P312Ch03B_TheRetina

advertisement
The Retina – the Neural System of the Eye – Chapter 3
G8 Chapter 3 Outline
Focusing Light Onto the Retina
Light: The Stimulus for Vision
The Eye
Light is Focused by the Eye
Transforming Light Into Electricity
How Does Transduction Occur
Hecht’s Psychophysical Experiment
The Physiology of Transduction
Pigments and Perception
Distribution of Rods and Cones
Dark Adaptation of the Rods and Cones
Measuring Cone Adaptation
Measuring Rod Adaptation
Visual Pigment Regeneration
Spectral Sensitivity of the Rods and Cones
Measuring Spectral Sensitivity
Rod and Cone Absorption Spectra
Neural Convergence and Perception
Why Rods Result in Greater Sensitivity Than Cones
Why We Use Our Cones to See Details
Lateral Inhibition and Perception
What the Horseshoe Crab Teaches Us About Inhibition
Lateral Inhibition and Lightness Perceptioin
The Hermann Grid: Seeing Spots at Intersections
Mach Bands: Seeing Borders More Sharply
Lateral Inhibition and Simultaneous Contrast
A Display That Can’t Be Explained by Lateral Inhibition
Something to Consider: Perception is Indirect
The coverage that follows does not exactly follow the text.
The Retina - 1
3/8/2016
Retina: A layer of cells including receptors and neurons on the back of the interior wall of the eye.
Neural cells making up the retina include: 1) Rod receptors, 2) cone receptors, 3) bipolar cells, 4) ganglion
cells, 5) amacrine cells, 6) horizontal cells.
Receptor cells
Horizontal cells
Bipolar cells
Amacrine cells
Ganglion cells
Light
The retina is constructed so that the receptors are farthest away from the light. The light entering the eye
strikes and passes through all the other cells and also the bodies of the receptors.
Light initiates activity when it strikes the receptor outer segments, the part of the retina furthest from the
light.
Why do the receptors face away? Nutrition.
It’s more efficient to supply the nutrient-carrying blood to the receptors from the back of the eye.
The Retina - 2
3/8/2016
The receptors
Two types – Rods and Cones
What the receptors have in common
1. They all transduce light into release of neurotransmitter substance.
In the absence of light, both rods and cones release glutamate, a neurotransmitter substance.
When light strikes them, they reduce the amount of glutamate released.
So, light is signaled by a reduction in release of neurotransmitter substance.
2. All receptors become more sensitive to light when placed in darkness and less sensitive when placed in
the light. The change in sensitivity occurs over several minutes.
3. They all synapse with bipolar cells.
Differences between rods and cones
1. Location: Cones are located in the center of the retina; rods are in the periphery.
2. Sensitivity: Cones are much less sensitive to light than are rods.
So, at night, we are better able to see faint lights out of the corner of our eyes.
Looking directly at a faint light in the night may make it disappear.
Rods quit working when light level is about that corresponding to twilight. They’re “max’d” out.
Cones require low sunlight or better to be activated.
This graph does not do
justice to the huge 1000:1
difference in sensitivity
between the cones and
rods.
Sensitivity
Cones
Rods
The Retina - 3
3/8/2016
3. Number: There are about 6 million cones, about 100 million rods per eye.
4. Types: There are 3 types of cones, only one type of rod.
5. Differential sensitivity to wavelength. G8 p56
Let’s name the cones, the S, for short wavelength, M, for medium wavelength, and L for long wavelength
cones.
The S receptors are actually sensitive to ultraviolet radiation, but lens absorbs UV radiation, so none of it
strikes the receptors.
6. Differential wiring: Differential connections to bipolar and ganglion cells.
Cones are more likely to have few-to-1 connections – few cones, perhaps only one, drives one bipolar
which drives one gangliion cell.
Rods are characterized by many-to-1 connections – 100s, sometimes 1000s of rodsx synapse with one
bipolar. Many of these bipolar cells synapse with 1 ganglion cell.
So:
Cones: few-to-1
Rods: many-to-1
The Retina - 4
3/8/2016
Questions to consider 1. Why two types of receptor? Why not just one type?
One outfielder cannot cover the whole outfield, so baseball has left fielders, center fielders, and right
fielders.
My guess is that the range of intensities to which we’re sensitive is too large to be covered by just one
receptor type. Two receptor types – one for night and one for day provides better coverage of the range of
lights in which we have to operate.
Range of intensities in which we exist is about 1 million to 1. That is, the brightest sunlight in which we
are able to use our eyes is about 1 million times more intense than the faintest starlight in which we can still
see shadows.
But the range of responses of receptors and the bipolar – ganglion cells to which they connect is only about
500-1000 to 1. This means that significant changes in intensity would be represented by very small changes
in response rate of the cells involved. This would likely result in many intensity changes going unnoticed.
So the two receptors “divide” up the intensity range, with the rods taking the faint lights and the cones
taking the bright lights.
2. Why 3 types of cone? Why not just one type?
The short answer is that having three types of cones makes it MUCH easier for us to identify different
wavelengths than if we had just one. Review the previous lecture on the simple circuit to detect short and
long wavelengths. If we had only one receptor type, the world would be only shades of gray.
The fact is that the more cone types you have, the better able you are to discriminate wavelengths – the
richer your color perception is.
See figures on next page. More on this in the color perception chapter.
3. Why don’t we have more than 3 types of cones? Wouldn’t our color vision be even better than it is
now?
The answer is yes. However it turns out that each type of cone requires many thousands of extra
neurons to process and integrate the information from that type. But the brain has other things to do than to
provide good color vision. What about music? Form? Touch? Arithmetic? Literature?
So the choice is to devote extra neurons to better color vision or devote them to processing auditory
information, or form, or touch, or something else. The bottom line is that our brain is a compromise. More
on this in the chapter on color
The Retina - 5
3/8/2016
Perceptual consequences of our having two types of receptors
1. Dark Adaptation G8 p52
When put in the dark, our sensitivity to light increases, then levels off after about 2 minutes, then
increases again for up to 30 minutes.
Measuring dark adaptation.
1) Expose the participant to light for > 5-10 minutes.
2) Put the participant in an absolutely lightless environment.
3) Wait a predetermined time, then quickly determine the threshold for detection of light.
Repeat over and over using different times in the dark.
Plot the threshold as a function of time in the dark.
Threshold – lower
means more sensitive
3 min
20-30 min
Time in dark before threshold measured
Difference in thresholds is about 1,000,000 to 1 between Time 0 and 30 minutes in the dark. Our visual
system is about 1 million times more sensitive after 30 minutes in the dark.
Early researchers thought the “bump” in the curve was an artifact. But they kept getting the bump.
Probably some research assistants were fired because their superiors thought they were careless.
We now know that the upper part of the curve is the dark adaptation curve of cones and that the lower part
of the curve is the dark adaptation curve of rods.
Why did pirates wear eye patches?
2. The Purkinje shift – another consequence of two receptor types. G8 p. 57
As it gets dark, blue-green objects become brighter relative to other objects than they are when it’s light.
When it’s dark outside, the rods contribute much to our vision. Because they’re most sensitive to light
that’s blue-green, those colors appear brighter relative to others when it’s dark. This shift in brightness is
called the Purkinje shift.
When it’s bright outside, the rods contribute little to vision, and because the cones, as a set, are not as
responsive to blue-green, blue-green objects don’t seem overly bright in high illumination.
Rods can’t see red, so emergency equipment that needs to be seen at night should not be red.
The best compromise is a bluish/green combination – visible by both rods and cones.
3. Different parts of the retina are used for different purposes.
Center for fine detail in lighted conditions – the wiring of the cones
Center for color perception – the 3 types of cones
Periphery for faint lights – the rods are more sensitive and wired for sensitivity.
The Retina - 6
3/8/2016
Other cells in the retina.
We’ll first follow the neural activity upwards toward the brain – along the receptor -> bipolar -> ganglion
pathway.
The Bipolar cells. G8 p59
So called because it’s difficult to tell which is the “receiving” end and which is
the “sending” end.
They transmit information from the receptors to the ganglion cells.
Two types:
One responds positively to increase in glutamate. Recall that glutamate is
released in abundance in the absence of light. Thus, this converts absence of
light into a positive response.
Others respond positively to decreases in glutamate. This converts presence
of light into a positive response
Why do they exist? Perhaps to provide an extra synapse between the receptors and
ganglion cells.??
The first synapse, the receptor –> bipolar synapse may allow one type of comparison, perhaps local. That
is, other cells beside the receptor also synapse with the bipolar cell, resulting in comparisons. This is called
lateral inhibition.
The second synapse, the bipolar –> ganglion synapse may allow a completely different type of comparison,
perhaps more global.
This is pretty much the way things are in the brain – the comparisons, decisions, “thinking”, of the brain is
done in the synapses.
The Retina - 7
3/8/2016
Ganglion cells
Only cells in retina that are clearly neurons.
About 1 million per eye.
Axons form the optic nerve. So EVERYTHING WE SEE from
an eye is carried to the rest of the brain over the 1 million axons of
the ganglion cells in that eye.
This is a 100:1 compression of information.
Ganglion cells receive neurotransmitters from bipolar and amacrine cells.
Most of them send their axons to the lateral geniculate nucleus.
Their axons are a few inches long.
Three general types of ganglion cell:
M for Magni, larger in size with large receptive fields.
P for Parvi, smaller with smaller receptive fields.
K for koniocellular, kind of like P cells, with B/Y antagonism.
The M ganglion cells apparently carry information about movement and about gross features of the visual
environment.
The P ganglion cells apparently carry information about wavelength and about fine details of the visual
environment.
K cells - not well understood right now.
The blind spot
The axons of the ganglion cells cross the surface of the retina and gather together at one place. There, they
go toward the back of the head, out of the eye. That spot, obviously, has no receptors. It is called the blind
spot. G8 p 51-52 with a demo of the blind spot on p. 52.
Five of the
approximately 1
million ganglion
cells
Blind
spot
The Retina - 8
3/8/2016
The Gossiping cells of the retina
Horizontal Cells
These cells receive input from receptors and send inhibition to neighboring bipolar cells.
Since the inhibition is sent laterally, it’s called lateral inhibition.
R
B
R
B
H
B
R
R
Lateral Inhibition
One likely function of the horizontal cells is to heighten the contrast of borders between light and dark.
The amacrine cells
These cells receive input from bipolar cells and send inhibition to neighboring ganglion cells.
G
G
A
Amacrine
B
High level
Lateral
Inhibition
B
Horizontal
H
R
R
One conjecture is that the Amacrine cells are involved in inhibition associated with wavelength, such as
illustrated in the previous lecture on use of wavelength information.
Amacrine cells may act as switches between rod and cone systems driving the same ganglion cell.
The Retina - 9
3/8/2016
Lateral Inhibition – inhibition that is propagated across the retina.
G8 p61
The transmission of inhibition laterally from one part of the retina to another adjacent area is called lateral
inhibition.
Lateral inhibition plays a very important part in the processing of the visual image in the retina.
It is apparently involved in the following aspects of that processing . . .
1) Antagonism between center and surround of ganglion cell receptive fields.
2) Heightening or sharpening contrasts between adjacent light and dark areas.
3) Illusions such as Mach bands and the Hermann grid.
4) Simultaneous contrast
5) The “creation” of colors from the responses of the three cone types.
The Retina - 10
3/8/2016
Phenomena explained (at least partially) by lateral inhibition
1) Mach Bands
Actual
Intensity
Perceived
Intensity
The Retina - 11
3/8/2016
Text Figure 3.37, p. 65
Light
Receptors
100
-10
100
-10
-10
100
-10
-10
20
-2
-10
20
-2
-2
Horizontal
cells
20
-2
-2
-2
-2
Bipolars
100-10-10
= 80
100-10-10
= 80
100-10-2
= 88
20-10-2
=8
The Retina - 12
20-2-2
= 16
20-2-2
= 16
3/8/2016
2) Hermann Grid
The Hermann Grid is illustrated in G8 p 62-64.
Below is an example of the figure.
Most persons see gray dots in the intersection of the lines in the periphery of their vision, although they do
NOT see the dots in the center of their visual field.
The explanation is based on the fact that receptive fields in the periphery are larger than those in the fovea.
Perception in the periphery: Receptive fields are larger than the streets and intersections.
Most of surround
Peripheral perception of the “intersection” is darker than
in the light so
the perception of the “street” because surrounds of the
ganglion cell
receptive fields of “intersection” ganglion cells are
response is “gray”
mostly in the light, inhibiting the responses of those
Fixation
ganglion cells.
Peripheral perception of the “street” is lighter than of the
“intersection” because surrounds of receptive fields of
those ganglion cells are mostly in the dark, resulting in
less inhibition.
point
Most of surround in
the dark so
ganglion cell
response is “light”
Perception in the fovea: Receptive fields are small, so the whole receptive field lies within the street
and perception remains the same regardless of location.
In the center of the visual field, the receptive fields are so small that the whole receptive field is in the light
regardless of whether you look at the intersection or the middle of the “street”.
The Retina - 13
3/8/2016
3) Simultaneous Contrast
(To demonstrate, grab one of the squares and hit the Backspace key. Then press CTRL+Z to undo.)
The circles are all EQUALLY intense. The explanation is that when the surround is bright, that brightness
inhibits the responses of ganglion cells perceiving the center, making the center seem darker.
The Retina - 14
3/8/2016
Sensitivity vs. Acuity G8 p58
Convergence ratio: The number of receptors “connecting” to a single ganglion cell.
Low convergence ratio: Just a few receptors “connect” to a ganglion cell.
High convergence ratio: Many receptors “connect” to a ganglion cell
Resolution: Ability to distinguish bands of dark or light.
High resolution/acuity: Ability to distinguish narrow bands
Low resolution/acuity: Only able to distinguish wide bands
The relationship of resolution to convergence ratio
High convergence ratios -> Low resolution/acuity
Response is the same regardless of where the dark bar is located.
Low convergence ratios -> High resolution/acuity
A
A
responds
B
A
The Retina - 15
B
B
responds
3/8/2016
Convergence ratio and sensitivity
Sensitivity: Ability to detect faint lights.
High sensitivity: Good ability to detect faint lights
Low sensitivity: Ability to detect only bright lights
High convergence ratios -> High sensitivity
Responds to faint light
Low convergence ratios -> Low sensitivity
A
B
Neither responds
The Retina - 16
3/8/2016
Obtaining Good acuity and high sensitivity in the same overall system.
How does the visual system handle the dual demands of acuity and sensitivity?
To have good acuity, we need small receptive fields, with 1:1 connections between receptors and ganglion
cells.
To have high sensitivity, we need large receptive fields with many:1 connections between receptors and
ganglion cells.
Apparently, a two-pronged solution was required.. . .
1. Receptors with different individual sensitivities.
2. Subsystems with different convergence ratios.
These are combined to form two separate systems.
The scotopic system: Rods with high individual sensitivity and high convergence ratios.
The photopic system: Cones with lower individual sensitivity and low convergence ratios.
The retina could have been all cones connected to ganglion cells with different convergence ratios.
But cones are not individually sensitive enough.
The retina could have been rods and cones, with equal convergence ratios
But increasing convergence is needed to maximize sensitivity.
The duplex solution in the eye is a wonderful compromise.
The Retina - 17
3/8/2016
Possible test question concerning dark adaptation.
You take your nephew or niece to a movie. When you enter the theater, you can barely
even see the seats. Eventually you are able to see much better than just after you’d entered.
Your nephew or niece asks you, “Why can I see so much better now than when we just came
into the theater?”
What are the reasons that after 30 minutes in darkness you are so much more sensitive to
light than when you just came into the theater.
1. Individual cones have become more sensitive if they’re in the dark for a period of time –
due to changes in the molecules within the cones.
2. The visual system begins using output from rods, each of which is individually more
sensitive to light than cones.
3. The visual system beings using output from rods, which are wired for spatial summation,
increasing sensitivity.
4. Individual rods have become more sensitive after being in the dark for a period of time –
their internal chemicals – retinal and opsin – have recombined.
The Retina - 18
3/8/2016
Review of issues associated with the neural apparatus of the eye
Issues associated with receptor placement – facing forward vs. facing backward
Issue of number of receptor types – one vs. two or more
Issue of differences in receptor types – difference in sensitivity, difference in preference for wavelength
Issues of number of cells in the eye – receptors vs. receptor + ganglion vs. receptor + bipolar + ganglion.
Issues of how ganglion cell receptive fields should be organized.
Issues of convergence ratio and acuity vs. sensitivity.
The Retina - 19
3/8/2016
Download