Commentary/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension memory problems should at least be extended or discussed in a different context. In particular, the question arises of whether sentence comprehension deficits in aphasic or central nervous system (CNS) dysfunction patients are solely a consequence of a specific working memory resource deficit and/or a computational deficit. In other words, can there be an independent resource deficit without some interference from a computational deficit at the “postinterpretive” processing level? We will present data from an aphasic patient with verbal working memory deficits to broaden this discussion. Patient H. G., a 45-year-old male, suffered an ischemic stroke in 1996. T2-weighted images in the axial plane taken about half a year after the stroke revealed an extended lesion in the left temporoparietal region and a small lesion in the left posterior frontal area. The temporoparietal lesion encloses (1) the left Heschl gyrus, (2) the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus, (3) the supramarginal gyrus, and (4) the occipital gyri. The frontal lesion involves the middle portion of the praecentral gyrus and the posterior part of the frontal gyrus (F2p). The inferior frontal gyrus (F3) is spared. The functional deficit due to the frontal lesion could be twofold: (1) an articulatory deficit due to the lesion in the praecentral gyrus or (2) a verbal working memory deficit due to the lesion in the posterior F2 area. The lesioned supramarginal gyrus in the temporoparietal region should crucially affect the phonological short-term memory (STM; see Warrington 1975), with intact rehearsal processes (see unaffected F3 region). In terms of the functional–neuroanatomical correlates, H. G. should show the classical picture of an aphasic patient with an STM deficit and potentially a syntactic processing deficit during sentence comprehension. In a first step, we tested H. G. for deficits in the articulatory rehearsal and phonological storage system. Results indicate no STM deficit in H. G. Tests for verbal working memory capacities such as verbal word span (span: 2– 3) and an adapted aphasic reading span task (span: 1) as well as digit spans (forward and backward), however, indicate a severe verbal working memory deficit in H. G. Next, H. G. was tested with several sentence-comprehension paradigms engaging syntactic complexities (word category violations, subject–object relative clauses, subordinate clause violations). Furthermore, a combination of syntactic complexity with increasing propositional content was applied. We used both sentence picture-matching tasks and auditory and visual comprehension tasks in the two test batteries. Results indicate that H. G. did not show a significant deficit in sentence comprehension at the “interpretive” or “post-interpretive” level of processing. Thus, if working memory impairments exclusively influence “post-interpretive” processing, as proposed by C&W (sects. 3.3 and 3.4), H. G.’s data provide evidence against this hypothesis. Most strikingly, he did not show any effect of increasing propositional content. One example of this effect is that he scored 100% on a German version of the Token Test (DeRenzi & Vignolo 1962; Martin & Feher 1992) and in a judgment task with increasing propositional content. To ensure that the “untimed” accuracy measures of sentence comprehension were not due to a ceiling effect, we proceeded to test H. G. in a combined syntactic and semantic judgment paradigm utilizing event-related potentials (ERPs). We assumed that if the verbal working memory deficit of H. G. has any effect on sentence comprehension it should be visible in the on-line measurements of ERP components specific to syntactic and semantic processes (see also Friederici et al., in press). Even though this judgment paradigm did not use the most complex syntactic structures nor a dual-task manipulation with additional load, H. G. displayed an interesting picture. Whereas the performance data were in the normal range, H. G. showed that on-line computation of syntactic structure and semantic integration was influenced by the input parameters. The evaluation of syntactic violations (word category errors) and semantic violations (selectional restriction errors) and correct sentences in connected speech did not elicit any of the expected ERP components in H. G. A similar picture 104 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 emerged in the visual modality with a fast presentation rate (SOA: 500 msec). In a long SOA (1100 msec) visual manipulation, the N400 (onset: 350 msec) was elicited to semantic violations and a delayed P600 (onset: 1,000 msec) was elicited to syntactic violations. These results, although by no means exhaustive in the discussion of resource-capacity models, give evidence that reduced verbal working memory capacities might go hand in hand with a temporal deficit. In-line measures of syntactic processing using event-related brain potentials Marta Kutas and Jonathan W. King Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0515. kutas@cogsci.ucsd.edu king@cogsci.ucsd.edu Abstract: Scalp-recorded event-related potential (ERP) measures of reading and listening have been proved more sensitive to the time course of syntactic processing than the chronometric and behavioral data described by Caplan & Waters. ERP studies using sentences containing relative clauses indicate that there are individual differences in syntactic processing that appear at the earliest theoretically relevant time points and are attributable to working memory operations. Leaving aside a discussion of Caplan & Water’s (C&W’s) specific proposal that there are two distinct verbal working memory systems, we note that C&W base much of their argument on chronometric and behavioral data from the processing of sentences containing subject-relative (SR) and object-relative (OR) clauses. The focus on the contrast between SR and OR clauses is clear; arguments concerning limitations in working memory capacity permeate the experimental literature on this topic. The decision to focus on reaction time measures, however, is less clear to us. Although a host of measures show consistent chronometric differences between the two sentences, these effects are generally observed substantially after the point in the sentence where they would be expected owing to a difference in working memory storage demands per se. In the following examples, OR: The reporter who the senator harshly attacked admitted the error. SR: The reporter who harshly attacked the senator admitted the error. the noun phrase “the senator” in the OR sentence marks the beginning of the differential working memory load between the two sentence types. However, RT differences are not seen until the end of the relative clause or even later, on the main clause verb. Thus, one might question the sensitivity of the chronometric measures on which C&W base their arguments, at least if monotonic increases in memory load lead to monotonic increases in processing time, as many assume. There is, however, one measure of brain processing that shows differential activity for OR versus SR sentences starting precisely where one would expect from the usual working memory arguments, namely, event-related potentials (ERPs). While C&W miscite who found out what and when, they do note that the processing of OR sentences is associated with a greater negativity than the processing of SR sentences. This electrophysiological pattern has been observed repeatedly during word-by-word reading in English (King & Kutas 1995) and German (Mecklinger et al. 1995; Münte et al. 1997) as well as in connected speech (Müller et al. 1997). This ERP difference between subject and object relatives is large and is more pronounced over anterior electrode sites; with visual materials, it is also left-lateralized. (Indeed, it is this topographic characteristic of the data that C&W highlighted when referring to ERP results in their section on the anatomical locus of verbal working memory processes.) But, to emphasize the point, the electrophysiological data show a much earlier point of divergence. Moreover, King and Kutas (1995) found that the Commentary/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension ERPs to the main-clause verb in both SR and OR sentences showed a greater left anterior negativity than the second verb in sentences that did not contain relative clauses. Although tempted, we will refrain from arguing from this pattern of effects that the insensitivity of the behavioral reaction time data suggests they are more likely to reflect “post-integrative” effects. C&W also claim that there is no (behavioral) evidence of an interaction between sentence complexity and working memory capacity (WMC), and, because there is no evidence that sentence complexity has larger effects on low-WMC subjects, C&W conclude that RT data from the SR–OR contrast support their proposal for a verbal working memory subsystem specialized for syntactic analysis. Setting aside their interpretation of the behavioral data, we would like to point out that the ERP studies cited above also include notable demonstrations of individual differences in syntactic processing. In both written and spoken English, good comprehenders show large ERP differences between SR and OR sentence types, whereas poorer comprehenders show almost none (see, e.g., King & Kutas 1995; Müller et al. 1997). In good comprehenders, subject relatives elicit a frontal positivity, whereas object relatives show more negative deflections from this pattern. In poor comprehenders, the responses elicited by both the SR and the OR sentences are almost identical to each other and also identical to the response elicited by OR sentences in good comprehenders. It is likely that these good comprehenders would score higher on many (possibly language specific) WMC measures than would poor comprehenders. More sensitive measures thus do show the kind of individual differences C&W failed to find. Additionally, however, we find that good and poor comprehenders show ERP differences even on simple transitive sentences (Kutas & King 1996), data that are not predicted by any of the existing capacity theories, because these sentence types require far less capacity to process. Whatever the ultimate subdivisions of verbal working memory turn out to be, we doubt that psycholinguists can afford to ignore the temporal information and processing perspective provided by sentence-level ERP measures. At present, although interesting and provocative, the C&W proposal awaits an exact empirical test. Accounting for the fine structure of syntactic working memory: Similarity-based interference as a unifying principle Richard L. Lewis Department of Computer and Information Science and Center for Cognitive Science, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210. rick@cis.ohio-state.edu www.cis.ohio-state.edu/~rick Abstract: A promising approach to more refined models consistent with the Caplan & Waters hypothesis is based on similarity-based interference, a general principle that applies across working memory domains. This may explain both the fine details of syntactic working memory phenomena and the gross fractionation for which Caplan & Waters have found evidence. Detailed models of syntactic processing that embody similarity-based interference fare well cross-linguistically. Caplan & Waters (C&W) present a compelling case for specialization within verbal working memory. They conclude their target article by looking ahead to additional specification of the hypothesis based on better developed models of working memory. In this commentary I describe some work that takes up the challenge to develop more refined working memory models. I will first discuss briefly some of the phenomena that must be accounted for by a more detailed theory. I will then argue two theoretical points: first, even though working memory may be quite specialized, there are general principles that apply across all kinds of working memory. Second, one of those general principles, similarity-based interference, may explain both the fine details of syntactic working memory phenomena and the gross fractionation and specialization for which C&W have found evidence (Lewis 1993; 1996b; 1998). C&W start their discussion of memory demands in sentence comprehension with the notoriously difficult English double center-embedded relative clause (their sentence [1]) and go on to cite a few of the computational models and linguistic metrics that have been developed to account for the problem with such embeddings. What they did not mention is how surprisingly difficult it has been to produce models and metrics that are empirically adequate. Though it is true that there is a “remarkable degree of similarity in the measurements” that some of the models produce (sect. 1), the vast majority of these measurements do not fare well when considered against a broad range of embeddings cross-linguistically. Furthermore, there has been little independent psychological motivation for the proposed memory structures (e.g., stacks, lookahead buffers) and their associated limitations (see Lewis 1996 for a review). As an example of the kind of empirical hurdle faced by any theory of syntactic working memory, consider a fact established by Cowper (1976) and Gibson (1991) in their seminal work: a metric based on the amount of center-embedding does not account for many difficulty contrasts in English and other languages. Consider sentences (1a) and (1b): 1a. That the food that John ordered tasted good pleased him (Cowper 1976; Gibson 1991). 1b. Der Bauer, der die Kuh, die schlechte Milch gab schlachtete ist krank. the farmer who the cow which bad milk gave killed is sick. “The farmer who killed the cow which gave bad milk is sick” (Hawkins 1994). Though both constructions involve double center-embedding of sentential structures (and sentence [1b] even involves centerembedding of relative clauses), neither causes the comprehension difficulty associated with the classic example cited in the target article. Although increasing center-embedding certainly increases difficulty, another important observation is that increasing the similarity of the embedded constituents increases difficulty, and making constituents more distinct or dissimilar in some way helps processing. This generalization is an old one; it goes back to Miller and Chomsky’s (1963) original self-embedding metric and has been noted several times since (e.g., Bever 1970; Kuno 1974). Why is this observation significant? Similarity-based interference is a principle that holds true of working memory in general. Starting with the early work of Baddeley (1966) and Conrad (1963), which identified a special system relying on phonological codes and covert rehearsal, evidence has accumulated for a wide range of distinct working memory types subject to selective, typespecific interference. The verbal versus visual-spatial distinction is the best known (Baddeley & Hitch 1974; Logie et al. 1990), but there is also evidence for distinct codes for kinesthetic memory (Williams et al. 1969), odor (Walker & John 1984), and sign language (Poizner et al. 1981), to name a few. The robust result across domains is that, when to-be-remembered items are followed by stimuli that are similar along some dimensions, the original items are more quickly forgotten (Shulman 1970; Waugh & Norman 1965). Crucially, similarity-based interference operates within major categories as well. The most familiar within-category effect is the phonological similarity effect. Ordered recall of phonologically similar lists of words, consonants, or nonsense trigrams is worse than with dissimilar lists (Baddeley 1966; Conrad 1963; Wickelgren 1965). Related effects show up in immediate memory for American Sign Language (Poizner et al. 1981) and visual orientation (Magnussen et al. 1991). Building on these results, and the work cited earlier by Gibson (1991), Cowper (1976), and others, I have hypothesized that similarity-based interference as a general principle applies to syntactic working memory as well. Lewis (1993; 1996b) described a BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 105 References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension line psycholinguistic processing and one or more resource pools that may be used for other verbally-mediated tasks. How fine a fractionation of resource systems will be needed to account for capacity limitations in language processing and other verbally mediated cognitive functions is not known at this point (Waters & Caplan 1996c). References Letters “a” and “r” appearing before authors’ initials refer to target article and response, respectively. Abelson, R. A. & Prentice, D. (1997) Contrast tests of interaction hypotheses. Psychological Methods 2:315–28. [AM] Abney, S. P. (1989) A computational model of human parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18:129–44. [aDC] Allik, J. & Bachmann, T. (1983) How bad is icon? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:12–13. [AT] Allik, J. & Help, T. (1985) Comments on S. T. Parker’s “A social-technological model for the evolution of language.” Current Anthropology 26:626. [AT] Almor, A., Kempler, D., MacDonald, M. C., Andersen, E. S. & Tyler, L. K. T. (1998) Why do Alzheimer’s patients have difficulty with pronouns? Working memory, semantics, and pronouns in Alzheimer’s disease (submitted). [MHC] Baddeley, A. D. (1966) Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic, and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 18:362–65. [RLL] (1976) The psychology of memory. Basic Books. [aDC] (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press. [aDC, RAM] (1990a) The development of the concept of working memory: Implications and contributions of neuropsychology. In: Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T. Shallice. Cambridge University Press. [aDC] (1990b) Human memory: Theory and practice. Allyn and Bacon. [AT] (1993) Working memory and conscious awareness. In: Theories of memory, ed. A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway & P. E. Morris. Erlbaum. [aDC] (1995) Working memory. In: The cognitive neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga. MIT Press. [aDC, AT] Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. & Spinnler, H. (1991) The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 114:2521–42. [aDC] Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (1974) Working memory. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 8: Advances in research and theory, ed. G. A. Bower. Academic Press. [arDC, MJK, RLL, JNT] (1994) Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology 8:485–93. [aDC] Baddeley, A. D., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S. D. & Spinnler, H. (1986) Dementia and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A:608–18. [aDC] Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Dronkers, N., Pizzamiglio, L., Wulfeck, B., Hernandez, A., Juarez, L. & Marangolo, P. (1994) Grammatical deficits in patients without agrammatism. Brain and Language 47:400–402. [EB] Baum, S. (1991) Sensitivity to syntactic violations across the age span: Evidence from a word monitoring task. Journal of Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 5:317–28. [aDC] Bayles, K. A. (1982) Language function in senile dementia. Brain and Language 16:265–80. [aDC] Berndt, R., Mitchum, C. & Haendiges, A. (1996) Comprehension of reversible sentences in “agrammatism”: A meta-analysis. Cognition 58:289–308. [aDC] Berwick, R. C. & Weinberg, A. (1984) The grammatical basis of linguistic performance: Language use and acquisition. MIT Press. [aDC] Bever, T. (1970) The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In: Cognition and the development of language, ed. J. R. Hayes. Wiley. [RLL] Blackwell, A. & Bates, E. (1995) Inducing agrammatic profiles in normals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7:228–57. [EB, rDC] Blanken, G., Dittmar, J., Haas, J. C. & Wallesch, C. W. (1987) Spontaneous speech in senile dementia and aphasia: Implications for a neurolinguistic model of language production. Cognition 27:247–74. [aDC] Boland, J. E. & Cutler, A. (1996) Interaction with autonomy: Multiple output models and the inadequacy of the great divide. Cognition 58:309–20. [NJP] Brown, R. G. & Marsden, C. D. (1988) Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 111:323–45. [aDC] 122 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 (1991) Dual task performance and processing resources in normal subjects and patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 114:215–31. [aDC] Buckner, R. & Petersen, S. (1996) What does neuroimaging tell us about the role of prefrontal cortex in memory retrieval? Seminars in the Neurosciences 8:47–55. [RCM] Budd, D., Whitney, P. & Turley, K. J. (1995) Individual differences in working memory strategies for reading expository text. Memory and Cognition 23:735–48. [PW] Caplan, D. (1983) A note on the ‘word-order problem’ in agrammatism. Brain and Language 20:155–65. [HHHJK] (1985) Comments on Fodor’s The modularity of mind: A neo-Cartesian alternative. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:6–7. [aDC] (1987) Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology: An introduction. Cambridge University Press. [aDC] Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Waters, G. S. (in press) Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. [aDC] Caplan, D., Baker, C. & Dehaut, F. (1985) Syntactic determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasia. Cognition 21:117–75. [arDC] Caplan, D. & Futter, C. (1986) Assignment of thematic roles to NPs by an agrammatic aphasic. Brain and Language 27:117–35. [rDC, HHHJK] Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1986) Language deficits and the theory of syntax: A reply to Grodzinsky. Brain and Language 27:168–77. [rDC] (1988) Disorders of syntactic comprehension. MIT Press/Bradford Books. [rDC, HHHJK] Caplan, D., Hildebrandt, N. & Makris, N. (1996) Location of lesions in stroke patients with deficits in syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Brain 119:933–49. [aDC] Caplan, D., Hildebrandt, N. & Waters, G. S. (1994) Interaction of verb selectional restrictions, noun animacy and syntactic form in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 9:549–85. [arDC] (1997) Determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasic patients in sentence- picture matching tasks. Journal of Speech Language and Learning Research 40:542–55. [aDC] Caplan, D. & Waters, G. (1990) Short-term memory and language comprehension: A critical review of the neuropsychological literature. In: Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T. Shallice. Cambridge University Press. [arDC, MJK, AM] (1995) Aphasic disturbances of syntactic comprehension and working memory capacity. Cognitive Neuropsychology 12:637–49. [aDC] (1996) Syntactic processing in sentence comprehension under dual-task conditions in aphasic patients. Language and Cognitive Processes 11:525–51. [arDC, HHHJK] Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. (1988) The role of working memory in language comprehension. In: Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon, ed. D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky. Erlbaum. [aDC] Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A. & Shell, P. (1990) What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review 97:404–31. [AM] Carpenter, P. A., Miyake, A. & Just, M. A. (1994) Working memory constraints in comprehension: Evidence from individual differences, aphasia, and aging. In: Handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. M. Gernsbacher. Academic Press. [AW] Case, R. (1985) Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. Academic Press. [JNT] Case, R., Kurland, M. & Goldberg, J. (1982) Operational efficiency and the growth of short term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33:386–404. [JNT] Cattell, R. B. (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54:1–22. [MJK] Chapman, R. & Miller, J. (1984) SALT: Systematic analysis of language transcripts. University of Wisconsin Press. [SK] Cheung, H. & Kemper, S. (1992) Competing complexity metrics and adults’ production of complex sentences. Applied Psycholinguistics 13:53–76. [SK] Chomsky, N. (1957) Syntactic structures. Mouton. [aDC] (1981) Lectures on government and binding. Foris. [aDC] (1986) Knowledge of language. Praeger. [aDC] (1993) The minimalist program. MIT Press. [aDC] (1997) Language and mind: Current thoughts on ancient problems. Pesquisa Linguistica 3:4. [ZB] Christensen, A. L. (1974) Luria’s neuropsychological investigation: Text. Munksgaard. [aDC] Christiansen, M. H. (1996) Recursive sentence structure in connectionist networks. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual CUNY Conference on Sentence Processing, CUNY, New York. [aDC] Cohen, J. (1978) Partialed products are interactions; partialed powers are curve components. Psychological Bulletin 85:858–66. [AM] (1983) The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement 7:249–53. [AM] References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edition. Erlbaum. [AM] Cohen, L. & Mehler, J. (1996) Click monitoring revisited: An on-line study of sentence comprehension. Memory and Cognition 24:94–102. [aDC] Conrad, R. (1963) Acoustic confusions and memory span for words. Nature 197:1029–30. [RLL] Conway, A. R. A. & Engle, R. W. (1996) Individual differences in working memory capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory 4:577–90. [MJK] Conway, A. R. A., Tuholski, S. W. & Engle, R. W. (1998) Individual differences in working memory: Insights from the counting span task. Paper presented at the Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. [MJK] Cowper, E. A. (1976) Constraints on sentence complexity: A model of syntactic processing. Ph. D. thesis, Brown University. [RLL] Cronbach, L. J. & Snow, R. E. (1977) Aptitudes and instructional models: A handbook for research on interactions. Irvington. [PW] Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. (1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:450–66. [aDC, MJK, JNT, MW] Daneman, M. & Merikle, P. M. (1996) Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin and Review 3:422–33. [aDC, AW] Daneman, M. & Tardif, T. (1987) Working memory and reading skill re-examined. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. M. Coltheart. Erlbaum. [rDC] Davis, C. A. & Ball, H. E. (1989) Effects of age on comprehension of complex sentences in adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32:143–50. [aDC] Della Sala, S. & Logie, R. H. (1993) When working memory does not work: The role of working memory in neuropsychology. In: Handbook of neuropsychology, vol. 8, ed. F. Boller & J. Grafman. Elsevier. [RAM] Demonet, J. F., Fiez, J. A., Paulesu, E., Petersen, S. E. & Zatorre, R. J. (1996) PET studies of phonological processing: A critical reply to Poeppel. Brain and Language 55:352–79. [rDC] Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co. [AT] DeRenzi, E. & Vignolo, L. (1962) The token test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain 85:665–78. [aDC, SAK] Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B. & Dronkers, N. (1998) Deficits in the interpretation of complex sentences in aphasic patients and normals under adverse processing conditions. University of California, San Diego, Center for Research in Language. [EB] Dominey, P. F., Venre-Dominey, J., Brousolle, E. & Jeannerod, M. (1997) Analogical transfer is effective in a serial reaction time task in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for a dissociable sequence learning mechanism. Neuropsychologia 35:1–9. [rDC] Emery, O. B. (1985) Language and aging. Experimental Aging Research (Monograph) 11:3–60. [aDC] (1986) Linguistic decrement in normal aging. Language and Communication 6:47–64. [aDC] Engle, R. (1995) Overview of working memory. Paper presented at the Conference on Cognitive Aging, Atlanta. [aDC] Engle, R. W., Cantor, J. & Carullo, J. J. (1992) Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18:972–92. [PW] Engle, R. W. & Conway, A. R. A. (1998) Working memory and comprehension. In: Working memory and thinking, ed. R. Logie & K. Gilhooly. Psychology Press. [MJK] Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J. & Tuholski, S. W. (in press a) Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In: Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control, ed. A. Miyake & P. Shah. Cambridge University Press. [MJK] Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E. & Conway, A. R. A. (in press b) Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [MJK] Eslinger, P. J. & Damasio, A. R. (1985) Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology 35:1731–41. [AM] Farah, M. J. (1994) Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A critique of the “locality” assumption. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17:43–61. [PW] Feier, C. D. & Gerstman, L. J. (1980) Sentence comprehension abilities throughout the adult life span. Journal of Gerontology 35:722–28. [aDC] Ferreira, F. (1992) Linguistic processing is modular. Talk presented at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Conference, Milwaukee, Wisconsin. [aDC] Ferreira, F., Anes, M. D. & Horine, M. D. (1996) Exploring the use of prosody during language comprehension using the auditory moving window technique. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25:273–90. [aDC] Ferreira, F., Henderson, J. M., Anes, M. D., Weeks, P. A., Jr. & McFarlane, D. K. (1996) Effects of lexical frequency and syntactic complexity in spoken language comprehension: Evidence from the auditory moving window technique. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 22:324–35. [aDC] Fiez, J., Raife, E., Balota, D., Scheartz, J., Raichle, M. & Peterson, S. (1996) A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information. Journal of Neuroscience 16:808–22. [rDC, RCM] Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press. [arDC, NJP, PW] Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. (1974) The psychology of language: An introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. McGraw Hill. [MW] Fodor, J. D. & Inoue, A. (in press) Attach anyway. In: Reanalysis in sentence processing, ed. F. Ferreira & J. D. Fodor. Kluwer. [FF] Ford, M. (1983) A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22:203–18. [aDC] Forster, K. I. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In: Sentence processing, ed. W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker. Erlbaum. [aDC] Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J. & Mehler, J. (1980) Monitoring around the relative clause. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:328–37. [aDC] Frazier, L. (1985) Syntactic complexity. In: Natural language parsing: Psychological, computational, and theoretical perspectives, ed. D. R. Dowty, L. Karttunen & A. M. Zwicky. Cambridge University Press. [SK] (1990) Exploring the architecture of the language-processing system. In: Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives, ed. G. T. M. Altmann. MIT Press/Bradford Books. [aDC] Frazier, L. & Clifton, C. (1996) Construal. MIT Press. [arDC] Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. (1982) Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 143:178–210. [aDC] Freidrich, F. J. (1990) Multiple phonological representations and verbal short-term memory. In: The neuropsychology of short-term memory, ed. T. Shallice & G. Vallar. Cambridge University Press. [aDC] Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A. & von Cramon, D. Y. (in press) First-pass versus second-pass parsing processes in a Wernicke’s and a Broca’s aphasic: Electrophysiological evidence for a double dissociation. Brain and Language. [SAK] Frick, R. W. (1995) Accepting the null hypothesis. Memory and Cognition 23:132–38. [AM] Friston, K. J., Price, C. J., Fletcher, P., Moore, C., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Dolan, R. J. (1996) The trouble with cognitive subtraction. NeuroImage 4:97–104. [KJF] Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993) Working memory and language. Erlbaum. [aDC, AM] Gibson, E. (1991) A computational theory of human linguistic processing: Memory limitations and processing breakdown. Ph. D. thesis, Carnegie Mellon University, CMU-CMT-91–125. [RLL] (in press) Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition. [aDC, EG] Gibson, E., Schutze, C. & Salomon, C. (1996) The relationship between the frequency and the processing of complex linguistic structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25:59–92. [SK] Gibson, E. & Thomas, J. (1996) The processing complexity of English centerembedded and self-embedded structures. In: Proceedings of the NELS 26 workshop on language processing: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, ed. C. Schutze. MIT Press. [aDC] Gick, M. L., Craik, F. I. M. & Morris, R. G. (1988) Task complexity and age differences in working memory. Memory and Cognition 16:353–61. [aDC] Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E. & Ivry, R. (1995) Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7:497–510. [aDC] Grafton, S. T., Mazziota, J. C., Presty, S., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Phelps, M. E. (1992) Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET. Journal of Neuroscience 12:2542–48. [aDC] Grober, E. & Bang, S. (1995) Sentence comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Developmental Neuropsychology 11:95–107. [aDC] Grodzinsky, Y. (1990) Theoretical perspectives on language deficits. MIT Press. [HHHJK] Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., Vernon, G. & Hurtig, H. I. (1991) Sentence comprehension and praxis deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 41:1620–28. [aDC] Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S. & Hurtig, H. (1992) Sentence BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 123 References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and memory. Brain and Language 42:347–84. [aDC] Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H. & Phillips, S. (1998) Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(6):803–65. [GA] Hart, S. (1988) Language and dementia: A review. Psychological Medicine 18:99–112. [aDC] Hawkins, J. A. (1994) A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge University Press. [RLL] Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, K. & Schindler, A. G. (1985) Language disintegration in dementia: Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language 25:117–33. [aDC] Hildebrandt, N., Caplan, D. & Evans, K. (1987) The man “left” without a trace: A case study of aphasic processing of empty categories. Cognitive Neuropsychology 4:257–302. [aDC] Hitch, G. J. & Baddeley, A. D. (1976) Verbal reasoning and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28:603–21. [JNT] Horn, J. L. (1980) Concepts of intellect in relation to learning and adult development. Intelligence 4:285–317. [MJK] Horn, J. L. & McArdle, J. J. (1992) A practical and theoretical guide to measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental Aging Research 18:117–44. [SK] Humphreys, L. G. & Fleishman, A. (1974) Pseudo-orthogonal and other analysis of variance designs involving individual-differences variable. Journal of Educational Psychology 66:464–72. [AM] Illes, J. (1989) Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of neurodegenerative disease: Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s. Brain and Language 37:628–42. [aDC] Irigaray, L. (1973) Le langage des dements. Mouton. [aDC] Irle, E. (1987) Lesion size and recovery of function: Some new perspectives. Brain Research Reviews 12:307–20. [AT] (1990) An analysis of the correlation of lesion size, localization and behavioral effects in 283 published studies of cortical and subcortical lesions in old-world monkeys. Brain Research Reviews 15:11–213. [AT] Johnson, M. (1996) Resource sensitivity in grammar and processing. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, CUNY, New York. [aDC] Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Awh, E., Minoshima, S. & Monton, M. A. (1993) Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363:623–25. [aDC] Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99(1):122–49. [GA, arDC, EG, MJK, HHHJK, AM, MW, PW] Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A. & Keller, T. (1996) Working memory: New frontiers of evidence. Psychological Review 103:773–75. [aDC] Just, M. A. Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F. & Thulborn, K. R. (1996b) Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274:114–16. [aDC] Kahneman, D. (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall. [AW] Kane, M. J. & Engle, R. W. (1988) Full frontal fluidity: Working memory capacity, attention, general fluid abilities, and the prefrontal cortex (submitted). [MJK] Kapur, N. (1996) Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-behaviour research. A critical review. Brain 119:1775–90. [AT] Kemper, S. (1986) Imitation of complex syntactic constructions by elderly adults. Applied Psycholinguistics 7:277–87. [aDC] (1987) Life-span changes in syntactic complexity. Journal of Gerontology 42:23238. [Sk] (1988) Geriatric psycholinguistics: Syntactic limitations of oral and written language. In: Language, memory, and aging, ed. L. L. Light & D. M. Burke. Cambridge University Press. [aDC, SK] Kemper, S., Anagnopoulos, C., Lyons, K. & Heberlein, W. (1994) Speech accomodations to dementia. Journal of Gerontology 49:223–29. [aDC] Kemper, S., Kynette, D. & Norman, S. (1992) Age differences in spoken language. In: Everyday memory and aging, ed. R. West & J. Sinnott. Springer-Verlag. [SK] Kemper, S., Kynette, D., Rash, S., O’Brien, K. & Sprott, R. (1989) Life-span changes to adults’ language: Effects of memory and genre. Applied Psycholinguistics 10:49–66. [SK] Kemper, S., LaBarge, E., Ferraro, F. R., Cheung, H., Cheung, H. & Storandt, M. (1993) On the preservation of syntax in Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence from written sentences. Archives of Neurology 50:81–86. [aDC] Kemper, S. & Rash, S. (1988) Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Cambridge University Press. [aDC] Kemper, S., Snowdon, D. A. & Greiner, L. H. (1996) Linguistic ability across the life span: Findings from the Nun study. Paper presented at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta. [aDC] Kempler, D., Curtiss, S. & Jackson, C. (1987) Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30:343–50. [aDC] 124 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 Kemtes, K. A. & Kemper, S. (1996) Younger and older adults’ working memory and on- line processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences. Paper presented at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta. [aDC] (1998) Age-based construct validation of working memory. Unpublished manuscript. [SK] Kilborn, K. (1991) Selective impairment of grammatical morphology due to induced stress in normal listeners: Implications for aphasia. Brain and Language 41:275–88. [EB, rDC] King, J. & Just, M. A. (1991) Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30:580–602. [aDC, PW] King, J. W. & Kutas, M. (1995) Who did what and when: Using word- and clauselevel ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7(3):376–95. [MK] Kintsch, W. & Keenan, J. M. (1973) Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of the propositions in the base structure of sentences. Cognitive Psychology 5:257–74. [SK] Kirshner, H. S. (1982) Language disorders in dementia. In: The neurology of aphasia, ed. H. S. Kirshner & F. R. Freeman. Swets and Zeitlinger. [aDC] Kluender, R. & Kutas, M. (1993) Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes 8:573–633. [aDC] Kolk, H. H. J. & van Grunsven, M. F. (1985) Agrammatism as a variable phenomenon. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2:347–84. [HHHJK] Kolk, H. H. J. & Weijts, M. (1996) Judgements of semantic anomaly in agrammatic patients: Argument movement, syntactic complexity and the use of heuristics. Brain and Language 54:86–135. [HHHJK] Kontiola, P., Laaksonen, R., Sulkava, R. & Erkinjuntii, T. (1990) Pattern of language impairment is different in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-infarct dementia. Brain and Language 38:364–83. [aDC] Koster, J. (1975) Dutch as an SOV language. Linguistic Analysis 1:111–36. [HHHJK] Krueger, L. E. (1994) Mind your p’s and q’s when accepting or rejecting the null hypothesis. Paper presented at the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, MO. [aDC] Kuno, S. (1974) The position of relative clauses and conjunctions. Linguistic Inquiry 5:117–36. [RLL] Kutas, M. & King, J. W. (1996) The potentials for basic sentence processing: Differentiating integrative processes. In: Attention and performance XVI, ed. I. Ikeda & J. L. McClelland. MIT Press. [MK] Kynette, D. & Kemper, S. (1986) Aging and the loss of grammatical forms. Language and Communication 6:65–72. [SK] Lalami, L., Marblestone, K., Schuster, S., Andersen, E., Kempler, D., Tyler, L. & MacDonald, M. (1996) On-line vs. off-line sentence processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at the Meeting of Cognitive Aging, Atlanta. [aDC] Lee, L. (1974) Developmental sentence analysis. Northwestern University Press. [SK] Lees, A. J. & Smith, E. (1983) Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain 106:257–70. [aDC] Lehto, J. (1996) Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A:29–50. [AM] Lewis, R. L. (1993) An architecturally-based theory of human sentence comprehension. Ph. D. thesis, School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon University (CMU-CS- 93–226). [RLL] (1996a) A theory of grammatical but unacceptable embeddings.Unpublished. [aDC] Lewis, R. L. (1996b) Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25:93–115. [SK, RLL] (1998) Working memory in sentence processing: Retroactive and proactive interference in parsing. Talk presented at CUNY 98, New Brunswick, NJ. [RLL] Lieberman, P., Friedman, J. & Feldman, L. S. (1990) Syntax comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 178:360–66. [aDC] Light, L. L. (1990) Interactions between memory and language in old age. In: Handbook of the psychology of aging, 3rd edition, ed. J. E. Birren & K. W. Schale. Academic Press. [aDC, AW] Logie, R., Zucco, G. M. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990) Interference with visual shortterm memory. Acta Psychologica 75:55–74. [RLL] Luria, A. R. (1974) Neiropskhologiya pamyati. Narusheniya pamyati pri lokal ‘nykh porazheniakh mozga. [Neuropsychology of memory. Memory disturbances in local brain lesions]. Pedagogika. [AT] MacDonald, M. C. (1997) Language and Cognitive Processes: Special Issue on Lexical Representations and Sentence Processing 12:121–399. [aDC] MacDonald, M. C. & Christiansen, M. H. (1998) Individual differences without working memory: A reply to Just & Carpenter and Waters & Caplan. (submitted). [MHC, NJP] References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992) Working memory contraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology 24:56–98. [arDC] MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994) The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101:676–703. [aDC, AM] Magnussen, S., Greenlee, M. W., Asplund, R. & Dyrnes, S. (1991) Stimulusspecific mechanisms of visual short-term memory. Vision Research 31:1213–19. [RLL] Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987) Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition 25:71–102. [aDC] Marslen-Wilson, W. D. & Tyler, L. K. (1987) Against modularity. In: Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding, ed. J. L. Garfield. MIT Press. [NJP] Martin, R., R. (1995) Working memory doesn’t work: A critique of Miyake et al.’s capacity theory of aphasic comprehension deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology 12:623–36. [RCM] Martin, R. & Feher, E. (1990) The consequences of reduced memory span for comprehension of semantic versus syntactic information. Brain and Language 38:1–20. [aDC, SAK] Martin, R. C. & Romani, C. (1994) Verbal working memory and sentence processing: A multiple component view. Neuropsychology 8:506–23. [rDC, RCM] Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. & Yaffee, L. (1994) Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language 33:83–111. [RCM] Mattys, S. L. (1997) The use of time during lexical processing and segmentation: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 4:310–29. [AW] Maxwell, S. E. & Delaney, H. D. (1993) Bivariate median splits and spurious statistical significance. Psychological Bulletin 113:181–90. [AM] Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D. & Dill, C. A. (1984) Another look at ANCOVA versus blocking. Psychological Bulletin 95:136–47. [AM] McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1987a) The double dissociation of shortterm memory for lists and sentences. Brain 110:1545–64. [RAM] (1987b) Understanding: A function of short-term memory? Brain 110:1565–78. [RAM] (1990a) Cognitive neuropsychology: A clinical introduction. Academic Press. [RAM] (1990b) Short-term memory: A phenomenon in search of a function? In: Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T. Shallice. Cambridge University Press. [RAM] McClelland, G. H. (1997) Optimal design in psychological research. Psychological Methods 2:3–19. [AM] McClelland, G. H. & Judd, C. M. (1993) Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin 114:376–90. [AM] Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K. & Friederici, A. D. (1995) Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition 23(4):477–94. [MK] Miller, G. A. & Chomsky, N. (1963) Finitary models of language users. In: Handbook of mathematical psychology, vol. II, ed. R. R. Bush & E. Galanter. Wiley. [RLL] Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. & Just, M. (1994) A capacity approach to syntactic comprehension disorders: Making normal adults perform like aphasic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology 11:671–717. [EB, aDC, AM] Morris, R. G. (1984) Dementia and the functioning of the articulatory loop system. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1:143–57. [aDC] (1986) Short-term forgetting in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Cognitive Neuropsychology 3:77–97. [aDC] (1987) Articulatory rehearsal in Alzheimer type dementia. Brain and Language 30:351–62. [aDC] Morris, R. G. & Baddeley, A. D. (1988) Primary and working memory functioning in Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 10:279–96. [aDC] Morris, R. G., Craik, F. I. M. & Gick, M. L. (1990) Age differences in working memory tasks: The role of secondary memory and the central executive system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42A:67–86. [aDC] Morris, R. G., Gick, M. L. & Craik, F. I. M. (1988) Processing resources and age differences in working memory. Memory and Cognition 16:362–66. [aDC] Müller, H. M., King, J. W. & Kutas, M. (1997) Event-related potentials elicited by spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research 5(3):193–203. [MK] Münte, T. F., Schwirtz, O., Wieringa, B. M., Matzke, M. & Johannes, S. (1997) Electrophysiology of complex sentences: Word and sentence-level ERPs. EEG-EMG-Zeitschrift für Elektroenzephalographie, Elektromyographie und verwandte Gebiete 28(1):11–17. [MK] Natsopoulos, D., Katsarou, Z., Bostantzopoulo, S., Grouios, G., Mentanopoulos, G. & Logothetis, J. (1991) Strategies in comprehension of relative clauses in Parkinsonian patients. Cortex 27:255–68. [aDC] Naucler, N., Wulfeck, B. & Bates, E. (1998) A developmental study of complex sentence interpretation abilities (Technical Report CND-9803). University of California, San Diego, Project in Cognitive and Neural Development. [EB] Navon, D. (1984) Resources – A theoretical soup stone. Psychological Review 91:216–34. [rDC, MHC] Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I. & Garrett, M. F. (1991) Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3:151–65. [aDC] Nõmm, M. (1996) The development of short-memory span: Further evidence on the contribution of long-term memory. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Tartu University. [AT] Norman, S., Kemper, S., Kynette, D., Cheung, H. & Anagnopoulous, C. (1991) Syntactic complexity and adults running memory span. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 46:346–51. [aDC] Obler, L. K., Fein, D., Nicholas, M. & Albert, M. L. (1991) Auditory comprehension and aging: Decline in syntactic processing. Applied Psycholinguistics 12:433–52. [aDC] Pearlmutter, N. J. & MacDonald, M. C. (1992) Plausibility and syntactic ambiguity resolution. In: Proceedings of the 14th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum. [NJP] (1995) Individual differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 34:521–42. [arDC, NJP] Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E. & Evans, A. (1993) Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Neurobiology 90:878–82. [arDC] Phillips, C. B., Zeki, S. & Barlow, H. B. (1984) Localization of function in the cerebral cortex. Past, present and future. Brain 107:327–61. [KJF] Plaut, D., McClelland, J., Seidenberg, M. & Patterson, K. (1996) Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103:56–115. [rDC] Poizner, H., Bellugi, U. & Tweney, R. D. (1981) Processing of formational, semantic, and iconic information in American sign language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7:1146–59. [RLL] Posner, M. I., Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T. & Raichle, M. E. (1988) Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240:1627–31. [aDC] Potter, M. C. (1993) Very short-term conceptual memory. Memory and Cognition 21:156–61. [AW] Price, C. J. & Friston, K. J. (1997) Cognitive conjunction: A new approach to brain activation experiments. NeuroImage 5:261–70. [KJF] Rakic, P. (1988) Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241:170–76. [aDC] Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Head, D., Dupois, J. H. & Acker, J. D. (1998) Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: Evidence from structural MRI. Neuropsychology 12:95–106. [AW] Restle, F. (1970) Theory of serial pattern learning: Structural trees. Psychological Review 77:481–95. [AT] Roberts, R. M. & Corkin, S. (1998) What do working memory tasks measure? Manuscript under review, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT. [EG] Roberts, R. M., Hooker, C. & Gibson, E. (1997) Sentence memory capacity and working memory. Poster presented at the 10th CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Santa Monica, CA, March 1997. [EG] Rochon, E. & Saffran, E. M. (1995) A semantic contribution to sentence comprehension impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented to the Academy of Aphasia, San Diego. [aDC] Rochon, E., Waters, G. S. & Caplan, D. (1994) Sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language 46:329–49. [aDC] Rosenberg, S. & Abbeduto, L. (1987) Indicators of linguistic competence in the peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults. Applied Psycholinguistics 8:19–32. [SK] Rosenthal, R. R. & Rosnow, R. L. (1985) Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. Cambridge University Press. [AM] Salthouse, T. A. (1991) Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Erlbaum. [aDC] Schneider, W. (1993) Varieties of working memory as seen in biology and in connectionist/control architectures. Memory and Cognition 21:184–92. [AT] Schneider, W. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing I.: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review 84:1–66. [aDC] Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S. M. & Saffran, E. M. (1979) Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language 7:277–306. [aDC] Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M. & Marin, O. S. M. (1980) The word order problem in agrammatism I: Comprehension. Brain and Language 10:249–67. [RAM] Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (1989) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96:523–68. [rDC] Shah, P. & Miyake, A. (1996) The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125:4–27. [aDC] Shallice, T. & Burgess, P. (1993) Supervisory control of action and thought selection. In: Attention: Selection, awareness, and control, ed. A. Baddeley & L. Weiscrantz. Clarendon Press. [MJK] BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 125 References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension (1996) The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London 351:1405–12. [HHHJK] Shallice, T. & Warrington, E. K. (1970) Independent functioning of the verbal memory stores: A neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 22:261–73. [RAM] Sherman, J. C., Schweickert, J., Growdon, J. & Corkin, S. (1988) Sentence comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Paper presented at the 18th Annual Meeting of the Society of Neuroscience, Toronto. [aDC] Shiffrin, R. M. & Schneider, W. (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review 84:127–90. [aDC] Shulman, H. (1970) Similarity effects in short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin 75:399–414. [RLL] Smith, S. (1989) Syntactic comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease.Paper presented at the Academy of Aphasia conference, Santa Fe, New Mexico [aDC] Snowdon, D. A., Kemper, S. J., Mortimer, J. A., Greiner, L. H., Wekstein, D. R. & Markesbery, W. R. (1996) Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association 275:528–32. [aDC] Squire, L. R. & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991) The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 253:1380–86. [aDC] Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: Extension of Donder’s method. Acta Psychologia 30:276–315. [KJF] Stevens, K., Kempler, D., Andersen, E. & MacDonald, M. C. (1996) Preserved use of semantic and syntactic context in Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at the Meeting of Cognitive Aging, Atlanta. [aDC] Stine, E. A. L. (1995) Aging and the distribution of resources in working memory. In: Age differences in word and language processing, ed. P. Allen & T. Bashore. North-Holland. [SK] Stowe, L. (1998) The cognitive function of the inferior frontal lobe in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience S126. [rDC] Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Rauch, S. (1996) Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language 52:452–73. [aDC] Sturt, P. & Crocker, M. W. (1996) Monotonic syntactic processing: A crosslinguistic study of attachment and reanalysis. Language and Cognitive Processing 11:449–94. [arDC] Swinney, D. & Love, T. (1998) Language input rate as a parameter on structural processing. Proceedings of the 11th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New Brunswick, NJ, March 1998. [MW] Swinney, D. & Zurif, E. (1996) Syntactic processing in aphasia. Brain and Language 50:225–39. [aDC] Swinney, D., Zurif, E., Prather, P. & Love, T. (1996) Neurological distribution of processing resources underlying language comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8:174–84. [aDC] Tabor, W., Juliano, C. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1997) Parsing in a dynamical system: A attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 12:211–71. [arDC] Tabossi, P., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., McRae, K. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994) Semantic effects on syntactic ambiguity resolution: Evidence for a constraintbased resolution process. In: Attention and performance XV, ed. C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch. MIT Press. [NJP] Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K. & Sedivy, J. (1995) The interaction of visual and verbal information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268:1632–34. [PW] Taylor, A. E., Saint-Cyr, J. A. & Lang, A. E. (1986) Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 109:845–86. [aDC] Tim, P. A. & Wingfield, A. (1993) Is speech special? Perception and recall of spoken language in complex environments. In: Adult information processing: Limits on loss, ed. J. Cerella, J. Rybash, W. Hoyer & M. L. Commons. Academic Press. [AW] Tirre, W. & Pena, C. (1992) Investigation of functional working memory in the reading span test. Journal of Educational Psychology 84:462–72. [arDC] Titchener, E. B. (1908) Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. Macmillan. [AW] Tomoeda, C. K., Bayles, K. A., Boone, D. R., Kaszniak, A. W. & Slauson, T. J. (1990) Speech rate and syntactic complexity effects on the auditory comprehension of Alzheimer patients. Journal of Communication Disorders 23:151–61. [aDC] Towse, J. N. (1998) On random generation and the central executive of working memory. British Journal of Psychology 89(1):77–101. [JNT] Towse, J. N. & Hitch, G. J. (1995) Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space in tests of working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 48A:108–24. [rDC, JNT] Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J. & Hutton, U. (1998) A re-evaluation of working memory capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language 39:195–217. [JNT] 126 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1 Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Garnsey, S. M. (1994) Semantic influence on syntactic processing: Use of thematic information in syntactic disambiguation. Journal of Memory and Language 33:265–312. [aDC, NJP] Turner, M. & Engle, R. W. (1989) Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language 28:127–54. [aDC, MJK] Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J. & Pinker, S. (1997) A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:266–76. [ZB, rDC] Vallar, G. & Shallice, T., eds. (1990) Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory. Cambridge University Press. [aDC] Waldstein, R. & Baum, S. (1992) The influence of syntactic and semantic context on word monitoring latencies in normal aging. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology 6:217–22. [aDC] Walenski, M., Figueroa, S. & Swinney, D. (1997) The sentence memory task: A methodological investigation. Proceedings of the 10th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Santa Monica, CA, March 1997. [MW] Walker, H. A. & John, E. E. (1984) Interference and facilitation in short-term memory for odors. Perception and Psychophysics 36:508–14. [RLL] Wanner, E. & Maratsos, M. (1978) An ATN approach to comprehension. In: Linguistic theory and psychological reality, ed. M. M. Halle, G. Miller & J. Bresnan. MIT Press. [aDC] Warrington, E. K. (1975) The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:637–57. [SAK] Warrington, E. K., Logue, V. & Pratt, R. T. C. (1971) The anatomical localisation of selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia 9:377–87. [RAM] Waters, G. & Caplan, D. (1996a) The capacity theory of sentence comprehension: A reply to Just and Carpenter. Psychological Review 103:761–72. [aDC] (1996b) Processing resource capacity and the comprehension of garden path sentences. Memory and Cognition 24:342–55. [aDC, AM] (1996c) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49:51–79. [arDC] (1997) Working memory and on-line sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 26:377–400. [aDC] (submitted) The effect of a digit load on syntactic processing in subjects with high and low working memory capacity. [aDC] Waters, G., Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1987) Working memory and written sentence comprehension. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. M. Coltheart. Erlbaum. [arDC] (1991) On the structure of verbal short-term memory and its functional role in sentence comprehension: Evidence from neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8(2):81–126. [arDC] Waters, G. S., Caplan, D. & Rochon, E. (1995) Processing capacity and sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology 12:1–30. [aDC, AM] Waters, G. S., Rochon, E. & Caplan, D. (in press) Task demands and sentence comprehension in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Brain and Language. [aDC] Waugh, N. C. & Norman, D. A. (1965) Primary memory. Psychological Review 72:89–104. [RLL] Wickelgren, W. (1965) Acoustic similarity and retroactive interference in shortterm memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 4:53–61. [RLL] Williams, H. L., Beaver, W. S., Spence, M. T. & Rundell, O. H. (1969) Digital and kinesthetic memory with interpolated information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology 80(3):530–36. [RLL] Wright, R. E. (1981) Aging, divided attention, and processing capacity. Journal of Gerontology 36(5):605–14. [aDC] Yngve, V. (1960) A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104:444–66. [SK] Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E. & Gjedde, A. (1992) Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science 256:846–49. [rDC] Zeki, S., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Friston, K. J., Kennard, C. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991) A direct demonstration of functional specialisation in human visual cortex. Neuroscience 11:641–49. [KJF] Zurif, E., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Solomon, J. & Bushell, C. (1993) An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language 45:448–64. [aDC] Zurif, E., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Wingfield, A. & Brownell, H. (1995) The allocation of memory resources during sentence comprehension: Evidence from the elderly. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24:165–82. [aDC]