Andrew Stockman Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Andrew Stockman Outline Introduction Rod and cone vision Rod vision is achromatic How do we see colour with cone vision? Vision and visual pathways Achromatic and chromatic cone vision (colour and luminance) NEUR3045 Visual Neuroscience Light 400 - 700 nm is important for vision ROD AND CONE VISION Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 1 Andrew Stockman The retina is carpeted with lightsensitive rods and cones An inverted image is formed on the retina Rods and cones Webvision Human photoreceptors Cones Rods Daytime, achromatic and chromatic vision 3 types Long-wavelengthsensitive (L) or “red” cone Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Human photoreceptors Cones Achromatic night vision 1 type Rod Daytime, achromatic and chromatic vision 3 types Long-wavelengthsensitive (L) or “red” cone Middle-wavelengthsensitive (M) or “green” cone Middle-wavelengthsensitive (M) or “green” cone Short-wavelengthsensitive (S) or “blue” cone Short-wavelengthsensitive (S) or “blue” cone 2 Andrew Stockman Our vision has to operate over an enormous range of 1012 (1,000,000,000,000) levels Sunlight Moonlight Why do we have rods and cones? Typical ambient light levels Indoor lighting Starlight Visual function Absolute rod threshold Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Damaging levels To cover that range we have two different types of photoreceptor... Two systems Rods that are optimized for low light levels Cones that are optimized for higher light levels Indoor lighting Starlight Sunlight Moonlight Typical ambient light levels Indoor lighting Starlight Photopic retinal illuminance (log phot td) -4.3 -2.4 -0.5 1.1 2.7 Scotopic retinal illuminance (log scot td) -3.9 -2.0 -0.1 1.5 3.1 SCOTOPIC Visual function Visual function Absolute rod threshold Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Sensitive ROD SYSTEM Lower range Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Damaging levels Less sensitive CONE SYSTEM Upper range Sunlight Moonlight Typical ambient light levels Absolute rod threshold Scotopic levels (below cone threshold) where rod vision functions alone. A range of c. 103.5 MESOPIC Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Mesopic levels where rod and cone vision function together. A range of c. 103 4.5 6.5 8.5 4.9 6.9 8.9 PHOTOPIC Damage possible Photopic levels (above rod saturation) where cone vision functions alone. A range of > 106 3 Andrew Stockman Rod vision Achromatic High sensitivity Poor detail and no colour Cone vision Achromatic and chromatic Lower sensitivity Detail and good colour Facts and figures There are about 120 million rods. They are absent in the central 0.3 mm diameter area of the fovea, known as the fovea centralis. There are only about 6 to 7 million cones. They are much more concentrated in the fovea. Rod and cone distribution 0.3 mm of eccentricity is about 1 deg of visual angle Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Rod density peaks at about 20 deg eccentricity At night, you have to look away from things to see them in more detail 4 Andrew Stockman Cone distribution and photoreceptor mosaics During the day, you have to look at things directly to see them in detail Original photograph Cones peak at the centre of vision at 0 deg The human visual system is a foveating system Simulation of what we see when we fixate with cone vision… Visual acuity gets much poorer with eccentricity Credit: Stuart Anstis, UCSD Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Credit: Stuart Anstis, UCSD 5 Andrew Stockman The foveal region is magnified in the cortical (brain) representation ROD AND CONE DIFFERENCES What is visual psychophysics? Psychophysicists study human vision by measuring an observer’s performance on carefully chosen perceptual tasks. INPUT Rod and cone differences can be demonstrated using several techniques, including visual psychophysics. PROCESSING ? STIMULUS VISUAL SYSTEM OUTPUT ? PERCEPTION The idea is to work out what is going on inside the visual system from the relationship between the stimulus at the input and the response of the observer. Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 6 Andrew Stockman Rod and cone threshold versus intensity curves Rod-cone threshold sensitivity differences Rod‐cone break How might we measure them? Rods are about one thousand times more sensitive than cones. They can be triggered by individual photons. Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Spectral sensitivity differences 7 Andrew Stockman Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Intensity 10-deg eccentric fixation Space (x) Space (x) Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Intensity 10-deg eccentric fixation Space (x) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Space (x) 8 Andrew Stockman Relative sensitivity (energy) -1 Rod and cone spectral sensitivity curves Plotted as the more conventional spectral “sensitivity” curve Plotted as “thresholds” versus wavelength curves -2 -3 -4 -5 Sensitivity = 1/threshold or log (sensitivity) = -log(threshold) Rod-cone dark adaptation curves Approximate dark‐ adapted photoreceptor sensitivities. 4 Log10 quantal sensitivity 3 Rods 2 1 Rod‐cone break L 0 S -1 M -2 -3 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 9 Andrew Stockman Rod-cone dark adaptation curves Cone plateau Temporal differences Rods take much longer to recover after a bleach than cones From Hecht, Haig & Chase (1937) Suction electrode recording Photocurrent responses Greater temporal integration improves rod sensitivity (but reduces temporal acuity) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 10 Andrew Stockman Highest flicker rates that can just be seen (c.f.f.) FLICKER INVISIBLE Rods Spatial differences (visual acuity) Cones FLICKER VISIBLE Photopically (cone) equated scale Rod and cone visual acuities Rod and cone visual acuities König (1897) 1/1.6=.63 Greater spatial integration improves rod sensitivity but reduces acuity 1/1=1 1/.2=5 König (1897) Rods Rods The loss must be postreceptoral because the rods are smaller than cones in the periphery) Rods Rods The acuity here is defined as the reciprocal value of the size of the gap (measured in arc minutes) that can be reliably identified. Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 11 Andrew Stockman Directional sensitivity differences Stiles-Crawford effect Rod vision is achromatic Vision at the photoreceptor stage is relatively simple because the output of each photoreceptor is: UNIVARIANT Why? What does univariant mean? Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 12 Andrew Stockman UNIVARIANCE UNIVARIANCE Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. Rod Once absorbed a photon produces the same change in photoreceptor output whatever its wavelength. UNIVARIANCE Rod So, if you monitor the rod output, you can’t tell which “colour” of photon has been absorbed. UNIVARIANCE Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. What does vary with wavelength is the probability that a photon will be absorbed. Rod This is reflected in what is called a “spectral sensitivity function”. All the photoreceptor effectively does is to count photons. Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 13 Andrew Stockman Rod spectral sensitivity function Rod spectral sensitivity function (V) More sensitive -1 Less sensitive -2 -3 CIE V' -4 -5 -6 0 -1 10V’ -2 CIE V' -3 log(V) -4 -5 -6 Much more detail at lower sensitivities -7 400 -7 400 500 600 700 Linear sensitivity plot Logarithmic sensitivity plot 500 600 700 800 Relative sensitivity (energy units) 0 Log relative sensitivity (energy units) Log relative sensitivity (energy units) (also known as the scotopic luminosity curve, CIE V) 1.0 CIE V' 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 400 Wavelength (nm) 800 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm) In order of rod sensitivity: 0 > > > > -1 > > > > -2 -3 CIE V' -4 -5 -6 -7 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 800 Log relative sensitivity (energy units) Log relative sensitivity (energy units) Rod spectral sensitivity function (V) 0 -1 So, imagine you have four lights of the same intensity (indicated here by the height) -2 -3 The green will look brightest, then blue, then yellow and lastly the red will be the dimmest CIE V' -4 -5 -6 -7 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) 14 Log relative sensitivity (energy units) Andrew Stockman 0 -1 We can adjust the intensities to compensate for the sensitivity differences. -2 -3 When this has been done, the four lights will look completely identical. CIE V' -4 Rod -5 -6 Changes in light intensity are confounded with changes in colour (wavelength) -7 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) UNIVARIANCE A consequence of univariance is that we are colour‐ blind when only one photoreceptor operates… A change in photoreceptor output can be caused by a change in intensity or by a change in colour. There is no way of telling which. Colour or intensity change?? Each photoreceptor is therefore ‘colour blind’, and is unable to distinguish between changes in colour and changes in intensity. Examples: SCOTOPIC VISION, cone monochromacy Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 15 Andrew Stockman Spectral sensitivities and the Purkinje shift Peak rod sensitivity Peak overall cone (L&M) sensitivity The Purkinje Shift Log10 quantal sensitivity 0 -1 A change in the relative brightness of colours as the light level changes because of the difference in spectral sensitivity between rod and cone vision (e.g., reds and oranges become darker as rods take over) -2 -3 L -4 Rods S -5 400 450 500 550 600 650 M 700 Wavelength (nm) Simulated: Dick Lyon & Lewis Collard at Wikimedia With three cone photoreceptors, our colour vision is chromatic… Cone spectral sensitivities Log10 quantal sensitivity 0 L M -1 S -2 -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 16 Andrew Stockman At the photoreceptors, colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Or to put it another way: How is colour encoded? Blue light 0 Log10 quantal sensitivity So, if each photoreceptor is colour‐ blind, how do we see colour? L -1 S M -2 -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm) Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light Blue light 0 L -1 S M Red light -2 -3 Log10 quantal sensitivity Log10 quantal sensitivity 0 L -1 S Green light M -2 Red light -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm) 17 Andrew Stockman Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light Red light Green light Purple light Yellow light White light But what happens next (i.e., how is colour encoded after the photoreceptors)? POSTRECEPTORAL COLOUR VISION Colour phenomenology Can provide clues about how colours are processed after the photoreceptors… Which pairs of colours coexist in a single, uniform patch of colour? Which pairs never coexist? WHY? Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 18 Andrew Stockman Reddish‐yellows? Reddish‐greens? Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Reddish‐blues? Bluish‐yellow? 19 Andrew Stockman The colour opponent theory of Hering The colour opponent theory of Hering Reds can get bluer or yellower but not greener Yellows can get greener or redder but not bluer The colour opponent theory of Hering The colour opponent theory of Hering Greens can get bluer or yellower but not redder Blues can get greener or redder but not yellower Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 20 Andrew Stockman The colour opponent theory of Hering The colour opponent theory of Hering is opposed to R-G is opposed to Y-B How might this be related to visual processing after the cones? Some ganglion cells are colour opponent Imagine that this is the region of space that the cell “sees” in the external world Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Some ganglion cells are colour opponent A red light falling on the central area excites the cell (makes it fire faster) 21 Andrew Stockman Some ganglion cells are colour opponent Some ganglion cells are colour opponent A green light falling on the surround area inhibits the cell (makes it fire slower) RED On-centre GREEN Off-surround Red-green colour opponency Some ganglion cells are colour opponent Four variants GREEN On-centre RED Off-surround Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 22 Andrew Stockman Blue/yellow pathway Source: David Heeger Summary LGN cell responses Trichromatic stage LESS COMMON Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Colour opponent stage 23 Andrew Stockman Colour… So that’s colour (chromatic) vision, but what about “luminance” (achromatic) vision? ACHROMATIC COMPONENTS CHROMATIC COMPONENTS Split the image into... CHROMATIC COMPONENTS By itself chromatic information provides relatively limited information… Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 24 Andrew Stockman ACHROMATIC COMPONENTS Achromatic and chromatic cone vision (colour and luminance) Achromatic information important for fine detail … In addition to neural pathways that signal colour there are also pathways that signal intensity or luminance: Luminance is encoded by summing the L‐ and M‐cone signals: Blue light L+M + Green light L+M + Yellow light L+M Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. + Red light + Purple light + White light + 25 Andrew Stockman Colour is in many ways secondary to luminance Colour after-effects + Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 26 Andrew Stockman + + + + Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 27 Andrew Stockman + Rob van Lier, Mark Vergeer & Stuart Anstis Boynton illusion Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Boynton illusion 28 Andrew Stockman Boynton illusion Boynton illusion Watercolour effect A wave-line colour illusion Seiyu Sohmiya Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 29 Andrew Stockman Neon Disk Neon Spreading www.blelb.com Interesting artistic effects occur when vision depends only on colour (and not on luminance) Detail from 'Plus Reversed', Richard Anuszkiewicz, 1960 from Viperlib Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 30 Andrew Stockman What are the postreceptoral neural substrates of the chromatic and luminance pathways? Red‐green chromatic pathways have been linked to the parvocellular retinal stream for L‐M. Parvocellular midget ganglion cell midget bipolars L These cells are chromatically opponent simply by virtue of the fact that they have single cone inputs to the centre of their receptive fields! M midget ganglion cell Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. From Rodieck (1998) 31 Andrew Stockman Koniocellular Blue‐yellow chromatic pathways have been linked to the koniocellular stream… diffuse bipolar diffuse bipolar S-cone bipolar blue-yellow bistratified ganglion cell From Rodieck (1998) Koniocellular diffuse bipolar S-cone bipolar diffuse bipolar blue-yellow bistratified ganglion cell Luminance pathways, which produce achromatic percepts, have been linked to the magnocellular stream. From Rodieck (1998) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 32 Andrew Stockman Magnocellular Austin Roorda, 2004 CONE ARRAY ON RETINA diffuse bipolar To be able to resolve this E, the image must be sampled at enough points. The parvocellular pathway, with its one‐to‐one cone to bipolar connections, provides enough samples. parasol ganglion cells From Rodieck (1998) The parvocellular pathway must be double‐duty supporting finely detailed luminance vision as well as much more coarse colour vision. The magnocellular pathway, with diffuse bipolar cells and many‐to‐one cone to bipolar connections, does not. Multiplexing colour and luminance information Austin Roorda, 2004 Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 33 Andrew Stockman Chromatic pathways, which produce chromatic percepts, have been linked to the parvocellular retinal stream. Luminance pathways, which produce achromatic percepts, have been linked to the magnocellular stream, but also depend on the parvocellular stream. Parvocellular pathway: High spatial frequencies (spatial detail) Low temporal frequencies Chromatic Lower contrast sensitivity Parvocellular pathway Koniocellular pathway Magnocellular pathway Magnocellular pathway: High temporal frequencies (motion/flicker) Low spatial frequencies Achromatic Higher contrast sensitivity From Rodieck (1998) Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. 34