Theoretical/Experimental Linguistic Cognition Advanced Studies Seeking the origins of language, students learn to blend an American flair for theory with French experimental design University of California, Los Angeles, and École Normale Supérieure 2009 laureate “Taking advantage of each other’s comparative advantages” is how Professor Dominique Sportiche of the University of California, Los Angeles linguistics department describes this joint study/research collaboration. Historically, Americans and the French have brought different strengths to the study of language. American linguists excel particularly at the theoretical side while the French possess a special expertise in experimental work, the testing and measuring of hypotheses. The comparative advantages don’t end there however. Linguistics is the scientific study of how language works. Humans are unique in the animal kingdom in possessing elaborate ways of expressing subtle or complex thoughts about the past or the future, about imaginary worlds, about other people’s feelings, intentions, or thoughts – a whole host of expressive possibilities. Everyone speaking a native tongue, any native tongue, has in his or her head a set of rules for building sentences and communicating in that language. Grammar refers to these rules governing the use of a language for common understanding among the members of a group. Much of this knowledge is acquired in infancy and early childhood, well before the onset of any formal, conscious effort to acquire these skills. Linguists look particularly at grammatical constructions in native tongues of the world, be they spoken or signed. For example, they study syntax to determine how sentences are formed, can be formed or could not be formed. How does agreement between subjects and verbs work? Why do certain languages have agreement between objects and verbs but English does not? How do different word placements affect intended meaning? Linguistic researchers are always looking for both the generalities and varieties within particular languages and among all languages. Such analysis has fascinated mankind for centuries. Examples of early linguistic activity can be traced back to India in the 4th Century B.C. and the work of Panini, a Sanskrit scholar, who developed a comprehensive grammar describing the many different features and rules of the language. But it was not until the mid-20th Century, due in great part to the work of Noam Chomsky at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, that it was understood that a lot of language structure is coded in how the human brain is wired and is not just a function of culture and society. This new emphasis changed the study of linguistics forever. Bring in the neuroscientists, experimental psychologists, education specialists, biostatisticians, and even doctors, ethologists and philosophers, and you have a team of cognitive scientists focused on how the brain perceives stimuli or inputs and proceeds to process them into outputs such as language. A cognitive scientist might ask: How does the brain process grammatical rules of which people are seemingly unconscious? How is subject-verb agreement actually achieved in speech? How many memory resources does it use? How are linguistic inputs analyzed to give rise to meanings so quickly and accurately? What exactly goes wrong in people experiencing language deficits? And what is different between the brains of people and animals? The advantages of adding the multidisciplinary approach of cognitive studies to linguistic exploration are many. There is a constant and productive interplay between formulating theoretical hypotheses about linguistic structures and studying how these structures are used in real time as we speak or understand or how they are acquired and used by babies and small children. New techniques of brain imaging allows one to pinpoint which parts of the brain are activated during speech, or which ones appear damaged in some patients, and with what linguistic structures these patients are struggling. The development of different levels of inquiry, of new research tools, systems of measurement and types of experimentation are bridging the humanities and the experimental sciences and bringing about substantial breakthroughs. The last 10 years, according to Sportiche, have been “transformational.” Today’s UCLA/ENS graduate students are among the first cohorts to be crosstrained in both the development of language theory and ways to test those hypotheses through experimental design, execution and measurement. The extent to which this is being done is a relatively new phenomenon in the field of linguistics, and Sportiche emphasizes the importance of having two first-rate institutions committed to building this collaborative approach. As he explains, just as languages have a lot of commonalities and differences, so do scientific cultures. It is absolutely critical that students, and especially their teachers and mentors, engage in new ways of thinking and appreciate the value in different approaches. Sportiche says it is hard work. “We’re not just trying to change ideas. We’re trying to change people,” he explains. “This is how progress is made.” American Team Leader: Professor Dominique Sportiche, Dominique.Sportiche@ucla.edu, (310) 825-0634 French Team Leader: Emmanuel Dupoux, Senior Research Scientist, Emmanuel.Dupoux@ens.fr, 011 33 (0)1 44 32 26 17