Findings Metaanalysis and a Qualitative review Individualism-Collectivism (meta-analysis); selfconcept; self-esteem; wellbeing; attribution; relationality Archival Overall cultural distance Transaction cost based on country scores economics from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); full versus shared ownership of foreign affiliates Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); ownership shares Transaction cost economics; eclectic theory (Dunning, 1988) As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, equity joint venture partners were more likely to acquire an equal or majority share, rather than a minority share. Archival Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); level of equity ownership in IJV in foreign country None Uncertainty avoidance was positively associated with firms’ preference for equity ownership in international joint ventures in China. North Americans (U.S. and Canadians) were more individualistic and less collectivistic than most countries included in the meta-analytic comparison; effects sizes tended to be small and varied depending on the content included in the various measures. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, Japanese firms were more likely to own their affiliates as opposed to sharing ownership, especially in non-restrictive host countries in which there is freedom to choose among alternative ownership structures. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 35 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Pan & Tse (1996) Pan & Tse (2000) Countries/ Sample 483 cooperative arrangements in the PRC between pairs of firms from Hong Kong, the U.S., Japan, Germany, and Britain Over 10,000 foreign entry activities into China (19791998) Method Variables and Measures Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); choice of exporting, contractual joint ventures, equity joint ventures, and whollyowned operations Archival Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); entry mode choice of wholly owned subsidiary, EJVs, contractual agreements, and export Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); the rate of joint venture dissolution Park & Ungson (1997) 137 cross-border Archival joint ventures and 49 domestic (U.S.-U.S.) joint ventures Peterson et al. (1995) 21 countries Archival and Survey Theories/ Models Transaction cost economics There was no effect of overall cultural distance on choice of cooperative arrangement. Transaction cost economics; hierarchical model of entry modes Firms from countries with large power distance preferred subsidiary and EJV entry modes while firms from countries high in uncertainty avoidance preferred contract agreements and export entry modes. Transaction cost economics; institutional theory; population ecology Individualism-Collectivism, Bureaucratic Power Distance, management Uncertainty Avoidance, and theory Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); role conflict; role ambiguity; role overload Key Findings As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the rate of joint venture dissolution decreased. Collectivism and power distance were negatively related to role ambiguity and positively related to role overload. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 36 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Peterson & Smith (1997) Countries/ Sample 32 countries Method Variables and Measures Archival and Survey IndividualismCollectivism; Power Distance; Uncertainty Avoidance; MasculinityFemininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); role ambiguity; role overload; ambient temperature Overall cultural distance (Kogut & Singh, 1988) and on specific value dimensions based on country scores from Hofstede; international joint venture satisfaction, efficiency, competitiveness Pothukuchi et al. (2002) 127 international joint ventures between Indian firms and firms in 21 other countries from 1992-1997 Archival and Survey Reuer (2001) 139 joint ventures with U.S. acquiring firms in 24 countries Archival Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988) supplemented by Ronen and Shenkar’s (1985) country clusters; parent firm valuation effect of JV termination Theories/ Models Institutional theory; eco-cultural theory Transaction cost economics Transaction cost economics Key Findings Power distance was the stronger predictor of role overload and role ambiguity even when the ambient temperature of the city in which the data collection took place was accounted for. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, so did the efficiency and competitive of international joint ventures; cultural distance on masculinity was positively associated with all performance dimensions; cultural distance on individualism-collectivism, power distance, and uncertainty avoidance were all negatively associated with international joint venture satisfaction. As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, the payoffs from JV partner buyouts decreased; country clusters allowed for more finegrained analyses. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 37 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) RiahiBelkaoui (1998) Countries/ Sample 16 countries Method Archival Richards & De Carolis (2003) 1,377 joint ventures originating in 47 countries Archival and Survey Ronkainen & GuerreroCusumano (2001) Data from 50 countries in Hofstede (1980) and the 1997 Global Software Piracy Report Archival Roth & O’Donnell (1996) Senior managers from 100 foreign subsidiaries from multiple countries Survey Variables and Measures Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); systematic risk in stock exchanges Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); R&D activity Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); intellectual property rights violations Overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); proportion of incentive-based compensation Theories/ Models Asset pricing theory Key Findings Individualism and power distance were positively related to a country’s systematic risk in its stock exchange while uncertainty avoidance was negatively related to systematic risk. Transaction cost economics; Coevolution theory (Koza & Lewin, 1988)nn none As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, joint venture partners were less likely to engage in R&D activities. Agency theory As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, a greater proportion of incentive-based compensation was used for subsidiary managers of host-country foreign affiliates. Countries with higher scores on collectivism, power distance, and uncertainty avoidance had higher rates of intellectual property rights violations; masculinity-femininity was not significantly related. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 38 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Ryan et al. (1999) Countries/ Sample Managers in over 1,000 companies in 20 countries Method Variables and Measures Survey Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); employee selection practices Theories/ Models none Salter & Niswander (1995) Stock market data and accounting values from 29 countries in 1989 and 1990 Archival Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); accounting values Gray’s (1988)oo model of accounting values Schneider & DeMeyer (1991) 303 MBA student and executives from multiple countries Survey responses based on reading case Country clusters based on Hofstede’s country scores (Ronen & Shenkar, 1985); reactions to crisis in a case Prospect theory Key Findings As the level of uncertainty avoidance increased, the number used and extent of verification methods in selection practices decreased but the number of test types, extent of testing, number of interviews, use of a fixed list of interviews, and the number of selection process auditing methods increased; as power distance increased, the overall number of interviews used in selection and peer involvement in decision making increased while the use of peers as interviewers decreased. Uncertainty avoidance was negatively related to the use of professional accounting standards (i.e., prescriptive legal requirements, statutory control) and positively related to uniformity (i.e., whether a country’s legal system is code law based and therefore uniform), conservatism (i.e., systems designed to stop financial reporting that increases income or assets), and secrecy (i.e., preference for confidentiality and non-disclosure of information); masculinity was positively related to uniformity of accounting principles in practice and negatively related to conservatism; and individualism was negatively related to secrecy. Compared to managers from around the world, Latin Europeans (e.g., France, Belgium, Italy, Spain, and Portugal) were the most likely to interpret the issue in the case as a crisis and as a threat and recommend proactive behavior. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 39 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Schuler & Rogovsky (1998) Countries/ Sample Worldwide compensation data Method Variables and Measures Archival and Survey IndividualismCollectivism; Power Distance; Uncertainty Avoidance; MasculinityFemininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); compensation preferences Power Distance (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); preferences for foreign direct investment versus licensing; overall cultural distance Power Distance (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); overall cultural distance based on country scores from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); preferences for foreign direct investment versus licensing; overall cultural distance Shane (1992) Over 100 manufacturing industries investing in 33 countries from 1977 to 1986 Archival Shane (1994) Licensing and foreign direct investment from over 100 manufacturing industries investing in 50 countries in 1977 and 1982 Archival Theories/ Models Resource-based theory of the firm Transaction cost economics Transaction cost economics Key Findings Individualism was positively related to the use of pay-for-performance and individual performance, social benefits such as child care and career breaks, and employee stock ownership plans (ESOPs); power distance was negatively related to social benefits and ESOPs; uncertainty avoidance was positively related to seniority- and skill-based pay plans and ESOPs and negatively related to a focus on individual performance; masculinity was positively related to individual bonuses and commissions, career breaks, and maternity leave was negatively related to flexible benefits and childcare. While controlling for overall cultural distance, power distance was positively related to preferences for foreign direct investment over licensing; overall cultural distance was related to less licensing and more foreign direct investment. While controlling for overall cultural distance, licensing was preferred in countries in which trust was greater (i.e., countries low in power distance). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 40 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Shane (1995) Countries/ Sample Over 4,000 employees in 68 countries Method Survey Shane et al. (1995) Over 1,000 employees in 30 countries Survey Shenkar & Zeira (1992) 44 international joint ventures originating in Israel between 1985 and 1987 Managers in 23 countries Survey Smith et al. (1998) Survey Theories/ Models Individualism-Collectivism, Institutional Power Distance, theory; Uncertainty Avoidance, and transaction cost Masculinity-Femininity (all economics assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); innovation championing roles Individualism-Collectivism, none Power Distance, and Uncertainty Avoidance (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); innovation championing strategies Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance Role theory and on specific value dimensions (using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); CEO role ambiguity Individualism-Collectivism, none Masculinity, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Power Distance; (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); conflict management style Key Findings Lower levels of uncertainty avoidance were associated with preferences for four innovating championing roles: the organizational maverick, the network facilitator, the transformational leader, and the organizational buffer. As the level of uncertainty avoidance increased, preferences for champions to work through organizational norms, rules, and procedures to promote innovation increased; higher power distance levels were associated with an increased preference for champions to focus on gaining support from those in authority; more collectivistic societies preferred champions to seek cross-functional support for their ideas. Specific cultural distance scores on individualismcollectivism and uncertainty avoidance were negatively related to CEO role ambiguity while overall cultural distance was not related. Power distance was negatively related to the frequency of out-group disagreements; within collectivistic countries, disagreements were more frequently handled through reliance on rules rather than personal experience or training; within low power distance nations, ingroup disagreements were handled more frequently through reliance on subordinates while out-group disagreements were more frequently handled through reliance on peers. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 41 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Spector et al. (2001b) Countries/ Sample 5,185 managers from 24 countries Method Survey Steensma et al. (2000b) 494 manufacturing firms in Australia, Indonesia, Norway, and Sweden Survey Thomas & Grosse (2001) 176 foreign direct investments from 11 countries into Mexico from 1980-1995 1,800 undergraduate students in nine countries Archival 21 countries Archival and Survey Thomas & Mueller (2000) Van de Vliert & Van Yperen (1996) Survey Theories/ Models Individualism-Collectivism, Various models (Hofstede, 1994)pp; work of job design locus of control; and stress psychological and physical well-being; job satisfaction Individualism, Uncertainty Transaction cost Avoidance, Masculinityeconomics; Femininity (all assessed dependency using country scores from theory Hofstede, 1980); propensity of firm-to-firm cooperation for technology innovation alliances Overall cultural distance Eclectic theory based on country scores (Dunning, 1988) from Hofstede (Kogut & Singh, 1988); FDI in Mexico Variables and Measures Overall cultural distance based on Hofstede’s country scores (Kogut & Singh, 1988); innovativeness; locus of control; risk-taking; energy level Power Distance (assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); role ambiguity; role overload; ambient temperature Key Findings Higher scores on individualism were positively related to internal locus of control; individualismcollectivism was not related, however, to wellbeing at the ecological level; individualism and job satisfaction were positively related. Firms in more masculine cultures were less likely to pursue technology alliances than were firms in more feminine cultures; firms in more individualistic cultures were less likely to pursue equity ties in their alliance formation than were firms in more collectivistic cultures. As the overall cultural distance between Mexico and other countries increased, the higher the level of FDI into Mexico; cultural distance was not significant when examining FDI into the maquiladora region of Mexico separately. none As the overall cultural distance between countries increased, entrepreneurship-related attributes such as internal locus of control, risk-taking, and energy levels decreased. none Average daytime temperature for a country’s capital was positively related to role overload while power distance was not related (see Peterson et al., 1995, and Peterson & Smith, 1997, for related studies). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 42 Appendix A - Type I Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Main Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Weber et al. (1996) Zaheer & Zaheer (1997) Countries/ Method Sample Eight Archival and international and Survey eight domestic international mergers and acquisitions from 1985 to 1987 in the U.S. 25 countries Archival Variables and Measures Individualism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (cultural distance on each using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); corporate culture distance; stress; commitment; cooperation Individualism-Collectivism and Uncertainty Avoidance (dichotomous classification as high or low using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); information seeking in networks Theories/ Models none Institutional theory; convergence theory Key Findings National culture distance better predicted stress, negative attitudes towards the merger, and actual cooperation than did corporate culture distance. People in countries high in individualism exhibited lower levels of information seeking in their networks. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 43 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Au et al. (2001) Brockner et al. (2000) Countries/ Sample 87 PRC and 91 Canadian undergraduates 89 managers in Taiwan and Canada; 96 graduate students in the PRC; 74 U.S. MBA students; 86 U.S. MBA students from 20 cultures Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Hui, 1988); post-complaint behavior; fairness of the complaint process; selfattribution; attribution to the service provider Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (independentinterdependent selfconstrual; Singelis et al., 1995); procedural fairness; outcome favorability; reactions to a decision Theories/ Models Procedural justice; attribution theory Social exchange theory; equity theory; procedural justice Key Findings Canadians were significantly more individualistic than the Chinese; country moderated the relationship between voice and taking responsibility such that when a service provider offered voice, Canadians were less likely to attribute the responsibility to themselves than were the Chinese. Only country, and not INDCOL, was tested as a moderator. A three-way interaction (country X procedural fairness X outcome favorability) showed that when procedural fairness was high, outcome favorability had less of an impact on reactions to a decision in Taiwan than Canada; U.S. subjects had significantly higher independent selfconstrual than PRC; results were generally replicated substituting individualism-collectivism for country in the three-way interaction; individualism-collectivism also mediated the moderating effects of country. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 44 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Brockner et al. (2001) Chen et al. (1998b) Dickson & Weaver (1997) Countries/ Sample 254 MBA students in the PRC and the U.S.; 297 business students in Mexico and the U.S.; 450 undergraduate students in Hong Kong and Germany; 102 managers and 253 employees in the PRC A total of almost 400 undergraduates from the PRC and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Theories/ Models Voice effect in procedural justice judgments Experiment Power distance (four different measures including two study specific measures and Maznevski et al., 2003, and Earley & Erez, 1997); voice; procedural fairness perceptions; organizational commitment Experiment Collective primacy orientation (Singelis, 1994; Triandis, et al., 1986); ingroup favoritism; performance Social identity theory Individualism-Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1987); perceived uncertainty; inter-firm alliance use Transaction cost economics; resource dependency; institutional theory Top managers in Survey 433 Norwegian manufacturing firms Key Findings Country moderated the relationship between voice and commitment such that participants were more likely to respond unfavorably (i.e., with lower levels of commitment) to low levels of voice when they were in low power distance countries (e.g., the U.S., Germany) rather than in high power distance countries (e.g., the PRC, Mexico); results were replicated in each study when power distance was substituted for country; in addition, power distance mediated the moderating effects of country showing that culture was the explanation for country effects. Participants from the PRC had more of a collective primacy than U.S. subjects, and the PRC subjects exhibited more ingroup favoritism when they performed well individually while their group performed poorly; results were replicated substituting collective primacy for country as a moderator; in some cases, collective primacy mediated the moderating effects of country Individualism-collectivism moderated the relationship between perceived uncertainty and the odds of using alliances such that the relationship was stronger when managers were collectivistic rather than individualistic. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 45 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Dorfman & Howell (1988) Earley (1989) Countries/ Sample A total of 752 Mexican, Taiwanese, and U.S. managers working in Mexico and Taiwan A total of 96 managerial trainees in the PRC and the U.S. Method Survey Experiment Theories/ Models Individualism-Collectivism, none Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using study specific measures); leader behavior; subordinate performance and satisfaction Variables and Measures Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1987); performance; individual versus shared responsibility; high versus low accountability Social loafing phenomenon Key Findings Cultural socialization (i.e., strong beliefs in the key cultural values of a society) moderated the relationship between directive leadership and subordinate performance and satisfaction such that for individuals with high, rather than low, cultural socialization, directive leadership had stronger relationships to outcomes; the effects of contingent reward leadership behaviors remained invariant regardless of the levels of cultural socialization. Participants from the PRC were significantly higher on collectivism than U.S. participants; in a three-way interaction, collectivism moderated the relationship between both accountability and shared responsibility and performance such that highly individualistic people performed poorest under conditions of high shared responsibility and low accountability; highly collectivistic people performed better under conditions of high shared responsibility, regardless of accountability; country did not explain any unique variance in performance beyond collectivism; country was substituted for culture in all analyses and similar results were obtained; thus, social loafing tended to occur in the U.S. but not in the PRC Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 46 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Earley (1993) Earley (1994) Countries/ Sample A total of 165 managers from the PRC, Israel, and the U.S. A total of 251 managers in Hong Kong, the PRC, and the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Erez & Earley, 1987); performance; ingroup versus outgroup; social loafing; self and group efficacy beliefs Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Earley, 1993); self-focused versus group-focused training; self-efficacy; performance Theories/ Models Social loafing phenomenon; agency theory; social identity theory; expectancy theory Self-efficacy phenomenon Key Findings U.S. managers were significantly lower on collectivism than PRC and Israeli managers (who did not differ); individualism-collectivism moderated the relationship between group condition (i.e., working in an ingroup, outgroup or alone) and individual performance such that the performance of individualists who thought they were working in an ingroup or an outgroup was lower than the performance of individualists working alone; the performance of collectivists was lower in an individual or outgroup context than in an ingroup context; and participant ratings of self or group efficacy and their anticipated performance outcomes mediated the effects of individualism-collectivism on performance. Country moderated the effects of type of training (individual- vs. group-focused) on self-efficacy and performance such that self-focused training had a stronger impact on self-efficacy and performance for U.S. subjects while groupfocused training had stronger effects for Hong Kong and PRC subjects; within all three countries, individualists responded more favorably to individual-focused training than group-focused training and collectivists showed the opposite pattern; collectivism mediated the moderating effects of country of origin. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 47 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Erez & Earley (1987) Eylon & Au (1999) Countries/ Sample 180 undergraduates from the U.S., urban Israel, kibbutzim Israel 135 mixed origin MBAs in Canada Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism and Power Distance (study specific measures based on Hofstede, 1980); goal acceptance; performance; goal strategies In-basket simulation Power Distance (participants were divided into high and low power distance groups by language and by countryof-origin); empowerment; job satisfaction; performance Theories/ Models Goal setting Empowerment; self-efficacy phenomenon; expectancy theory Key Findings U.S. subjects were significantly lower on both collectivism and power distance than Israeli subjects; country moderated the relationship between type of goal (assigned, representative, or participative) and individual performance such that the relationships between both representative and participative goal setting and performance were stronger in Israel versus the U.S.; identical findings were obtained when power distance (but not collectivism) was substituted for country such that the relationships between both participative and representative goal setting and performance were stronger for those lower, rather than higher, in power distance. Power distance grouping moderated the relationship between empowerment condition (i.e., empowered, control, disempowered) and job performance such that for participants in the high power distance grouping, the disempowerment condition was associated with higher job performance than the empowered or control condition; participants low in power distance performed similarly across empowerment conditions; power distance grouping did not moderate the relationship between empowerment condition and job satisfaction; power distance was not directly assessed. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 48 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Gelfand & Realo (1999) Countries/ Sample 102 Caucasian and Asian American undergraduates in the U.S. and 109 in Estonia Jung & Avolio A total of 347 (1999) Asian and U.S. undergraduates Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis, 1994)qq; negotiator accountability; psychological states, behaviors, and outcomes Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (Bass & Avolio, 1997)ss; transactional versus transformational leadership; number of ideas generated Theories/ Models Adams’s (1976)rr boundary role model Transformationa l and transaction leadership models Key Findings Asians were significantly higher on collectivism than U.S. students; Estonian and U.S. students did not differ; collectivism moderated the relationship between accountability and profit from negotiations such that in high accountability negotiations, the more collectivistic the dyad, the higher the level of: willingness to concede, cooperative behavior, profit from the negotiation, and positive impressions of one’s opponent; in low accountability negotiations, collectivism was negatively associated with these outcomes. Asian students were significantly higher on collectivism than U.S. students; country moderated the relationship between leadership style and quantity of ideas such that Asian students generated more ideas working with a transformational leader rather than transactional; U.S. students generated more ideas with a transactional leader versus transformational; group performance was greater than individual performance; there were no statistical tests involving individualism-collectivism directly. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 49 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Lam et al. (2002a) Countries/ Sample 265 Hong Kong and 288 U.S. bank employees of a multinational bank Method Variables and Measures Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998)tt; participative decision making opportunity; participation efficacy; individual performance Theories/ Models Self-efficacy; collective efficacy; shared mental models Lam et al. (2002b) 218 Hong Kong and 185 U.S. bank tellers of a multinational bank Survey Individualism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998); Power Distance (Erez & Earley, 1987); distributive and procedural justice; job satisfaction; performance; absenteeism Equity theory Lee et al. (2000b) 729 university employees in Hong Kong Survey Power Distance (Hofstede, 1980); trust in supervisor; psychological contract fulfillment Procedural justice Key Findings U.S. employees were significantly higher on individualism and lower on collectivism than Hong Kong employees; in a three-way interaction, collectivism moderated the relationship between both participative decision making and participative efficacy and individual performance such that participative decision making and individual performance were positively related for individuals high in individualism and participation efficacy; further analyses showed that individualism mediated the moderating effects of country of origin. Hong Kong tellers were significantly higher in power distance, but lower in individualism, than U.S. tellers; power distance (but not individualism) moderated the relationship between both distributive and procedural justice and work outcomes such that the relationships between the two types of justice and job satisfaction, performance, and absenteeism were more strongly positive for those lower, rather than higher, in power distance; there were no moderating effects for country of origin. Power distance moderated the relationships between both distributive and procedural justice and both trust in supervisor and psychological contract fulfillment such that the relationship between procedural justice and trust in supervisor was stronger for those lower, rather than higher, in power distance; the relationship between distributive justice and psychological contract fulfillment was stronger for those lower, rather than higher, in power distance. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 50 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Lind et al. (1997) Martella & Maas (2000) Countries/ Sample Over 1000 undergraduates from Germany, Hong Kong, Japan, and the U.S. 229 employees and students in northern and southern Italy Theories/ Models Group-value theory of justice; social identity theory Method Variables and Measures Experiment Power Distance (Hofstede, 1980); voice; procedural fairness perceptions Survey Individualism-Collectivism none (Triandis, 1989; Triandis et. al. 1988)uu; life satisfaction; happiness; self-esteem Key Findings Chinese respondents had the highest level of power distance, followed by Americans and then Germans in Study 1; Japanese respondents had higher power distance than Americans in Study 2; country failed to moderate the relationship between typical relational antecedents of procedural fairness (i.e., voice, trust, status recognition, perceived neutrality and reported use of procedures) and actual procedural fairness judgments; the only exception was in Study 2 in which status recognition was more strongly related to procedural justice judgments in the U.S. compared to Japan; there were no statistical tests involving power distance directly. Southern Italians were more collectivistic than northern Italians; region moderated the relationship between being unemployed and having lower life satisfaction, self-esteem, and happiness such that the relationships were stronger for northern Italians then for southern Italians; there were no interaction tests involving individualism-collectivism directly. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 51 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Ng & Van Dyne (2001) Countries/ Sample 690 U.S. undergraduates in 162 groups Method Variables and Measures Experiment Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998); improvement in decision quality; minority influence Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); job scope; participative management; extrinsic rewards; role clarity; organizational commitment Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998; job demands; job control; self-efficacy; collective efficacy; absenteeism; turnover intention; anxiety; depression Palich et al. (1995) Almost 2,000 managers in 15 European and Canadian affiliates of a U.S. multinational Survey Schaubroeck et al. (2000) A total of 431 bank tellers in Hong Kong and the U.S. Survey Theories/ Models none none Karasek’s (1979)vv job demand-control (decision latitude) model; self- and collective efficacy Key Findings Horizontal individualism and horizontal collectivism moderated the relationship between minority influence and improvement in decision quality such that in groups with minority influence, individuals higher, rather than lower, in horizontal collectivism were less likely to improve their decision quality; these results also held for those higher, rather than lower, in horizontal individualism; influence targets higher, rather than lower, in vertical collectivism also benefited more when the influence agent held a high status position in the group. None of the cultural values moderated the relationships between U.S.-based predictors of organizational commitment (e.g., role clarity, participative management, extrinsic rewards, and job scope) and commitment in foreign affiliates; individualism, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity had direct effects on organizational commitment but explained only 2.7% of the variance. U.S. tellers were significantly higher on individualism and lower on collectivism than the Hong Kong tellers; in a four-way interaction, job demands, job control, and job self-efficacy were associated with anxiety, depression, and turnover intention only for those high in IND (but not high in COL); results were replicated in the U.S. sample alone (sample size prevented replication in the Hong Kong sample). Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 52 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Thomas & Au (2002)ww Vandenberghe et al. (2001) Countries/ Sample 111 New Zealand and 107 Hong Kong executive education participants 580 employees of the European Commission representing 12 countries Method Variables and Measures Survey Horizontal Individualism; Vertical Collectivism (Singelis et al., 1995); exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect; job satisfaction; quality of job alternatives Survey Individualism-Collectivism, Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Masculinity-Femininity (all assessed using country scores from Hofstede, 1980); affective, normative, and continuance, commitment; intention to quit Theories/ Models None Meyer and Allen’s (1991)xx organizational commitment model Key Findings Horizontal individualism and vertical collectivism moderated the relationships between both quality of job alternatives and job satisfaction and exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect; horizontal individualists were more sensitive to quality of job alternatives when considering whether or not to exit; high levels of vertical collectivism enhanced the relationship between quality of alternatives and exit; quality of alternatives and voice were positively related at high, but not low, levels of horizontal individualism; in a three-way interaction, for employees high in vertical collectivism, job satisfaction was positively related to loyalty, but the relationship was stronger when quality of alternatives was high; for employees low in vertical collectivism, the opposite picture emerged. Country scores on the cultural values failed to moderate the relationships between three foci of commitment (i.e., affective, normative, and continuance) and intention to quit. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 53 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Individual Level of Analysis) Author(s) Wagner (1995) Countries/ Sample 492 undergraduates in the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (combination of items from Erez & Earley, 1987; Hui, 1988; Triandis et al., 1988; Wagner & Moch, 1986); group size; individuals’ identifiability; shared responsibility; cooperation Theories/ Models Social loafing phenomenon Key Findings Individualism-collectivism moderated the relationships between both size and identifiability and cooperation in small groups such that size and identifiability have stronger effects on the cooperation of individualists than collectivists. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 54 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Group/Organization Level of Analysis) Author(s) Chatman & Barsade (1995) Chatman et al. (1998) Countries/ Sample 139 MBA students in the U.S. 258 MBA students in the U.S. Method Variables and Measures Experiment IndividualisticCollectivistic organizational cultures (manipulated; manipulation check with study specific measure); individualistic vs. cooperative individuals; cooperative behavior Experiment IndividualisticCollectivistic organizational cultures (manipulated; manipulation check with study specific measure); relational demography; social interaction; conflict; productivity; creativity Theories/ Models Person-situation congruence Social categorization theory Key Findings Individualistic-collectivistic organizational culture moderated the relationship between personal cooperation and cooperative behavior such that cooperative subjects in collectivistic cultures demonstrated more cooperative behavior than individualistic subjects in the individualistic organizational culture, individualistic subjects in a collectivistic organizational culture, and individualistic subjects in an individualistic organizational culture. Individualistic-collectivistic organizational culture moderated the relationships between relational demography and social interaction, conflict, productivity, and creativity such that in collectivistic organizational cultures, demographic heterogeneity was positively related to communication; more conflict was found in individualistic versus collectivistic cultures but collectivists viewed conflict as more beneficial; as demographic similarity decreased, subjects in collectivistic cultures perceived themselves as more creative than those in individualistic cultures; similar people were more productive in individualistic rather than collectivistic cultures, while dissimilar people were equally productive across the two cultures. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 55 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Group/Organization Level of Analysis) Author(s) Earley (1999) Erez & Somech (1996) Countries/ Sample 288 managerial trainees from England, France, Thailand, and the U.S. in 96 groups 122 managers from Israeli kibbutzim and urban cities in 40 groups Method Variables and Measures Experiment Power Distance (Earley & Erez, 1997); group member status; individual estimates of group efficacy; collective estimates of group efficacy; team performance Experiment Individualism-Collectivism (independent and interdependent selves, McPartland et al., 1961)yy; group performance; group goals; group incentives Theories/ Models Social cognition; social legitimacy; social learning theory Social loafing and facilitation phenomena Key Findings Thai and French samples were significantly higher in power distance than English and U.S. samples; the Thai sample was significantly higher in power distance than the French sample, but the English and U.S. samples did not differ; power distance moderated the relationship between team members’ personal estimates of group efficacy and collective judgments of group efficacy such that in high power distance cultures, collective judgments of group efficacy were more strongly tied to higher, rather than lower, status group members’ personal judgments of group efficacy; in low power distance cultures, group members contributed comparably to group efficacy and performance judgments; there were mixed results for judgments of team performance. Kibbutzim managers were significantly higher in collectivism than urban managers; the kibbutzim vs. urban split moderated the relationship between both type of goal (individual vs. group) and type of incentive (individual vs. group) and group performance such that group performance loss was less likely in kibbutzim versus urban groups; the highest level of group performance occurred in the kibbutzim groups with a group goal and group incentives; there were no direct tests of individualism-collectivism. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 56 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Group/Organization Level of Analysis) Author(s) Gibson (1999) Lam et al. (2002a)zz Countries/ Sample 294 undergraduate students in the U.S. and Hong Kong in 60 groups and 185 nurses in the U.S. and Indonesia in 71 teams 265 Hong Kong and 288 U.S. bank employees of a multinational bank in 109 work units Method Variables and Measures Experiment and Survey Collectivism (Earley, 1993); group efficacy; group performance Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998); participative decision making opportunity; participation efficacy; group performance Theories/ Models Social cognition Self-efficacy; collective efficacy; shared mental models Key Findings Hong Kong students were significantly more collectivistic than U.S. students; Indonesian nurses were significantly more collectivistic than U.S. nurses; collectivism moderated the relationship between group efficacy and group performance such that group efficacy and performance were positively related when collectivism was high, but not when it was low. Individualism-collectivism moderated the relationship between participative decision making opportunity and group performance such that participative decision-making and group performance were positively related for groups high, but not low, in collectivism and high participation efficacy; individualism-collectivism mediated country effects. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 57 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Diener & Diener (1995)aaa Countries/ Sample Undergraduates in 31 countries Method Variables and Measures Archival and Survey Individualism-Collectivism (country ratings by Triandis, personal communication, 1992); friendship satisfaction; satisfaction with self; life satisfaction Individualism-Collectivism [(country ratings provided by Triandis, personal communication, 1996) and the factor scores obtained by Hofstede (1980)]; subjective well-being; life satisfaction; marital status Overall cultural distance (assessed by perceptual estimates of the cultural distance to China from the foreign country); overall and specific control; foreign parent and Chinese parent satisfaction with IJV Power Distance (metaanalysis); job level; job satisfaction Diener et al. (2000) 59,169 people in 42 countries (World Values Survey II) Archival Luo et al. (2001) 295 international joint ventures originating in the PRC Survey Robie et al. (1998) 35 empirical studies Metaanalysis Spector et al. (2002) 5,185 managers from 24 countries Survey Individualism-Collectivism (Hofstede, 1994); work locus of control; psychological and physical well-being; job satisfaction Theories/ Models Maslow’s hierarchy of needs Key Findings Individualism-collectivism moderated the relationship between both friendship satisfaction and satisfaction with self and life satisfaction such that the relationships were stronger in individualistic, rather than collectivistic, nations. none Individualism-collectivism moderated several relationships; in terms of life satisfaction, the benefit of marriage over cohabitation was greater in collectivist than in individualist nations; in terms of positive emotions, the benefit of being married over being divorced or separated was smaller in collectivist than in individualist nations. Transaction cost economics Cultural distance moderated the relationship between control and performance such that when the cultural distance from the host country was higher, rather than lower, there was a weaker positive relationship between control and performance for foreign parents but not for Chinese parents. none Power distance moderated the relationship between job level and job satisfaction such that the relationship was weaker in lower, rather than higher, power distance countries. Individualism-collectivism failed to moderate the relationship between locus of control and wellbeing. Various models of job design and stress Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 58 Appendix B - Type II Studies Summaries of Articles Reviewed – Culture as a Moderating Effect (Country Level of Analysis) Author(s) Steensma et al. (2000b)bbb Countries/ Sample 494 manufacturing firms in Australia, Indonesia, Norway, and Sweden Method Survey Theories/ Models Individualism-Collectivism, Transaction cost Power Distance, economics; Uncertainty Avoidance, resource Masculinity-Femininity (all dependency assessed using country theory scores from Hofstede, 1980); perceived technological uncertainty; use of technology alliances Variables and Measures Key Findings Uncertainty avoidance and masculinity moderated the relationship between perceived technological uncertainty and the use of technology alliances such that the relationship was stronger for firms in high, rather than low, uncertainty avoidance countries and for firms in less, rather than more, masculine countries; individualism-collectivism moderated the relationship between perceived technological uncertainty and the pursuit of equity ties such that the relationship was positive only for firms in individualistic (and not collectivistic) countries. a Ali, A. 1987. Scaling an Islamic work ethic. Journal of Social Psychology, 128: 575-583. b The word “none” in the Theories/Models column does not mean that the article did not include a theory section or review of the literature. We used the word “none” to indicate that the author(s) built hypotheses based on Hofstede’s cultural values alone. That is, no additional theory was used to explain the underlying linkages between the cultural values and outcomes. c Triandis, H.C. 1995. Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview. d Indicates a study that used country scores assigned down to the individual level of analysis. Such studies are highly vulnerable to cross-level contamination. e Wagner, J.A., & Moch, M.K. 1986. Individualism-collectivism: Concept and measure. Group & Organization Studies, 11: 280-303. f Bontempo, R. 1993. Translation fidelity of psychological states: An item response theory analysis of an individualism-collectivism scale. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 24: 149-166. g Hui, C.H. 1988. Measurement of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22: 17-36. h Perloe, S. I. 1967. Social Values Questionnaire. In J.P. Robinson and P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes: 576-585. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research. i Lewin, K. 1943. Defining the ‘field at any given time.’ Psychological Review, 50: 292-310. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 59 j Befu, H. 1990. Conflict and non-Weberian bureaucracy in Japan. In S. N. Eisenstadt & A. Ben-ari (Eds.), Japanese models of conflict resolution. New York: Kegan Paul International. Befu’s scale is a modified version of Hamaguchi’s (cited and translated into English by Befu, 1990) scale. Also, three items from Triandis et al (1986) were added to the Hamaguchi derivative. k Ilgen, D.R., Fisher, C.D., & Taylor, M.S. 1979. Consequences of individual feedback on behavior in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64: 349-371. l Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Tummala, P., Kurokawa, M., & Kato, K. S. (1991). Self-other similarity judgments depend on culture. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. m Spence, J.T., & Helmreich, R.L. 1978. Masculinity & femininity, their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press. n MOW – International Research Team. 1987. The meaning of work. London: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. o Scott, W. 1965. Values and orientation: A study of fraternities and sororities. Chicago: Rand McNally. p Maznevski, M.L., DiStefano, J., Gómez, C.B., Noorderhaven, N., & Wu, P. 2003. Cultural dimensions at the individual level of analysis: The cultural orientations framework. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 2: 275-296. q Hui, C.H. 1984. Individualism–collectivism: Theory, measurement and its relation to reward allocation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. r Thomas, K.W., & Kilmann, R.H. 1974. The Thomas-Kilmann mode instrument. New York: Xicom. s Fiske, A.P. 1992. The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99: 689-723. t Erez, M., & Earley, P.C. 1993. Culture, self-identity, and work. London: Oxford. u Tversky, A. 1977. Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84: 327-352. v Maznevski, M. L., DiStefano, J. J., Gomez, C. B., Nooderhaven, N. G., & Wu, P. 1997. Variations in cultural orientations within and among five countries. Paper presented at the Academy of International Business Annual Meeting, Monterrey, Mexico (note this paper eventually published as Maznevski et al., 2003). w Craig, D. & Tower, R.K. 1993. Marketing western trends to the new Europeans: A case study of how consumers build relationships with imported spirits brands and perceive western advertising. Paper presented to the seminar on ‘Marketing integration of east and west Europe: Evolution and transition’, Budapest, May. x Hui, C.H. & Triandis, H. C. 1986. Individualism–collectivism: A study of cross-cultural researchers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17: 225-248. y Triandis, H. C., Leung, K., Villareal, M. & Clack, F. 1985. Allocentric vs. idiocentric tendencies: Convergent and discriminant validation. Journal of Research in Personality, 19: 395-415. z Greenwald, A.G. 1982. Ego-task analysis: An integration of research on ego-involvement and self-awareness. In A. Hastorf & A.M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology: 109-147. New York: Elsevier North Holland. aa Kagan, A.R., & Levi, L. 1974. Health and environment – psychosocial stimuli. Social Scientific Medicine, 8: 225-241. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 60 bb Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. 1994. A collective fear of the collective: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20: 568-579. cc Rokeach, M. 1973. The nature of human values. New York: Free Press. dd Yamaguchi, S. 1994. Collectivism among the Japanese: A perspective from the self. In U. Kim, H.C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, methods and applications: 175-188. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ee Triandis, H.C., Bontempo, R., Betancourt, H., Bond, M., Leung, K., Brenes, A., Georgias, J., Hui, C.H., Marin, G., Setiadi, B., Sinha, J.B.P., Verma, J., Spangenberg, J., Touzard, H., & Montmollin, G. 1986. The measurement of the ethnic aspects of individualism and collectivism across cultures. Australian Journal of Psychology, 38: 257-267. ff Also found in the main effects, individual level section. gg Kilmann, R.H., & Saxton, M.J. 1983. Kilmann-Saxton culture-gap survey. Tuxedo, NY: Organizational Design Consultants Inc. hh Gudykunst, W.B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., & Nishida, T. 1996. The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on communication styles across cultures. Human Communication Research, 22: 510-543. ii Also found in the main effects, individual level section. jj Dunning, J. 1988. The eclectic paradigm of international production: A restatement and some possible extensions. Journal of International Business Studies, 19: 1-31. kk ll Ronen, S., & Shenkar, O. 1985. Clustering countries on attitudinal dimensions: A review and synthesis. Academy of Management Review, 10: 435-454. Prahalad, C.K., & Doz, Y.L. 1987. The multinational mission. New York: Free Press. mm Miles, R.E., & Snow, C.C. 1978. Organizational strategy, structure, and process. New York: McGraw-Hill. nn Koza, M.P., & Lewin, A.Y. 1998. The co-evolution of strategic alliances. Organization Science, 9: 255-264. oo Gray, S.J. 1988. Towards a theory of cultural influence on the development of accounting systems internationally. Abacus, 3: 1-15. pp Hofstede, G. 1994. Values Survey Module 1994 manual. Maastrict, The Netherlands: Institute for Research on Intercultural Cooperation. qq Triandis, H.C. 1994. INDCOL. Unpublished research scale on Individualism and Collectivism. University of Illinois, Champaign. rr Adams, J.S. 1976. The structure and dynamics of behavior in organization boundary roles. In M.D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology: 1175-1199. Chicago: Rand McNally. ss Bass, B.M., & Avolio, B.J. 1997. Full range of leadership development: Manual for the Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire. Redwood City, CA: Mindgarden. tt Triandis, H.C., & Gelfand, M.J. 1998. Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74: 118-128. Located at doi:10.1057/Palgrave.jibs.8400202 uu Triandis, H.C. 1989. The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts, Psychological Review, 96: 506-520. vv Karasek, R.A. 1979. Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24: 285-310. ww Also found in the main effect, individual level section. xx Meyer, J.P., & Allen, N.J. 1991. A three-component conceptualization of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review, 1: 61-89. yy McPartland, T.S., Cummings, J.H., & Garretson, W. 1961. Self-conception and ward behavior in two psychiatric hospitals. Sociometry, 24: 111-124. zz Also found in the moderating effect, individual level section. aaa Also found in the main effect, country level section. bbb Also found in the main effect, country level section. 61