ANALYZING EFFECTS OF FILM TRAILERS’ RHETORICAL STRUCTURES ON COMPREHENSION AND AUDIENCE REACTIONS ____________ A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Chico ____________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Interdisciplinary Studies International Cognitive Visualization ____________ by © Christina S. Bwana Fall 2014 ANALYZING EFFECTS OF FILM TRAILERS’ RHETORICAL STRUCTURES ON COMPREHENSION AND AUDIENCE REACTIONS A Thesis by Christina Bwana Fall 2014 APPROVED BY THE DEAN OF GRADUATE STUDIES AND VICE PROVOST FOR RESEARCH: ________________________________ Eun K. Park, Ph.D. APPROVED BY THE GRADUATE ADVISORY COMMITTEE: ________________________________ Neil H. Schwartz, Ph.D. Graduate Coordinator ________________________________ Neil H. Schwartz, Ph.D., Chair ________________________________ Erica de Vries, Ph.D. ___________________________________________ Wolfgang Schnotz, Ph.D. PUBLICATION RIGHTS No portion of this thesis may be reprinted or reproduced in any manner unacceptable to the usual copyright restrictions without the written permission of the author. iii DEDICATION I would like to dedicate this thesis to Kody, Kory, Mark and Mark, true artists, true vision and true professionals. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my most heartfelt gratitude to Dr. Neil H. Schwartz, research advisor, mentor and fellow avid movie watcher, who trained me in the ropes in research, who readily agreed when I asked him not to let me provide anything but an excellent piece of work, and whose methods of imparting his wide knowledge willingly after making me go out and research what I could on my own built a strong independence and confidence in a once novel researcher like myself. I also extend this heartfelt gratitude to Dr. Wolfgang Schnotz and Dr. Erica de Vries for their individual and unique inputs and encouragement that helped me form a stronger research project! A special thank you to Dr. Lawrence Herringer and Dr. David Philawoer. Dr. Herringer, who agreed to take time out of a well deserved holiday in order to sit with me, teach me and run analysis with me. Dr. Philaower, who made statistics not only interesting, but entertaining. The methods and analyses in this investigation would just not have been as efficient and thorough were it not for the both of you! An enormous thank you to Tania Leeder and the rest of the LCIRG team, without whom I would have most likely looked like a headless chicken, running around trying to sort out a million different things at once. Thank you for the late nights spent setting up, taking down then re-setting up computer labs and software, and just every other way you generously offered to help! To my family. I always felt your warm wishes and your kind words of empathy, even though far away! To all my friends, your prayers and support in countless different ways leave me in awe! v TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE Publication Right ……………………………………………………………………. iii Dedication ………………………………………………………………………….. iv Acknowledgements …………………………………...…………………………….. v List of Tables ……………………………………………………………………….. viii List of Figures ............................................................................................................. ix Abstract ....................................................................................................................... x CHAPTER I. II. III. IV. Introduction ................................................................................................ 1 Background .................................................................................... The Present Investigation ............................................................... 1 2 Literature Review ....................................................................................... 11 Introduction .................................................................................... Defining Rhetorical Structure in Prose .......................................... Importance of Rhetorical Structures for Comprehension .............. Choosing a Rhetorical Structure in Prose ...................................... Investigating Rhetorical Structure in Film ..................................... 11 13 15 21 26 Methodology ............................................................................................... 29 Design and Participants ................................................................ Experimental Materials ................................................................ Procedure ..................................................................................... 29 29 40 Results ....................................................................................................... 42 Hypotheses .................................................................................. Rhetorical Structures and Comprehension .................................. Rhetorical Structures and Reactions ............................................ Path Analysis ............................................................................... 42 43 44 48 vi V. Discussion ........................................................................................................ 52 Overview ....................................................................................... Narrative Structure and Comprehension ....................................... Narrative Structure and Reactions ................................................ Path Model .................................................................................... Limitations and Implications ........................................................ 52 53 56 58 59 References .................................................................................................................. 62 vii LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1. Table 1 ............................................................................................................... 45 2. Table 2 .............................................................................................................. 49 3. Table 3 .............................................................................................................. 50 viii LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 1. Figure 1 ............................................................................................................. 31 2. Figure 2 ............................................................................................................. 46 3. Figure 3 ............................................................................................................. 46 4. Figure 4 ............................................................................................................. 47 5. Figure 5 ............................................................................................................. 51 ix ABSTRACT ANALYZING EFFECTS OF FILM TRAILERS’ RHETORICAL STRUCTURES ON COMPREHENSION AND AUDIENCE REACTIONS by © Christina Bwana Master of Arts in Interdisciplinary Studies International Cognitive Visualization California State University, Chico Fall 2014 Research has shown that the comprehension of prose transcends modalities; that is, text and film as forms of prose are equally understood. The factors involved in comprehension of both text and film include the situation models of an individual, and the rhetorical structure used by the author of the prose to convey his or her intended meaning. This study was designed to examine the influence of rhetorical structures of film trailers on an audience, measured by i) the audience comprehension of the plot and theme within a trailer, and ii) the audience reaction towards the trailer, measured by affinity towards the trailer, viewing desire, viewing intention and intention to share the trailer. There were four experimental conditions, which were the four rhetorical structures of a single trailer: narrative, descriptive, original and control. Instruments to measure comprehension and reaction were created specifically for this experiment, and analysed x using multivariate analysis of covariance. Results showed the narrative, descriptive and control structures, all of which were segmented into comprehensible parts within a whole trailer, had equal influence on comprehension of plot elements. The narrative structure had a significantly greater influence on comprehension of theme, and on affinity towards the trailer, as compared to the other three structures. A film audience would be more likely to understand the plots of a topic within a trailer if the trailer was segmented into comprehensible parts, regardless of structure. A film audience would also be more likely to comprehend the themes or higher order ideas of a trailer as intended by the author, if the ideas at the top level of structure are presented in a linear, clear manner. Finally, the coherent, linear structure of a film trailer is more aesthetically more pleasing to a film audience. xi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background Research done in film marketing offers a vast amount of literature on the dynamic role of individual differences—such as affect, feelings and emotions—in consumer behavior—that is, the movie going experience. The foci of research done in film marketing tend to be on which affect of the audience and which surface elements of the film—such as actors, visual effects, references to popular culture—predict enjoyment of the movie, and how the relationships between all these factors influence the popularity of some genres over others (see for example Hixson, 2005; Kernan, 2004; Marich, 2005). However, the existing literature does not provide any feedback about the interaction between a film, the audience comprehension of contents of the film, and the audience reaction towards the film thereafter. Attempts led by Eliashberg and Sawhney (1994) have proposed models of the dynamic interactions between temporary emotional moods, stable individual personality factors, and all the emotional contents of a movie. However, existing models ignore the additional interactions of attention, cognition and comprehension on the topic content, as well as the emotional content which, when combined, make up the movie experience (Tan, 2013). There is still a wide dearth of research on the relationship between the semantic contents of a film or what the author wants to convey and an audience’s comprehension thereof. These semantic contents of a film are organized through a 1 rhetorical structure—a rhetorical structure is defined as the spatial organization of ideas within a prose. There is also a lack of research on the relationship between the rhetorical structures of films that affectively convey their semantic content, and specific audience reactions to these films. The lack of research on film trailers overall, and the dependence on mere instinct and experience to create movie trailers, has been heavily criticized (Hixson, 2005). Movie trailers are one of the most important sources of information that influence decisions on cinema attendance and predict performance of their related motion pictures in theatres (Eliashberg & Shugan, 1997; Kernan, 2004; Marich, 2005). The focus of research should therefore shift onto a relationship between structural characteristics of film trailers and individual differences and reactions in audiences (Tan, 2013). The Present Investigation This study was designed to examine the influence of rhetorical structures of film trailers on an audience, measured by i) the audience comprehension of the plot and theme within the trailer, as the intended meanings of the author, and ii) the audience reactions towards the trailer. Comprehension is measured using plot and theme. We define plot as “the sequence of individual events that progress a story from beginning to end” and theme as “the underlying ideas that go beyond the action of the plot, revealing the intent of the author”. Reactions towards the trailer are defined as: (a) affinity towards the trailer, (b) desire to watch the movie after viewing the trailer, (c) intention to watch the movie, and (d) intention to share the trailer. The basic research questions in this investigation were aimed at recognizing the advantages some rhetorical structures may have over 2 others in accurately representing the semantic content of a film trailer and in eliciting positive audience reactions from it. The lack of sufficient literature on film trailers lends to our borrowing of research from text comprehension and film marketing. Rhetorical structure and comprehension Chatman (1990) noted two inherent similarities of film and books: one being their temporal control over their audience—that is to say, if one skips a scene or a chapter, one has to somehow infer what has happened in the interim; the other being their use of text structure, be it in written or spoken form (Grimes, 1975). The importance of structure in any form of text-based work, including books and films, is its ability to convey and identify ideas and meaning within the content in a way that is logical and organized (Meyer, 1999). By having a discernable structure, both the role relationships (content words relating together) and rhetorical relationships (superordinate ideas relating together) within the content of a text can be analyzed and comprehended by the reader, as intended by the author (Grimes, 1975; Meyer, 1975). Authors use structure to convey meaning, and readers of the text use this structure to interpret the author’s intended meaning. This comprehension is effectively known as shared meaning (Meyer, 1999). However, comprehension is most often dependent on the interaction between several factors, such as characteristics of the text and characteristics of the reader (Grimes, 1975; Meyer, Brandt & Bluth, 1980; Meyer, 1999; Meyer & Poon, 2001). Although authors choose their structure and how to organize their ideas with audience characteristics in mind, they cannot always accurately predict how their audience will 3 interpret meaning, due to the vast individual differences a reader brings into the reading experience, such as learning task, background knowledge, emotional state, etc (Grimes, 1975; Meyer, 1999; Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998). Therefore, it is important, if not always probable, to choose a readily identifiable structure and to ensure that both experienced and novice readers (or in this case, an avid film watcher versus one who rarely watches films) will be able to identify and analyze the structure in order to comprehend the intended meanings (Meyer, 1975). Once a piece of work is released, any feedback on shared meaning can only, at best, be used as a guideline for the next piece of work, but cannot be used in retrospect to change the existing work (Meyer, 1999). If individuals desire or are averse to a film they imagine after just viewing a trailer of it (Kernan, 2004), then it is important to establish a coherent structure that can accurately represent, already in the context of the trailer, the plot and themes of the whole film, as intended by the author. Establishing the general importance of having a coherent and attainable rhetorical structure in any text-based work is simple. The difficulty lies in the mapping between the semantic levels—what the author wants to say—and the rhetorical levels—how to best say it (Mann & Thompson, 1988). Researchers insist that the choices of rhetorical structures are countless enough to require linguistically motivated guidelines in order to choose a working structure (Kosseim & Lapalme, 2000; Mann & Thompson, 1988). However, there seems to be no universal consensus on how to name and describe each structure—to what degree of specification without overlapping into another structure— and which structure is best for a particular topic (Grimes, 1975; Mann & Thompson, 1988). 4 We can therefore safely assume two things for a trailer. First, some rhetorical structures can overlap and co-occur without compromising the meaning of an author’s message in a story (Chatman, 1990; Grimes, 1975; Mann & Thompson, 1988). However, the main constraints would then still be conceptual, semantic and rhetorical in nature: what does the audience already know about a topic, what does the author really want to say, and how best can the author convey everything he or she wants to say in the best possible structure or combination of structures. Furthermore, we can assume that there are no fixed rules on which structure to use for a specific topic, but choosing which structure to use is more a matter of pragmatic heuristics (Kosseim & Lapalme, 2000): first, if there is an even more direct relationship from structure to meaning that is not explained by the author’s structure choice, and second, how the audience might imagine this topic and interpret the author’s structure, different to their own structure that they have previously applied for the target topic. The choice of rhetorical structure is thus critical in an analysis of audience comprehension of film trailers. Comprehension and recall have been found to be a function of the rhetorical structure, independent of the topic (Meyer, 1974; Meyer et al., 1980; Thorndyke, 1977). The same content structure may produce similar recall, regardless of topic, but the inherent nature of some content structures produce superior recall than others (Graesser, McNamara & Louwerse, 2003; Graesser, Singer & Trabasso, 1994; Lehr, 1988; Meyer, 1975; Thorndyke, 1977). The narrative structure—or the direct story telling of a sequence of events, for example—is temporal in nature, as is the plot of a story; therefore, the author would be able to more easily structure plot, and audience members more easily determine plot, through a narrative rhetorical structure (Bordwell, 5 2013; Chatman, 1990; Grimes, 1975). Additionally, because of its simplicity, narrative structure is already used for a majority of films (Bordwell, 2013; Chatman, 1990, Tan, 2013). Reasoning from the work in text structure (Cowen, 1988; Graesser et al., 1994; Kintsch, 1988; Meyer, 1975), it makes sense that placing plot at the rhetorical level of relations would make it easier for an audience to comprehend with less judgment and reasoning, regardless of the audience’s background knowledge of the topic. If this is the case, accumulation of plot elements can lead to a higher-level thought or “theme” (Black & Wilensky, 1979; Thorndyke, 1977), making for a direct pragmatic relationship between plot and theme. Thus, the narrative trailer structure would be most effective for the occurrence of shared meaning or comprehension of the plot and themes between author and viewer. Specifically, the audience viewing a film trailer with a narrative structure would have higher scores on comprehension of the plot elements and plot sequence, as well as a closer comprehension of theme between the author and audience. The descriptive structure—or the verbal characterization through the use of significant details—is more efficient in rendering the properties of things both visible and only imaginable to the senses (Chatman, 1990; Grimes, 1975). Additionally, the audience of a descriptive structure has the goal of being informed—acquiring new, more in-depth knowledge of a specific topic (Graesser et al., 1994). Therefore, we reasoned that a descriptive rhetorical structure would be a strong structure for informing readers of a topic, leading to a possible understanding of the topic’s theme—which is never directly represented by narrations and requires more judgment (Kosseim & Lapalme, 2000). Therefore, we assumed that a descriptive structure would be comparable to a narrative structure in its ability to convey the authors’ intended theme. To be clear, the descriptive 6 structure might not be the most pragmatic in conveying plot and theme, but an audience viewing a trailer with a descriptive structure could, with more cognitive effort, comprehend especially the theme the author intended to convey. Rhetorical structure and audience reactions The purpose of any form of media—books and films alike—can be summarized into one, or a combination of, four basic goals: to inform, to entertain, to persuade, and/or to provide an aesthetic experience (Brewer, 1980). In order to inform, comprehension or shared meaning must be present (Meyer, 1999). We have already established the possible advantage of the narrative rhetorical structures for comprehension. In order to entertain, the film must provide an optimum emotional experience—emotions elicited by the film should not be low, but should also not be so high as to forget that the viewing experience is actually fictional (Tan, 2013). It is an interaction of this emotional experience provided by the film, and comprehension of the film, that can predict audience reactions to the film (Bower, 1992; Brewer & Lichtenstein, 1981; Iran-Nejad, 1987; Phillips, 2000; Schiefele, 1991; Tan, 2013). Topic interest, personality and text-based emotional states (aroused by specific text features) have been found to relate to learning from text (Schiefele, 1991). These text-based emotional states can be interpreted as the entertainment experience provided by the film (Tan, 2013). The emotional states of a reader elicited by the text can affect interpretation of the meaning of a story for several reasons. Emotions positively influence comprehension by increasing attention to preceding or accompanying events that have similarity to events responsible for the current emotional arousal; these emotions also 7 increase attention to the topic information that learners judge to be significant to existing expectations (Bower, 1992; Frijda, 1987). Because emotional arousal has a slow decay, it hosts continuous rehearsal of information (Bower, 1992). An optimum emotional experience affects or mediates the relationship between comprehension and affinity. According to Zillmann and Bryant (1985), affinity is highest when the arousal is optimum—not too high and not too low. Comprehension resulting from resolved intellectual activity, as a result of optimum arousal and a conclusive narration, can increase both affinity and interest towards the text—affinity and interest both being a possible audience reaction (Iran-Nejad, 1987; Phillips, 2000). This is supported by Tan’s (2013) empirical research on film narratives and emotions, stating that film narratives that require more cognitive involvement and stimulation, but end with a comprehensible solution, are favored among film viewers. Additionally, according to Brewer and Lichtenstein’s (1981) “structural-affect theory”, narratives must stimulate surprise, curiosity and suspense (as the entertainment experience) and have a conclusion, and it is these dimensions that strongly correlate to an audience liking a story. Comprehension can also influence emotions, and audience reactions. Kernan (2004) claims that “an original, innovative and comprehensive” film trailer has the ability to release emotions and motivate cinema attendance. This was supported by Hixson (2005), who found that knowledge of a movies’ plot—through the trailer or other advertising means—is a significant predictor of cinema attendance. Kernan (2004) and Hixson (2005) thus both support associations between comprehension, emotions, and audience reactions. 8 Tan (2013) stresses that no model exists to describe the psychological effects of the structural functions of a feature film. Therefore, we cannot confidently state a direct relationship between structures of film—rhetorical or surface—and influence on audience reactions. We can, however, assume a relationship between rhetorical structures and audience reactions that is mediated by comprehension of a film and the emotional experience provided by a film. First, the narrative structure of film is hypothesized to be the most pragmatic for shared meaning—or comprehension of plot and theme. This is due to the manipulation of fictional situations and aspects of those situations in a temporal, direct manner (Tan, 2013). Second, we have discussed the relationships between comprehension and audience reactions, mediated by an optimum emotional experience. The narrative structure fulfills the requirements for the creation, maintenance and modulation of emotions, and allows for a conclusive ending, as advocated by the structure-affect theory (Brewer & Lichtenstein, 1981; Tan, 2013). Additionally, just like the audience of a descriptive structure has the goal of being informed, the audience of a narrative structure has the goal of being entertained (Graesser et al., 1994; Tan, 2013). We therefore assume that the narrative structure will have the best influence on audience reactions. Specifically, an audience watching a film trailer with a narrative structure will be more likely to have a stronger affinity to the trailer, desire to watch the film, intend to watch the film, and intend to share the film’s trailer. An individual’s pre-conceived views of a topic or of the world, as well as the personality type of the individual, can influence to what extent the individual comprehends a topic, and how they receive the topic, in terms of affinity (van Dijk & 9 Kintsch, 1983; Mischel, 2013; Schommer, 1990; Wittrock, 1989). The focus of the present investigation was on the influence of rhetorical structure on comprehension and audience reaction, independent of the different pre-conceived views and personalities of participants. Pre-conceived topic or worldviews can vary significantly from one individual to the next, making it more difficult to account for their influence on an individuals comprehension and reaction. On the other hand, personality traits are stable and inherent (Mischel, 2013). That is to say, different individuals can have the same personality type, making it easier to predict these individuals’ assessments of, and reactions to, any situation (Mischel, 2013). Therefore, we decided to investigate two hypotheses—namely, that narrative structure would lead to a) higher comprehension of plot and theme, and b) stronger audience affinity, viewing and sharing intentions—while only controlling for pre-conceived views of the audience. 10 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW Introduction There has been a strong interest in not only text comprehension, but also comprehension of films and montage—that is, a series of shots involving camera techniques and editing manipulations (Branigan, 2013; Smith, Anderson & Fischer, 1985). Early research has claimed—but never fully ascertained—a number of disadvantages in using visual media for comprehension, such as the quick change of scenes, camera zooms and pans, and symbolic representations in the videos, etc. (cf. Smith et al, 1985). Such research claims that an understanding of film montages, and television programs, appear only at the age of eight, with all the research on children younger than eight showing the children’s lack of comprehension of story sequence in a film. However, several studies have been able to challenge this general assumption. A study done by Smith, Anderson and Fischer (1985) on 71 pre-school children between the ages of three and five asked the children to reconstruct the events of a film, using dolls and other props provided to them. The film shown to the children contained various camera techniques and film editing. Results implied that children could nevertheless comprehend film and television programs, despite these camera techniques and film editing. However, this study used film montages that were shorter then what would be expected in a normal film or television program, therefore not entirely generalizable to 11 longer films and television programs. Despite this methodological limitation, their research did show the importance of modifying measurement techniques to capture, a) the language ability of the age group and b) the medium being measured, in their case, film. Language ability and task medium had not been taken into consideration in previous research on comprehension of short films or montage. Other studies involved testing film comprehension of older participants, with a more advanced language vocabulary, and a different measurement technique—recall protocols. Bagget’s (1979) study on university students compared the recall protocol of students who watched a short film or heard a verbal narration of the same topic. The two conditions were an exact match, in terms of episodic structure. The only differences were due to content aspects; whereas the verbal narration could explicitly state everything, some story content within the film condition had to be inferred. Students had to note what they recalled and this was compared to a recall protocol that had been normed for measuring recall in any medium. Students in both conditions produced structurally similar recall—that is, the spoken narration had no advantage over the visual narration. Similarly, Gernsbacher, Varner and Faust (1990) tested 270 college students on their comprehension of modified children’s stories in three different modalities: written, auditory and visual. Comprehension was measured using a recall protocol. The performance on these modalities correlated substantially, with correlations between visual and auditory, written and picture, and written and auditory being .72, .83, and .92, respectively. The only difference was found between students with low and high prior knowledge of the story content. 12 In essence, research in film comprehension versus text comprehension shows that a reader’s construction of mental representations of a given topic transcends the modality or medium used to convey this topic (Branigan, 2013; Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998). Therefore, there must be other factors—of the text and of the individual—guiding the comprehension of reader of a prose, which is independent of the modality used to convey the prose. Some of these factors influencing comprehension, as well as their importance in evaluating comprehension, are discussed in the following sections. Defining Rhetorical Structure in Prose A first step in being able to understand comprehension is to study the strategies used by the author of this prose to influence comprehension—these strategies being some of the factors of a prose. The author must have strategies in place to guide the reader to a correct interpretation of the deeper level meaning of the topic; that is, the higher level ideas (Meyer, 1975). Common sense or mere instinct to create text—or prose—can only get one so far; rather, a scientific framework is needed to provide insight into comprehension processes, and possibly even to predict comprehension (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). An example of such strategies that an author can use to assist reader’s correct comprehension of the topic, as the author intended, is to have a rhetorical structure in the prose. Defining rhetorical structure The focus of this paper is on rhetorical structure of a prose, according to Meyer (1975). The reason for this choice is that, different researchers had proposed different ways of analyzing prose in terms of structure (cf. Meyer, 1975), but only Meyer’s 13 analysis of structural organization could be used for larger texts and was comparable between different types of text; that is, not exclusive to one type of text, such as an instruction text or a psychology text. Therefore, according to Meyer (1975), a rhetorical structure looks at the different parts or proposition networks of a prose, and the principles of combining these parts into one entire, coherent text (Kintsch & Yarbrough, 1982; Mann, Matthiessen & Thompson, 1992). Certain general factors are needed for understanding specific elements in sentences, but these general factors are not found in the sentences themselves (Grimes, 1975). That is, an idea in a later segment of the prose can clarify or be contingent on another idea in an earlier segment of the prose; therefore, a general comprehension of both ideas and the relationship between them is needed for providing a deeper understanding of both individual sentence. A rhetorical structure—or discourse structure—shows how these ideas within a prose are spatially organized and related (Grimes, 195; Meyer, 1975). The ideas within a prose are represented by different content words, such as lexical predicates and arguments. Lexical predicates are the content words that can have a relationship with other content words. Arguments can be either; a) single content words that have a unique relationship to only one lexical predicate, or b) a proposition of connected content words. Each lexical predicate and argument is grouped into a different node. These nodes are all connected by lines, showing the overall organization of the content within the prose. These lines are labeled to classify the relationship among the content, and can be labeled as a role relation or a rhetorical relation. A role relation—or lexical proposition—specifies the relationship between lexical predicates and single 14 arguments. Lexical predicates and their arguments can be related in several ways, such as patient and agent roles. Therefore, role relations are always dominated by a lexical predicate, and can be found at all levels throughout the structure. On the other hand, a rhetorical relation specifies the relationship between superordinate ideas; that is, rhetorical propositions. In this case, both the predicate and the argument are rhetorical or high level ideas, already connected in propositional networks. Therefore, rhetorical relations are most often found at the top level of the structure. The spatial organization of ideas within a prose into lexical and rhetorical propositions results in a hierarchical structure containing subordinate and superordinate ideas. Because rhetorical relations clarify how two or more superordinate ideas relate together, they can be used to relate whole paragraphs and chapters together. The nature of these rhetorical relations within a prose is then responsible for defining the overall organization or structure of the prose. Importance of Rhetorical Structures for Comprehension The use of a proper, coherent rhetorical structure—the effective spatial organization of a prose—is therefore necessary for a deeper comprehension of a topic, and for evaluating this comprehension to interpret the effectiveness of the prose (Meyer, 1975). Rhetorical structure in comprehension Comprehension of syntax and other surface factors of a text are important for creating a surface representation of the text in a reader’s mind (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). However, previous research on comprehension at the syntax and surface level 15 proved that this focus on surface was too limiting to judge if text meaning has occurred (Marshall & Glock, 1978). In fact, Sachs argued that people could not differentiate between sentences with slight word changes, but that people remembered the “meaning” of a text more so than the actual words and sentences (as cited in Marshall & Glock, 1978, p. 12). This “meaning” or semantic text comprehension is important for comprehension of a topic at a deeper, semantic level; which is then necessary for measuring judgments of the topic of the text beyond text meaning, judgments of affinity towards the text and topic, reproductions of stories, and other reactions (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). The rhetorical structure of a prose guides a reader to a deeper, semantic comprehension of a text, and topic of the text (Fillmore, 1977; Marshall & Glock, 1978; Meyer, 1999). This is based on the three basic assumptions of Meyer’s (1975) rhetorical structure. The first assumption is that, the information located at the top level of a hierarchical organization of a prose are the main ideas of an author of that prose, and leads to higher recall and comprehension, as well as being retained for longer. Comprehending these superordinate ideas are what allow for deeper topic comprehension. The second assumption is that the relations between top-level ideas in a content hierarchy influence the recall of these top-level ideas significantly, whereas relations between bottom-level ideas have no influence on the recall of the bottom-level ideas. That is, how the superordinate ideas are related to each—for example, are the facts of a topic related to each other by a cause-effect relationship, or an argument relationships, or other—affects how the mental representation of a topic will be formed in the mind of a reader. Finally, the third assumption in this theory is that manipulating the 16 top-level structure in a text affects the recall and retention of information within the text. In other words, changing the superordinate ideas and their relations changes the mental model of the topic within the reader’s mind. According to both Bordwell (2013) on film comprehension, and Mann & Thompson (1988) on text comprehension, both the writer and the reader of a prose have schemas of the layout of different rhetorical structures. If the rhetorical structure in a prose is clear and coherent, adult readers, and even some young children who are already good readers—can easily be aware of what rhetorical structure is being used, and may use this rhetorical organization to influence the comprehension and recall of top-level or superordinate ideas (Fitzgerald & Spiegel, 1983; McGee, 1982; Meyer, 1975; Meyer & Poon, 2001). Meyer, Brandt and Bluth (1980) demonstrated the ability of rhetorical structure to influence recall of top-level ideas in their study on 102 ninth grade students. The sample contained poor, average and good comprehenders, based on their performance on previous SATs. They were tested on their recall of two prose passages, both with clearly identifiable top-level structures. One passage had a problem-solution rhetorical structure, and the other passage had a comparison rhetorical structure. Students who used the toplevel structure of the two passages had better recall and comprehension of the contents, regardless of which structure they used. McGee (1982) found similar results, when testing a sample of 60 third and fifth graders—including good and bad readers—on their recall of two prose passages, organized in a similar hierarchical structure. Results showed that regardless of reading 17 ability, the fifth graders had a high awareness of text structure, and therefore recalled more superordinate than subordinate ideas, regardless of topic. Because the superordinate ideas are the meanings and fact that the author wants to convey, comprehension of these superordinate ideas is correct comprehension as the author intended—that is, shared meaning has occurred (Meyer, 1999). Situation models in comprehension Along with using the rhetorical structure to guide their comprehension of a text, readers use their pre-existing situation model—or mental model—to interpret the semantic contents of this text and form a mental representation of the topic as a whole (Johnson-Laird, 1983; van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983; Zwaan & Radvanksy, 1998). The situation model is one of the factors in an individual that influences comprehension. Situation models are subjective, utilizing the pre-existing structures of linguistic knowledge, topic background knowledge, and general situations knowledge, in order to interpret the deeper meaning of the text (Johnson-Laird, 1983; van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983; Wittrock, 1990). McNamara, Kintsch, Songer and Kintsch (1996) examined the importance of situation models for prose comprehension. They examined the effects of both a low and a high coherence text on both low and high-knowledge participants. Fifty-six middle school students (M = 13 years, 11 months) were first tested for their prior knowledge of a specific topic. The researchers then presented some of these participants with the low coherence text first, and other participants with the high coherence text first. As expected, the participants with high prior knowledge—or a good situation model of the topic— 18 could more easily comprehend the low coherence text. Additionally, Ozuru, Dempsey and McNamara’s (2009) study on 108 college students (mean age = 21.1, SD = 3.6) found that an individuals’ prior knowledge was positively correlated to their comprehension of a biology text, and this text comprehension increased in the high coherence text condition compared to the low coherence text condition. These studies on situation models show that a situation model is important in comprehension when a text has low coherence, but that it is the high coherence of a text or text structure that directly increases comprehension. In other words, regardless of the reader’s situation model, incorrect comprehension occurs when the reader of a text or an audience member of a film builds a mental representation of a topic that is coherent but is not the mental representation of the topic that the author intended, as conveyed by the rhetorical structure (Bransford, 1979). The semantic comprehension of a text can only be defined as correct when it resembles the deeper meaning of the topic as intended by the author—this correct comprehension is then known as shared meaning (Meyer, 1999). The author’s main ideas are conveyed at the top-level of the structure, and using the rhetorical structure of a text to guide encoding and retrieval of these higher level ideas is thus most effective in leading readers to shared meaning (Meyer, 1987; Meyer et al, 1980). In turn, shared meaning or correct comprehension can be evaluated using the structure of the recall of a reader. Evaluating comprehension Readers use the rhetorical structure to comprehend the topic as the author intended; that is, for shared meaning to occur. By doing this, they recall the information 19 in the same hierarchical organization that the information was originally presented in (Marshall & Glock, 1978; McGee, 1982; Meyer, 1975; 1977). This means that the structure of their mental representation of a topic will resemble the structure the author used to convey the deeper meaning of the topic (Marshall & Glock, 1978; McGee, 1982). The structure of their recall can be used to evaluate if they have indeed comprehended the meaning of the text, as intended by the author. Problems in evaluating the comprehension of a text were inherent in the measures of comprehension previously used (Meyer, 1975). Previous measures of comprehension used a question and answer format (for an overview of research in these comprehension measures, see Meyer, 1975). The most consistent problems in using this kind of comprehension technique was the inability of the examiners to decide which topic points within the prose to question on, and the difficulty in providing a rationale of why these specific topic points were chosen to measure comprehension of the prose as a whole (Meyer, 1975). Because of these problems, examiners could not evaluate if a true, global comprehension of a text had occurred. In contrast, rhetorical structures allowed examiners to evaluate comprehension of the passage. Examiners scored the verbatim and paraphrased recalls in terms of content word units and relationship units. They then scored these text content units and text relationship units according to their absence or presence in the structure of a passage (Meyer, 1975). Comprehension was evaluated as having high recall of both content and relationship units present in the structure of the passage, and according to this organization of the passage. This method was found reliable, and there was inter-rater 20 agreement over 99% of the time, as to which units were absent or present within the passage (Meyer, 1975). Therefore, a structured prose is important not only in leading a reader to having shared meaning with the author, but also to evaluate that shared meaning—or correct comprehension—has occurred. Therefore, choosing a clear, coherent rhetorical structure is necessary to judge the comprehension and effectiveness of a prose. Choosing a Rhetorical Structure in Prose An author understands the importance of structuring their prose according to a coherent and clear rhetorical structure. The next step would involve knowing which rhetorical structure would be the most effective for shared meaning. Introducing some rhetorical structures Terminology for rhetorical structures has differed between researchers (a few examples are in Meyer, 1975; Mann & Thompson, 1988; Kosseim & Lapalme, 2000) but the essence of each rhetorical relation is consistent, and it is these rhetorical relations that define the structure or organization of a prose. For example, a response rhetorical relation can define a problem-solution rhetorical structure, a question-answer rhetorical structure, or a remark-reply rhetorical structure. In all these three structures, the superordinate ideas carry equal weight and resolve one another. On the other hand, an argument rhetorical relation can define an argument rhetorical structure, an alternative rhetorical structure or a comparison rhetorical structure, where more weight is given to one superordinate idea and the rest of the ideas are used to support this. Time setting and location setting rhetorical relations define a narrative structure, where a sequence of events are related in 21 the time context they occur. Explanation or elaboration rhetorical relations describe a descriptive structure, where previously related information is further explained in either a more concrete or more abstract manner. Considering semantic content Having a coherent rhetorical structure can improve comprehension of a topic, regardless of what structure is being used by a reader or an author. This is seen in the Meyer, Brandt and Bluth (1980) study explained in the previous section. Students, who used the top- level structures of two passages with two different structures, had similar high recall, regardless of which structure was used in the passage. Therefore, it is important for an author to choose a rhetorical structure to convey the topic in a prose; regardless of what structure he or she chooses. Additionally, Meyer (1974) found that the same structure produce similar recall, regardless of the content of the prose. She found this to be the case when testing comprehension of two different passages with identical structure but different content. She tested two groups of 24 university students, giving them two passages, of identical structure but different content. She measured their comprehension by scoring their recall of content and relationship units. A correlation of .55 was found between recall frequencies in the content structure of the two passages. These results implied that the comprehension of a passage is closely related to the structure of the passage, regardless of the content. So how does the author decide which structure to use to convey his or her topic meaning? When deciding how to structure a prose, Kosseim and Lapalme (2000) pointed 22 out the importance of considering both the content of a topic to be conveyed, as well as the structure with which to convey this topic. The author, having a topic in mind, must know what he or she wants to say about that topic, and what meaning they want a reader to elicit from their text about that topic. This meaning that the author wants to convey to the reader ultimately places semantic constraints on how the author will structure the prose. For example, the author wants to write about a specific mechanical procedure. Specifically, he or she wants a reader to understand how to fix a specific problem that arises during that mechanical procedure. This specific intention of meaning automatically puts constraints on what rhetorical relations—and therefore rhetorical structure—the author must now use. In this case, a response rhetorical structure—with problem-solution relations between the superordinate ideas—will be more effective for shared meaning to occur, rather than an argument rhetorical structure or a descriptive rhetorical structure. Considering situation models On the other hand, if the intention of the author is to inform a reader about a topic with which the author predicts the reader to be unfamiliar with, the choice is often a descriptive structure, in which they can explain details of the topic in either more abstract or more concrete ways (Graesser et al., 2003). This can effectively support the semantic intentions of the author. However, this structure but might not be entirely effective in shared meaning. According to Graesser, McNamara and Louwerse (2003) and Graesser, Singer and Trabasso (1994), a reader of a descriptive text will have more difficulty in inferring connections between propositions in a descriptive structure, because a) temporal and 23 causal connection within a descriptive structure are harder to infer, therefore harder to interpret the relationship between one fact of the topic to the other, and b) the reader might not have an extensive knowledge—or situation model—of that topic to begin with, with which to interpret meaning. In fact, results of the study done by McGee (1982) on 40 third and fifth grade—as described earlier on—showed that despite comprehension being higher with an awareness of the rhetorical structure, only 40% of the topic material within this descriptive structure was generally comprehended. Therefore, an author must also take into account the situation model of the reader (Meyer, 1999; van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983). The situation model of a reader can lead them to make some misconstruction of meaning. However, the author cannot predict every individual difference of a reader, in order to prevent a misconstruction of meaning (Meyer, 1999). A good reader or an avid film watcher can modify the text or film structure to fit their situation model, or update their own situation model to understand the meaning of the author (Meyer, 1999; Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998). Therefore, the most important fact for the author to consider is to have a coherent structure or “thread of discourse” (Grimes, 1975; Meyer, 1999). Choosing rhetorical structures Having a coherent rhetorical structure can improve comprehension of a topic; regardless of what structure a reader or an author is using, or what the intended meaning of the topic is. However, the inherent nature of some content structures produces superior recall than others (Graesser et al., 2003; Graesser et al., 1994; Lehr, 1988; Meyer, 1975; Thorndyke, 1977). 24 For example, the narrative structure is perhaps the most effective if an author wants to convey a series of events that can happen in the general lifespan of an individual. The intention of the author is to convey, in space and time, an individual with goals; what these goals are; how this individual goes about pursuing these goals; how they deal with and react to obstacles in their pursuit of these goals; and, the themes or deeper meaning of these events or situation (Graesser et al., 2003). This describes the majority of most commercial books and films—that is, excluding academic, religious, and scientific and lifestyle books and films. Second, the narrative structure has a similar blueprint to the situation models of everyday experiences that a reader has (Cowen, 1988; Graesser et al., 1994). Therefore, the reader is more likely to comprehend both the events and the situation—including the deeper meaning of the text—regardless of the differing themes, because they can more easily use the rhetorical structure and superordinate ideas of the author, and use their situation models of life events (Graesser et al., 1994). In fact, Thorndyke (1977) measured the influence of plot or events in the top-level of a structure of a text on recall and comprehension of other ideas or themes within the text. He measured the recall and comprehension of 112 undergraduate students on two passages with similar plot structure but different contents. The results showed recall and comprehension were dependent on the amount of plot structure present, and not the overall content of the passages. Using this plot structure, participants recall and comprehension were higher for themes within the passages, and not as much the precise details of the passages. Perhaps the frequent use of the narrative structure gives it an unfair advantage over other structures for some topics. Reviews of several studies on the theme 25 comprehension of younger children (cf. Lehr, 1988) claim that young children were able to recall and comprehend theme from narrative texts more easily than from descriptive texts. This was due to their frequent encounter and familiarity with the narrative structure, and lack of familiarity with the descriptive structure, even though the latter is most used by authors to describe new, unfamiliar events. Story books for younger readers, or to read to younger children, almost always follow a narrative structure. Even though younger children are less able to construct proper, analytical statement of themes as adults can, both children and adults are more likely to interpret theme from the narrative structure (Lehr, 1988). Investigating Rhetorical Structure in Film Film literacy develops with an individual’s age and knowledge of the world, with adults comprehending film narratives as easily as text passages, regardless of length (Smith et al., 1985). The relationship between montage linearity, comprehension and recall is similar to that in text, confirming that schema of rhetorical structures transcends modalities (Cowen, 1988). However, according to Cowen (1988), the film is more expressive than the text in conveying time and spatial relationships, If the film does not follow the conventional narrative edit, an audience member will likely fill in the gaps, and achieve optimal intellectual activity and entertainment (Cowen, 1988; Phillips, 2000). To demonstrate the relationship between the structure of a film and its audience, Cowen (1988) manipulated a short film with a narrative structure into four different edits. All edited versions contained the same shots, and had no verbal information such as 26 voice-over or dialogue or text. They differed only in the degree of linearity between these shots. Linearity was defined as following a typical narrative story grammar. The 96 participants were randomly assigned to one of the four short film versions. They were tested on their recall and comprehension of linear events, and were asked to reconstruct the story with elaborations. Finally, they were asked for their judgments of affinity towards the character, towards specific shots and towards the film as a whole. Cowen found that having to mentally construct a more linear comprehension—in the less linear films—using an individual’ schema did not affect their judgments of comprehension of the film. However, the less linear the film edit, the more negative the participants evaluation of the actual film, as well as of the character and shots. In essence, it is the linearity of a film itself that affects the aesthetic experience of the film, not the linearity of the comprehension. Need for present investigation Some studies showed that the narrative structure was better for comprehension of plot and theme, due to its linear nature in which the author could convey the topic meaning. In contrast, Cowen’s study showed that linearity was not an important factor in comprehension, but it was an important factor in judging affinity. Overall, understanding how an audience comprehends a film trailer is necessary to measure judgments of the topic of the film, judgments of affinity towards the film, reproductions of stories, and other reactions. This is especially important in film trailers, which are widely used as marketing before a theatrical release. It therefore seems necessary to do further research on what type of rhetorical structures in films are most effective at leading to a 27 comprehension and affinity of a whole film topic and its themes, starting with a narrative and descriptive rhetorical structure. 28 CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY Design and Participants One hundred and thirty nine undergraduate students (70 percent female, 30 percent male) volunteered for participation from a mid-size university in the Western United states. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four between-subjects experimental conditions of Rhetorical Structure—Narrative vs. Descriptive vs. Original vs. Control, with 37, 33, 34, and 35 of the students present in each of the conditions, respectively. Demographic data of the participants revealed that: 1) their primary language was English (76%); and 2) they fell into four age groups as follows:18-23 (89%), 24-29 (10%), 30-35 (.7%) and 36+ (.7%). On a scale of 1 = not at all to 7 = daily, participants’ mean film viewing behavior was 4.64 (SD = 1.33), and their mean nonschool related reading behavior was 3.15 (SD = 1.70). Experimental Materials The experimental materials consisted of the viewing task and four categories of measures: Category 1 – measures of self – consisted of a measure of general attitudes toward the world and a measure of personality. Category 2 – measures of comprehension– consisted of tasks assessing students’ understanding of the plot and theme. Category 3 – reaction measures – was comprised of self-reported measures of affinity, viewing desire, viewing intention and intention to share. Category four— 29 engagement measures—consisted of measures of engagement with the content of the viewing task. Viewing Task The viewing task consisted of a three minute, four second trailer consisting of six scenes distributed into three acts. The first act introduced the social setting and personal history of a protagonist that leads him down a destructive path, setting the scenes for the rest of the trailer. The second act focuses more deeply on the protagonist’s journey of discovery of hidden theories and connections between elements in the universe, along with the way the protagonist questions how he has been living out his existence. The third act shows the protagonist in a new state of awakening, and what he ultimately decides to do with this new insight. The trailer was produced by a professional film company to be shown globally to a wide audience in advance of the release of an original film. The trailer was edited for structure following a specific segmentation procedure. First, the trailer was divided into the three acts – each act roughly one minute long; next, each act was further segmented into two scenes, with each scene roughly 30 – 35 seconds long and distributed equally across the trailer (see Figure 1). In order to manipulate the trailer for rhetorical structure, black frames containing 1-4 words were inserted between the scenes, totaling six black frames showing text in white Times New Roman 12-pt. font and appearing for two seconds each. When the rhetorical structure was a narrative, the frames contained words that, as an aggregate, conveyed a story—specifically; Worldwide chaos/ a tortured man/ helpless/ discovers the hidden/ reaching true awakening/ for himself and humanity. When the rhetorical 30 structure was descriptive, the frames contained words designed to elaborate on the fundamental idea inherent in the scene subsequent to the frame—specifically, Galactic connections/ the inner conscious/ connected/ an unending continuum/ allowing true awakening/ for self and humanity. The final variation of the trailer was used as a control. In the control trailer, the black frames were present but absent of text. All words present in each black frame across both the narrative and descriptive condition were of equal strength and had equivalent visually evoking properties. The original trailer was not manipulated. Figure 1. Segmenting a film trailer into acts, text screens (T) and scenes. This figure illustrates the trailer manipulations in order to vary rhetorical structures. 31 Measures of Self Two measures of self were used in this investigation– general affect towards the world, and personality. Affect towards the world was measured by the Semantic Differential Technique and personality was measured by the BFAS. Semantic Differential Technique. The Semantic Differential Technique (SDT) (Osgood & Suci, 1955) was used to measure students’ attitude towards the world – a measure of how participants feel when they think about the world. The SDT consists of 30 pairs of polar adjectives placed across a weighted seven-point continuum, allowing for a “neutral” point in the middle of the continuum for any ambivalent judgments (Kaplan, 1972). The SDT measures to what degree the bipolar adjectives represent the respondents’ emotional evaluation of the topic or object at hand, with a value of 1 at the end of the weighted scale representing a positive adjective, and a 7 at the other end of the weighted scale representing a negative adjective. When used in its original form, Osgood & Suci, (1955) reported internal consistency reliability values of the SDT ranging from .80 to .90. In the present investigation, the original SDT was modified to insert adjectives relevant to students’ conceptions of the world, according to a modification procedure proposed by Carter, Ruggels and Schaffee (2001). First, adjectives were sampled from three sources—1) from the original instrument, 2) from the authors of the trailer relative to the kinds of impressions and concepts the authors intended for viewers to derive from watching the film, and 3) from a group of undergraduate participants relative to the adjectives they considered when they thought about the world. Fourteen adjective 32 pairings came from the original instrument, seven single adjectives from the authors, and 30 positive and 30 negative adjectives from the undergraduates. The resulting adjective pool totaled 95. Next, the adjectives need to be placed on the bi-polar scales. To accomplish this, the authors’ adjectives were paired with corresponding first-order acronyms selected from the Webster’s dictionary, yielding fourteen adjectives placed on seven scales as semantically different pairs; the undergraduate adjectives were selected from both the positive and negative adjective pool only if the adjectives had been stated by more than one undergraduate student. Eleven adjectives met this criterion. Antonyms for these eleven adjectives were generated by the undergraduates by consensus; yielding 22 adjectives placed on eleven scales as semantically different pairs. Two adjective pairings were deleted from the original scale. The final pool of adjectives consisted of 12 pairings from the original scale, seven pairings from the author, and 11 pairings from the undergraduates, resulting in a final total of 60 adjectives, and 30 pairings. Finally, the scales for each adjective line were reversed, in order to prevent a repetitive effect of consistently circling only at one end of the scale. The modified SDT reported a Cronbach’s alpha of .92. Personality Measures. The Big Five Aspect Scales (BFAS), constructed by DeYoung, Quilty & Peterson (2007), was used to measure the five broad domains of personality: Openness/Intellect, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism (DeYoung, 2010) as five aspects of individual differences in audience reactions to the trailer. The BFAS is a 100 item instrument in which participants rate on a scale of 1= strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, how much each item statement 33 applies to them. The instrument has demonstrated high retest reliability, construct and external validity, as well as applicability across observers and cultures (cf. McCrae and Costa, 1989; Jang, Livesley, Angleitner, Riemann, & Vernon, 2002; DeYoung & Gray, 2009). In order to reduce the administration time of the BFAS, and to address only the personality variables of interest in the present investigation, only three of the five scales were assessed based on recommendations by DeYoung, Weisberg, Quilty & Peterson (2013). Thus, the domains chosen for this study were: 1) Agreeableness, with its subscales of Compassion and Politeness , 2) Conscientiousness, with its subscale of Industriousness and Orderliness, and 3) Openness, with its subscales of Openness and Intellect . Agreeableness reflects the tendency toward altruism and cooperation, instead of aggressiveness and exploitation of others; Conscientiousness reflects a respondent’s ability and tendency to exert top down control of behavior and impulses in order to follow rules, a seemingly more stable personality trait; finally, Openness reflects imagination, curiosity, creativity, reasoning, cognitive flexibility, divergent thinking, comprehension, memory, and intelligence—the aggregate of which is termed Cognitive Exploration. Openness is the only one of the big five domains that is consistently positively correlated with intelligence tests (r = .30; Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997; DeYoung, 2010). In the present investigation, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness yielded Cronbach’s Alpha values of .84, .84, and .85, respectively. 34 Comprehension Measures Two aspects of comprehension were measured in this investigation— comprehension of plot and comprehension of theme. Plot was measured by the Plot Element Selection Task and Theme was measured by the Theme Rating Task. Plot. The Plot Element Selection Task was an original instrumented designed for this investigation to measure comprehension of plot within the trailer, by selecting from a pool of 14 possible plot elements and sequences; seven of these plot elements were true; that is, they were events that occurred in the trailer, and seven of these plot elements were false; that is, they were events that did not occur in the trailer. Developing the task consisted of several steps. First, we selected the important plot elements that progressed the story, as gathered from an initial interview with the author, resulting in nine plot elements. Next, four undergraduate students were asked to view several different trailers for the first time and identify the plot elements and sequences they interpreted from these trailers. Following the acquaintance task, the students were then tasked with viewing the experimental trailer for the first time and defining trailer’s plot elements and sequences. The majority of the undergraduates interpreted the plot elements and sequences similar to the nine selected by the researchers. Based on feedback from the undergraduates, we then edited the nine plot elements and reached a consensus of seven plot elements, which were then approved by the author of the trailer. The seven false plot elements were then selected from the visuals in the trailer that corresponded to a true plot element in the same act, based on the initial misinterpretations of plot by the same undergraduate participants. 35 The final task involved two subtasks: the first subtask required participants to select seven of 14 possible plot elements provided on a numbered list. The second subtask directed participants to place these 7 selected plot elements into a proper sequence according to the degree to which participants understood the order of events in the trailer. These subtasks yielded a Cronbach’s Alpha of .84. Theme. The Rating Theme Task (RTT), developed specifically for this investigation, was used to measure comprehension of the themes the author intended to be present in the trailer. In order to construct the RTT, we first collected the intended author themes during initial interviews with the author, resulting in five themes. Next, we derived secondary themes that may have been present in the trailer but were not specifically intended by the author. In order to identify these secondary themes, 12 undergraduate volunteers watched the trailer two times, each time writing down the themes they believed were present in the trailer, followed by the production of a statement summarizing the gist of the trailer’s most salient themes. Next, the salient themes were used to generate a reduced list of secondary themes, culled for redundancies. The reduced list was comprised of 17 secondary themes. Next, the reduced list of secondary themes were each rated by five undergraduate volunteers on a 7-point Likert scale to reflect the degree to which the undergraduates believed the themes were present in the trailer, (1 = not close to the theme 7 = extremely close to the theme). From this list, five themes with a mean closest to one, (with a standard deviation of 1 or less) were selected, so as to have five secondary themes that were perceived by a subjective audience but that were not similar to the author themes. Finally, we composed two distracter themes for each of the 5 author themes – one of medium similarity, and one of 36 far similarity. Each of these distracter themes were normed by the same undergraduate students on a scale of 1 = not similar to the theme to 7 = extremely similar to the theme. For the distracter themes of medium similarity, we selected those with a mean in the range of three to five, and a standard deviation of less than one. For the distracter themes of far similarity, we selected those with a mean of less than two and a standard deviation of less than one. Based on this procedure, the final RTT was composed of 20 themes: five true author themes, five secondary themes, and ten distracter themes. Ratings on the Task was accomplished using a series of curved lines extending outwards from each theme statement, with the smaller curved line indicating a ‘close’ rating, and the bigger curved line extending further out indicating a ‘far’ rating for each theme. The closer the rating, the closer a respondent perceived the statement as describing a theme as being present in the trailer. The further the rating, the further a respondent perceived the statement as being present in the trailer. The final RTT involved contained 20 theme statements; each according to how close or far the respondent felt the statement described a theme they believed was present in the trailer. To score these ratings, we measured these ratings on a centimeter ruler stick. The RTT had a Cronbach’s alpha of .76. Reaction Measures Four aspects of reaction behavior were measured in the present investigation— affinity toward the trailer, viewing desire of the film after watching the trailer, intention to view the film after watching the trailer, and intention to share the trailer. For each of 37 the four declarative measures, participants were asked to respond to a dichotomous scale (yes/no) and to a Likert scale on a scale of 1 = not at all to 5 = completely. E-prime was used to analyze participants’ reaction time when responding to each of the scales. On the dichotomous and Likert scales respectively, the questions for affinity were “Do you like the trailer?” and “How much do you like the trailer?” For viewing desire, the questions were “Do you want to watch the movie?” and “How much do you want to watch the movie?” Viewing intention was measured by responses to the questions: “Do you intend to watch the movie” and “How much do you intend to watch the movie?” Sharing intentions were measured by the questions: “Do you intend to share the trailer?” and “How much do you intend to share the trailer?”. Attention Measures Attention was measured with two tasks— attention to the verbal descriptions of ideas presented within the trailer and attention to the verbal statement made in the trailer. The former was measured by the Description Task; and the latter was assessed using the Recognition Task. Description. The Description Task, developed for the present investigation, was used to measure viewers’ attention to ideas in the trailer. Participants were asked to choose four of eight given statements that they believed were ideas present in the trailer. Of these eight statements, four were true descriptions of ideas made concrete in the trailer, and four were descriptions of ideas never made concrete in the trailer. 38 Description statements were selected by noting all complete verbal descriptions made in the trailer, resulting in only four true descriptions of concrete ideas made in the trailer, which we summarized to form our four true description statements. Four false description statements were then constructed using ideas that were implied through the visual stimuli but not made concrete through use of verbal descriptions. Recognition. The Recognition Task, also developed for the present investigation, was a measure of attention to statements made in the trailer. The task consisted of 18 statements: six verbatim statements, six paraphrased statements, and six false statements. Participants were asked to check ‘Y’ if they believed the statement was in the trailer, and ‘N’ if they believed the statement was not in the trailer. We scored the results by taking the correct ‘Y’ answers to both the verbatim and the paraphrased statements. The statements used for this task were selected following several guidelines. In order to avoid a recency effect, we wrote down all statements from all the three acts, resulting in 28 statements, and then selected only those statements that were audible and complete statements, avoiding exceptionally long statements and double-barreled statements. This reduction process resulted in a pool of 24 statements, with only six of those statements being in the first act, eight in the second act, and ten in the third act. We then used an online random generator to: 1) discard two statements from the second act, and four statements from the third act, resulting in six statements per act, and 2) select which six statements to keep verbatim, which six to paraphrase, and which six to make false, by a simple negation of the facts. 39 Procedure A total of 15 sessions were run over a two-week period, lasting between 50-70 minutes each. Participants were tested in groups in a computer lab, where each computer was pre-assigned to one of the four trailer conditions. On arrival, participants had to sign an Non Disclosure Agreement (NDA) and an informed consent before further participation. The participants were then asked to complete the first set of measures—the measures of self – the SDT, the demographics questionnaire, and the BFAS, in such an order so as to avoid influence of SDT on their BFAS answers. Next, participants were then instructed to click on a link on their computer screens that took them to the viewing task (film trailer) and the reaction measures. There, they were directed to watch the trailer using headphones pre-set at a standard volume and answer the e-prime questions before taking off the headphones. Participants then had to complete the two comprehension tasks and the two attention tasks—both of which were counter-balanced. Only after all participants in a running group completed all four of the tasks did they get further instructions. Thus, when they were ready, the participants were the instructed to complete the SDT post exposure task. To analyze a general affect shift, or a change of rating on a specific adjective, the SDT had been adapted for post stimuli administration. The only difference in the SDT pre and post exposure was in task demands—for pre exposure, the task demand was to circle how accurately the adjectives described theirs feelings when they thought about the world, and for post trailer exposure, the task demand was to circle how 40 accurately the adjectives described their feelings when they thought about the world, after watching the trailer (Cronbach’s alpha of .94). Finally, participants were thanked for their participation, debriefed, told not to speak of the experiment to other students who were yet to participate, and excused. 41 CHAPTER IV RESULTS It was important to first establish the degree of attention of the participants to the trailer across the four experimental conditions. Attention One (A1) measured attention to the audiovisual descriptions of ideas within the trailer and Attention Two (A2) measured attention to verbal statements made within the trailer. Since the two measures of attention were unrelated, scores for each were entered into two separate univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) across the four conditions of rhetorical structure (narrative, descriptive, original and control). For both estimates, the analyses yielded non-significant results,(E1: F (3,135) = 1.39, MSerror = .55, p > .05; E2: F (3,135) = 1.32, MSerror = 5.14, p > .05), indicating no differences in participant’s levels of attention with each of the four versions of the trailers. Total attention levels as estimated by audiovisual descriptions (M = 3.17, SD = 0.75) and verbal statements (M = 11.06, SD = 2.28) revealed that viewers attention to the trailer was reasonably high— 79% and 61%, respectively. Hypotheses Two experimental hypotheses were tested, namely: 1) participants viewing the trailer within the narrative structure would have better comprehension of the plot and themes of the trailer, and 2) participants in the narrative structure would have stronger reactions to the trailer. 42 Rhetorical Structures and Comprehension With regard to hypothesis one, comprehension was measured for the degree to which participants understood the plot elements and sequence in the trailer, and the degree to which participants understood the themes of the trailer as intended by the author. Since plot elements and plot sequence were correlated with one another (r = .317, p < .001), but showed no correlation with theme, (r = -.024, p = .779 and r = -.043, p = .616, respectively), both measures of plot, and the single measure of theme were entered as dependent measures into two separate analyses, respectively. We also reasoned that viewers’ worldview may mediate the degree to which the rhetorical structure of the trailers would influence plot and theme. Thus, plot elements and sequence were entered into a 1-way MANCOVA, and theme was entered into a 1-way ANCOVA, with worldview as the covariate in both analyses. The metric for worldview was formed as a composite variable from the SDT adjective pairings, using the sum of the seven pairs provided by the author. The values ranged from a value of 7 to 49, with a Cronbach’s alpha =.70. With regard to the measure of plot, the MANCOVA revealed a marginally significant multivariate effect of rhetorical structure on plot comprehension when controlling for worldview, ΛRoy= .050, F (3,134) = 2.23, p = .088, partial η2 = .048. Although worldview was not a significant covariate for either plot elements (F (1,134) = .043, p > .05) or sequence (F (1,134) = .107, p > .05), follow up univariate ANOVA’s revealed that participants had marginally better plot element comprehension in the Narrative (M = 5.00, SD = 1.05), Descriptive (M = 5.03, SD = 0.95) and Control (M = 5.14, SD = 1.09) conditions, compared to the Original condition (M = 4.56, SD = 0.89), F 43 (3,134) = 2.22, MSerror = 1.01, p = .089, partial η2 = .047 (see Figure 2). There were no significant differences of plot sequence scores between the Control (M = 1.66, SD = 1.73), Descriptive (M = 1.67, SD = 1.36), Narrative (M = 1.32, SD = 1.29) and Original condition (M = 1.32, SD = 1.20). For theme, the analysis revealed that worldview was a significant covariate, F (1,133) = 4.39, p = .038, partial η2 = .032. Thus, when controlling for worldview pre exposure, the ANCOVA yielded significant variations in theme comprehension between the four trailer conditions, F (3,133) = 2.52, MSerror = 0.607, p = .061, partial η2 = .054. Specifically, post hoc tests of least significant differences revealed that participants viewing the trailer in the Narrative structure (M = 1.64, SD = 0.64) demonstrated a significantly better concept of theme than participants in the Descriptive (M = 2.02, SD = 1.06), Original (M = 2.03, SD = 0.51) and Control (M = 2.07, SD = 0.85) conditions (see Figure 3). Rhetorical Structures and Reactions With regards to the second hypothesis, namely that participants in the narrative structure would have stronger reactions to the trailer, reactions were measured in terms of four variables—affinity toward the trailer, viewing desires, viewing intentions, and intentions to share the trailer. We again reasoned that the same covariate as above, worldview, may mediate the degree to which the rhetorical structure would influence reactions to the trailer. Because the four reaction variables were significantly correlated (see Table 1), a 1-Way MANCOVA was run to analyze differences in reaction due to the influence of rhetorical structures. The analysis revealed that worldview was not a significant covariate for any of the four univariate analyses of reaction. However, the MANCOVA revealed a 44 significant multivariate effect of rhetorical structures on reaction, ΛRoy= .250, F (3,134) = 8.31, p < .001, partial η2 = .200. Follow up univariate ANOVAs revealed participants had a significantly higher affinity to the trailer in the Narrative condition (M = 4.35, SD = 0.82), as compared to the Descriptive (M = 3.54, SD = 1.03), Original (M = 3.79, SD = 0.88) and Control (M = 3.51, SD = .85) conditions, F (3,134) = 6.89, MSerror = 0.797, p < .001, partial η2 = .134 (see Figure 4). However, there were no significant differences on viewing desire, viewing intention and sharing intention, as a result of the trailer conditions. Table 1 Bivariate Correlations (Pearson’s R) between Affinity, Viewing Desire, Viewing Intention and Sharing Intention. Affinity Viewing Viewing Sharing Desire Intention Intention .579** .406** .861** .692** Affinity -- .552** Viewing Desire -- -- Viewing Intention -- -- -- .700** Sharing Intention -- -- -- -- ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2 – tailed). 45 7 Comprehension 6 5 4 3 2 1 Narrative Descriptive Original Control Figure 2. Comprehension of Plot Elements across Rhetorical Structures. Higher mean Comprehension values indicate higher comprehension 5.2 5 4.8 4.6 4.4 4.2 4 3.8 3.6 3.4 3.2 3 2.8 2.6 2.4 2.2 2 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 Narrative Descriptive Original Control Figure 3. Comprehension of Theme across Rhetorical Structures. Lower mean values indicate higher comprehension 46 5 Affinity 4 3 2 1 Narrative Descriptive Original Control Figure 4. Affinity towards the trailer across Rhetorical Structures. Higher mean values indicate higher affinity. 47 Path Analysis from Personality to Sharing a Trailer From an applied point of view, we reasoned that it was important to establish a possible path, starting from when an individual views a trailer to the individual sharing the trailer, as this is the focus of marketing movie trailers—to incur cinema attendance. Therefore, a path analysis was constructed to address this issue. Seven variables were included in the path analysis. These variables were: the “openness” and “conscientiousness” personality traits of the participants viewing the trailer, theme comprehension of the movie trailer, feelings of the participant viewing the trailer when coming into the situation (participants’ world view), affinity towards the trailer, intention to view the film after watching the trailer, and intention to share the trailer. We selected these variables and constructed a path model based partly on theoretical views and partly on correlations calculated between these variables (see Tables 1 and 2). Personality types were used as our two exogenous variables because: 1) they are stable and fixed—they are not influenced by situations (but rather, influence reactions towards situations) (DeYoung, 2011), 2) they are correlated, but are not caused by each other, and 3) openness—being strongly linked to intelligence—was a predictor of theme comprehension, (R = .178, F (1,136) = 4.46, p = .036, with 3.2% of the variance accounted for), and 4) conscientiousness was a predictor of an individual’s worldview (R = .177, F (1,137) = 4.43, p = .037, with 3.1% of the variance accounted for). We chose theme and worldview as endogenous variables specifically because comprehension leads to positive or negative reactions towards a film (Tan, 2013), and having a similar 48 worldview as the author can be assumed to influence comprehension of the themes of the author. All paths from theme lead to affinity as a strong predictor of viewing intention, and ultimately, sharing intention (for the path model, see Figure 5). Table 2 Bivariate Correlations (Pearson’s R) between Conscientiousness, Openness, Agreeableness, Worldview, Plot Elements, Plot Sequence, Theme, Affinity, Viewing Desire, Viewing Intention and Sharing Intention. Conscientiousness Conscient- Open- Agree- World Plot Plot iousness ness ableness View Elements Sequence -- Openness .224** .031 -.177* -- -.129 -.186** -- -.092 -- Agreeableness Worldview Plot Elements Theme -.142 -.076 .031 .077 .065 -.178* .100 -.141 -.018 .025 -.029 .169* .317** -.024 -- Plot Sequence -- Theme -.043 -- Affinity .209* .252** .024 -.120 .077 -.061 -.250** Viewing Desire .181* .188* .102 -.017 .017 -.017 -.066 Viewing Intention .262** .214* .053 -.062 -.036 -.030 -.166 Sharing Intention .180* .080 .092 -.006 .001 -.031 -.147 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2 – tailed). * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 – tailed). 49 The variables in our model accounted for 49.9% of the variance, with a final error term of .71. However, while openness correlated with sharing intention -.072, the model implies a total effect of only .0146. In addition, conscientiousness correlated with sharing intention .180, but the path model implies a total effect of only .0024 (see Table 3). Thus, the model seems to underestimate the correlations between the two personality types and sharing intention, and is therefore not a good fit for the prediction. Therefore, the personality type of an individual and the individuals’ comprehension of the themes of a movie trailer is not enough to lead an individual to share the trailer. Table 3 Calculations in the path analysis from personality types to sharing intentions Variable OpennessSharing Intention Conscientious – Sharing Intention Direct Effect Variables Indirect Effect 0.0146 .080 Indirect 1 (openness-theme-affinityviewing intention-sharing intention) 0.0024 .180 Indirect 1 (conscientiousness-worldviewtheme-affinity-viewing intention-sharing intention) 50 Figure 5. A path analysis from personality types to sharing intention. β indicates the predictive value of the predicting variable and error (e) indicates error or other variance not accounted for by the predicting variable. 51 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Overview We sought to measure students’ attention to, comprehension of, and reactions towards, four different movie trailers manipulated for rhetorical structure—narrative, descriptive, original and control. The results revealed a mixed picture of the influence of rhetorical structure on the three outcomes of film trailer viewing. Since no differences were observed in participant’s attention to the four trailer conditions, it allowed us to effectively measure differences in comprehension and audience reaction between the trailer structures. Comprehension of the trailer, as measured by the number of plot elements viewers derived, was influenced by the narrative, descriptive and control trailer structures significantly more than the original structure of the film. However, there was no significant difference between the trailer structures for comprehension of plot sequence. In both measures, worldview was not a significant covariate. The narrative structure had a significantly stronger influence on comprehension of theme, compared to the other three structures. For theme, worldview was a significant covariate. The narrative structure also had a stronger influence on audience affinity towards the trailer, compared to the other three structures, and worldview was not a significant covariate. For application purposes, we proposed a model of possible cause-effect relationships from personality types to sharing intention. Our variables accounted for approximately half of the variance of sharing intention, but 52 the path model itself underestimated the correlations between sharing intention and personality types. Narrative structure and Comprehension Our first hypothesis was partially supported; namely, that participants viewing the trailer within the narrative structure would have better comprehension of the plot and themes of the trailer. Participants in the narrative structure did not have a better comprehension of plot elements and plot sequence than participants in the other three conditions, but they did have a better comprehension of theme than participants in the other three conditions. We expected the narrative structure to have a significantly higher influence on audience comprehension of plot elements and plot sequence than the other three trailer conditions. This influence was expected to occur because of the shared temporal nature of the narrative structure and the plot (Bordwell, 2013; Chatman, 1990). Additionally, we reasoned that plot elements that were emphasized at the higher rhetorical relations in the narrative structure would be more accurately and easily recalled and comprehended than elements at lower levels of the rhetorical hierarchy (Meyer, 1975). The results did not support our hypothesis. Instead, the narrative, descriptive and control conditions were equally effective in influencing comprehension of plot elements. A possible explanation for comprehension being equal across the three structures may lie in the nature of a film—that is, whether a film is non-fiction, or fiction. A non-fiction film relates real-life events in the correct order as the events happen. Thus, audience members of a non-fiction film have to construct a correct mental 53 model of the timeline of the real-life events depicted in the film (Chatman, 1990; Johnson-Laird, 1983; Meyer, 1975). For any film, the narrative structure—or the direct storytelling of the sequence of events in correct temporal order—is therefore more crucial in conveying the timeline of events in a non-fiction film, as compared to a fiction film. (Chatman, 1990; Tan, 2013). In a fiction film, the author has the freedom to decide what events happen, in what sequence they happen, and how these events happen. A fictionfilm author is therefore unrestricted in conveying his or her intentions using different structures—such as descriptive, cause-effect, comparisons—or a combination thereof. It thus seems likely that in the case of a fiction film, any structure can effectively influence plot comprehension. Correct comprehension is then dependent on how clear the author makes his or her intentions through the structure, and with what situation model an audience member interprets the film (van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983; Meyer, 1999). Therefore, any rhetorical structure can be effective in influencing shared meaning, but this shared meaning is also influenced by the nature of the topic itself and the interaction with the situation model of an audience member. Thus, it is not surprising that the narrative, descriptive and control trailer structures in our investigation were equal in influencing comprehension, and only had an advantage over the no structure or original condition of the film trailer used in this investigation. A possible explanation for the significant advantage of the narrative, descriptive and control conditions over the original condition in influencing comprehension of plot elements lies in the manipulation of our trailer conditions. As supported by research (cf. Smith et al., 1985), both children and adults prefer short films with segments, or cut sequences. This is because a lengthy montage has too many transitions that may not be 54 comprehended in fragments—making overall comprehension more difficult. The original condition was the only one left un-manipulated, and therefore was not segmented in any way. Therefore, we viewed the narrative, descriptive and control structure—all segmented—as being more optimal than the original structure for comprehension. According to the definition of plot, comprehending plot sequence is illogical without a correct comprehension of plot elements. Thus, we would expect comprehension of plot sequences to be better in the narrative, descriptive and control condition, than the original condition because more plot elements were remembered in these three conditions. However, there were no significant differences in comprehension of plot sequence between the four conditions. Here we reason that comprehension can be subjective, based on the situation model an individual has of the topic within the trailer (van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983). Although plot elements were interpreted correctly as facts within the trailer, some of these plot elements could have been interpreted as happening in different orders by different individuals, depending on what rhetorical structure and situation models the viewers constructed during viewing (Meyer, 1975). Therefore, plot element placement in a timeline would be up to a viewer’s subjective interpretation, and not always as the author intended. We also expected the narrative structure to have a significantly higher influence on audience comprehension of theme compared to the other three trailer conditions. We reasoned that plot elements that were made concrete in the rhetorical relations of the narrative structure would lead to easier comprehension of plot, and therefore easier comprehension of the higher level idea or “theme” of the story (Meyer, 1975). Results supported this prediction, with the narrative structure having a significantly stronger 55 influence on comprehension of theme, compared to the other three structures. The findings from this investigation provide strong evidence that the comprehension of rhetorical relations leads to the correct interpretation or comprehension of higher order ideas, and therefore a correct mental model of the topic as intended by the author (Meyer, 1975). We expected an individual’s pre-conceived views of a particular topic, based on their situation model of the topic, to influence the extent to which the individual comprehended the topic. We therefore controlled for participants’ worldview when measuring comprehension and this “worldview” was based relative to the kinds of impressions and concepts the author predicted would be stimulated in viewers when watching the film. Worldview was not a significant covariate for plot comprehension, but was a significant covariate for theme. Since Chatman, (1990) contends that the plot of a movie deals with a sequence of events and acts as the facts in a film, we assumed that plot would be interpreted objectively. However, the theme of a movie is not expressed as a fact, but as the underlying ideas that go beyond the action of the plot, revealing the intent of the author. Thus, since the theme of a film is not explicitly stated, we can assume that audience members subjectively interpreted the theme, contingent upon their worldview. Narrative Structure and Reactions Our second hypothesis was also partially supported; namely, that participants viewing the trailer within the narrative structure would have stronger reactions to the trailer. 56 We expected participants in the narrative condition to have higher affinity towards the trailer, higher desire to watch the film on which the trailer was based, higher intention to watch the film, and higher intention to share the trailer. Our expectation was supported by the data. Audience members in the narrative condition had a higher affinity towards the trailer, compared to the other three conditions. This finding is supported by previous research showing that comprehension, resulting from resolved intellectual activity as a result of optimum arousal and a conclusive narration, can increase affinity towards a text (Phillips, 2000; Tan, 2013). The narrative structure had a high mean comprehension of both plot and theme. Thus, higher plot and theme scores, as indices of comprehension, suggest that the narrative structure had optimal intellectual activity and stimulation, which increased the viewers’ affinity to the topic and the trailer. Additionally, previous research has shown that as editing of a film becomes more linear, judgments of affinity towards the characters and judgments of affinity towards the film as a whole become more positive (Cowen, 1988). Additionally, it is possible that audience members who understood the theme of the author were more likely to appreciate and like the theme, and therefore have higher affinity toward it—at least in the narrative trailer. This result shows the importance of using a trailer to convey the theme of a film, and not just the plot. There was no significant influence of the narrative structure on viewing desire, viewing intention and sharing intention, compared to the other three structures. We make sense of this finding based on existing research of film trailers and movie attendance. Specifically, Kernan (2004) and Hixson (2005) both claim that “an original, innovative and comprehensive” film trailer is one that has the ability to motivate cinema attendance; 57 in our investigation, we measured viewing and sharing intentions as indices of cinema attendance. Our results show plot comprehension as being equal across all three conditions. Therefore, each condition was as likely as the next to motivate audience members to want to see the film and share the trailer of it. Additionally, our investigation does not focus on what an “original” or “innovative” film trailer entails; therefore, we can assume that predictions of movie attendance is more diverse than the scope of our current investigation. This could also explain why worldview was not a significant covariate on any of the four audience reactions; essentially, there seems to be more variables in the relationship between rhetorical structures, an audience’s comprehension thereof, and an audience’s reactions to a film trailer. Path Model From an applied point of view, we reasoned that it was important to establish a possible path, starting from when an individual views a trailer to the individual sharing the trailer, as this is the focus of marketing movie trailers—to incur cinema attendance. Therefore, a path analysis was constructed to address this issue. The variables we examined were the personality types of ‘openness’ and ‘conscientiousness’ as our starting exogenous variables; theme comprehension, worldview, affinity, and viewing intention were the variables along the path model; and, intention to share the trailer was our final endogenous variable. Our path model did account for a high percentage of variance in predicting cinema attendance. This was due to the variables within the model—such as personality, 58 comprehension, worldview (or topic opinions) and affinity—encompassing the possible factors that might affect an individual when deciding to share a trailer. However, what our model did not account for was possible other factors relating to the recipient or the medium with which the trailer would be shared. Non-empirical research of viral videos have proposed models considering the relationship—and the sender’s evaluation thereof—between the sender and recipient of the shared video. We also need to consider the medium available to share the trailer; do the participants intend to share the trailer online, via email, on a social media site, etc.? Our measures of audience reactions were not strong enough individually to claim to measure a phenomenon such as sharing intention or viewing intention; therefore, a possible modification for future research would be to form a composite variable of “reaction” using the four measures and going forward with this composite variable to measure audience reaction—both as a function of rhetorical structure and as an endogenous variable in a path analysis. The reaction measures in the current investigation meant the path model underestimated correlations between personality types and sharing a trailer. However, all four of the reaction measures were strongly correlated to one another (see Table 1); therefore, further investigations can focus on how to increase one, in order to increase the others. Limitations and Implications In conclusion, the results of this investigation provide strong evidence that the rhetorical structure has a clear influence on comprehension of higher-level ideas of a story, such as the theme, and this translates across mediums, such as film trailers. The author of a trailer must therefore ensure that the rhetorical relations of the trailer structure effectively convey the themes of the film, as much as the plot of the film; whereas plot 59 comprehension can predict cinema attendance, theme comprehension can increase audience affinity towards the trailer, and it is affinity that can predict viewing desires and intentions, and sharing intentions. However, shared meaning—or correct comprehension—is dependent on the interaction between the reader and the film. The author can only structure their film trailer in a way they determines to be most effective in conveying the topic and what they want the audience to feel about the topic. The other factor involved in comprehension is the situation model of an audience member, of which we did not measure. Measuring the background knowledge of the audience members of this film topic could have explained some differences in comprehension between audience members, regardless of, or in addition to, the rhetorical structure. Because of time and permission constraints, the present investigation did not control for the audiovisuals of the trailer, which were identical between conditions. This could have affected the comprehension of plot and theme. Visual and auditory information is perceived simultaneously in a film, and their relationship from shot to shot, and from scene to scene, affects the interpretation of a film in the mind of an audience member (Cowen, 1988). These audiovisuals in our film trailer could have been different than just narrative or descriptive in their rhetorical relationships, thus competing with the effect of the narrative or descriptive text that was superimposed on them. As Huck, Helper and Hickman (1987) stated, “in a well designed book…both the illustrations and the text must bear the burden of narration” (p. 197). Additionally, we did not compare in our experimental conditions rhetorical structures beyond those that are narrative and descriptive. This could be a strong point for further research; to compare 60 more rhetorical structures against one another in the comparison of an identical film topic. However, despite these limitations, the present investigation provided strong evidence that a segmented montage is more effective in comprehension of the topic than one that is not segmented. Further investigations into the effect of rhetorical structures that encompass both text and audiovisuals on segmented trailers would add valuable information in the relatively novel field of film comprehension, allowing stronger links between text and film comprehension to be established. Overall, research into the various influences of different rhetorical structures of film trailers has received very little empirical attention. The present study has important implications in translating research that has so far been done only in text comprehension onto film comprehension. Future investigations into how to edit and organize film trailers to influence the audience members’ comprehension and reactions would be of value for both film marketing and the general field of film research. 61 REFERENCES 62 REFERENCES Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological bulletin, 121(2), 219. Baggett, P. (1979). Structurally equivalent stories in movie and text and the effect of the medium on recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(3), 333356. Black, J. B., & Wilensky, R. (1979). An Evaluation of Story Grammars. Cognitive Science, 3(3), 213-229. Branigan, E. (2013). Narrative comprehension and film. London: Routledge. Bordwell, D. (2013). Narration in the fiction film. London: Routledge. Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning. In S. A. Christianson (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp.3-31). Psychology Press. Branigan, E. (2013). Narrative comprehension and film. London: Routledge. 63 Bransford, J. D. (1979). Human cognition: Learning, understanding and remembering. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Brewer, W. F. (1980). Literary theory, rhetoric, and stylistics: Implications for psychology. Theoretical issues in reading comprehension, 221, 239. Brewer, W. F., & Lichtenstein, E. H. (1982). Stories are to entertain: A structural-affect theory of stories. Journal of Pragmatics, 6(5), 473-486. Carter, R. F., Ruggels, W. L., & Chaffee, S. H. (1968). The semantic differential in opinion measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 666-674. Chatman, S. B. (1990). Coming to terms: The rhetoric of narrative in fiction and film. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Cowen, P. S. (1988). Manipulating montage: Effects on film comprehension, recall, person perception, and aesthetic responses. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 6(2), 97-115. DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Toward a theory of the Big Five. Psychological Inquiry, 21(1), 26-33. DeYoung, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior, and cognition. The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology, 323-346. DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of personality and social psychology, 93(5), 880. 64 DeYoung, C. G., Weisberg, Y. J., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2013). Unifying the aspects of the Big Five, the interpersonal circumplex, and trait affiliation. Journal of personality, 81(5), 465-475. Eliashberg, J., & Sawhney, M. S. (1994). Modeling goes to Hollywood: Predicting individual differences in movie enjoyment. Management Science, 40(9), 11511173. Eliashberg, J., & Shugan, S. M. (1997). Film critics: Influencers or predictors? The Journal of Marketing, 68-78. Fillmore, C. J. (1977). The case for case reopened. Syntax and semantics, 8(1977), 59-82. Fitzgerald, J., & Spiegel, D. L. (1983). Enhancing children's reading comprehension through instruction in narrative structure. Journal of Literacy Research, 15(2), 117. Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency. Cognition and emotion, 1(2), 115-143. Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 430-445. Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2003). What do readers need to learn in order to process coherence relations in narrative and expository text. Rethinking reading comprehension, 82-98. 65 Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological review, 101(3), 371-395. Grimes, J. E. (1975). The thread of discourse (Vol. 207). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Hixson, Thomas. (2005). Mission Possible: Targeting Trailers to Movie Audiences. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing, 14(3), 210-224. Iran-Nejad, A. (1987). Cognitive and affective causes of interest and liking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(2), 120. Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Vernon, P. A. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(1), 83-101. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kaplan, K. J. (1972). On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic differential technique. Psychological Bulletin, 77(5), 361. Kernan, Lisa. (2004). Coming Attractions: Reading American Movie Trailers. Austin, Tx: University of Texas. Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological review, 85(5), 363. 66 Kintsch, W., & Yarbrough, J. C. (1982). Role of rhetorical structure in text comprehension. Journal of educational psychology, 74(6), 828. Kosseim, L., & Lapalme, G. (2000). Choosing rhetorical structures to plan instructional texts. Computational intelligence, 16(3), 408-445. Lehr, S. (1988). The child's developing sense of theme as a response to literature. Reading Research Quarterly, 337-357. Mann, W. C., Matthiessen, C. M. I. M., & Thompson, S. A. (1992). Rhetorical structure theory and text analysis. Discourse description: Diverse linguistic analyses of a fund-raising text, 39-78. Mann, W.C., & Thompson, S. (1988). Rhetorical structure theory: Towards a functional theory of text organization. Text, 8(3), 243-281. Marich, Robert. (2005). Marketing to Moviegoers: a Handbook of Strategies Used by Major Studios and Independents. Burlington, MA: Focal Press. Marshall, N., & Glock, M. D. (1978). Comprehension of connected discourse: A study into the relationships between the structure of text and information recalled. Reading Research Quarterly, 10-56. McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American psychologist, 52(5), 509. McGee, L. M. (1982). Awareness of text structure: Effects on children's recall of expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 581-590. 67 McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and instruction, 14(1), 1-43. Meyer, B. J. (1974). The organization of prose and its effect on recall. Research Report No. 1, Reading.and Learning series. Department of Education, Cornell University. Meyer, B. J. (1975). Identification of the Structure of prose and Its Implications for the Study of Reading and Memorya. Journal of Literacy Research, 7(1), 7-47. Meyer, B. J. (1999). Importance of text structure in everyday reading. In Understanding language understanding (pp. 227-252). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Meyer, B. J., Brandt, D. M., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading research quarterly, 72-103. Meyer, B. J., & Poon, L. W. (2001). Effects of structure strategy training and signaling on recall of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 141. Mischel, W. (2013). Personality and assessment. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Osgood, C. E., & Suci, G. J. (1955). Factor analysis of meaning. Journal of experimental psychology, 50(5), 325. Ozuru, Y., Dempsey, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2009). Prior knowledge, reading skill, and text cohesion in the comprehension of science texts. Learning and instruction, 19(3), 228-242. 68 Phillips, B. J. (2000). The impact of verbal anchoring on consumer response to image ads. Journal of Advertising, 15-24. Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. Educational psychologist, 26(34), 299-323. Smith, R., Anderson, D. R., & Fischer, C. (1985). Young children's comprehension of montage. Child development, 962-971. Tan, E. S. (2013). Emotion and the structure of narrative film: Film as an emotion machine. London: Routledge. Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive psychology, 9(1), 77-110. Van Dijk, T. A., Kintsch, W (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press. Wittrock, M. C. (1989). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational Psychologist, 24(4), 345-376. Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological bulletin, 123(2), 162. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1985). Affect, mood, and emotion as determinants of selective exposure. Selective exposure to communication, 157-190. 69