Complexity effects are found in all relative

advertisement
Commentary/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
memory problems should at least be extended or discussed in a
different context. In particular, the question arises of whether sentence comprehension deficits in aphasic or central nervous system
(CNS) dysfunction patients are solely a consequence of a specific
working memory resource deficit and/or a computational deficit.
In other words, can there be an independent resource deficit without some interference from a computational deficit at the “postinterpretive” processing level? We will present data from an aphasic patient with verbal working memory deficits to broaden this
discussion.
Patient H. G., a 45-year-old male, suffered an ischemic stroke
in 1996. T2-weighted images in the axial plane taken about half a
year after the stroke revealed an extended lesion in the left temporoparietal region and a small lesion in the left posterior frontal
area. The temporoparietal lesion encloses (1) the left Heschl
gyrus, (2) the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus, (3) the
supramarginal gyrus, and (4) the occipital gyri. The frontal lesion
involves the middle portion of the praecentral gyrus and the posterior part of the frontal gyrus (F2p). The inferior frontal gyrus
(F3) is spared. The functional deficit due to the frontal lesion
could be twofold: (1) an articulatory deficit due to the lesion in the
praecentral gyrus or (2) a verbal working memory deficit due to
the lesion in the posterior F2 area. The lesioned supramarginal
gyrus in the temporoparietal region should crucially affect the
phonological short-term memory (STM; see Warrington 1975),
with intact rehearsal processes (see unaffected F3 region). In
terms of the functional–neuroanatomical correlates, H. G. should
show the classical picture of an aphasic patient with an STM
deficit and potentially a syntactic processing deficit during sentence comprehension.
In a first step, we tested H. G. for deficits in the articulatory rehearsal and phonological storage system. Results indicate no STM
deficit in H. G. Tests for verbal working memory capacities such
as verbal word span (span: 2– 3) and an adapted aphasic reading
span task (span: 1) as well as digit spans (forward and backward),
however, indicate a severe verbal working memory deficit in H. G.
Next, H. G. was tested with several sentence-comprehension paradigms engaging syntactic complexities (word category violations,
subject–object relative clauses, subordinate clause violations).
Furthermore, a combination of syntactic complexity with increasing propositional content was applied. We used both sentence
picture-matching tasks and auditory and visual comprehension
tasks in the two test batteries. Results indicate that H. G. did not
show a significant deficit in sentence comprehension at the “interpretive” or “post-interpretive” level of processing. Thus, if
working memory impairments exclusively influence “post-interpretive” processing, as proposed by C&W (sects. 3.3 and 3.4), H.
G.’s data provide evidence against this hypothesis. Most strikingly,
he did not show any effect of increasing propositional content.
One example of this effect is that he scored 100% on a German
version of the Token Test (DeRenzi & Vignolo 1962; Martin & Feher 1992) and in a judgment task with increasing propositional
content.
To ensure that the “untimed” accuracy measures of sentence
comprehension were not due to a ceiling effect, we proceeded to
test H. G. in a combined syntactic and semantic judgment paradigm utilizing event-related potentials (ERPs). We assumed that
if the verbal working memory deficit of H. G. has any effect on
sentence comprehension it should be visible in the on-line measurements of ERP components specific to syntactic and semantic
processes (see also Friederici et al., in press). Even though this
judgment paradigm did not use the most complex syntactic structures nor a dual-task manipulation with additional load, H. G. displayed an interesting picture. Whereas the performance data were
in the normal range, H. G. showed that on-line computation of
syntactic structure and semantic integration was influenced by the
input parameters. The evaluation of syntactic violations (word category errors) and semantic violations (selectional restriction errors) and correct sentences in connected speech did not elicit any
of the expected ERP components in H. G. A similar picture
104
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
emerged in the visual modality with a fast presentation rate (SOA:
500 msec). In a long SOA (1100 msec) visual manipulation, the
N400 (onset: 350 msec) was elicited to semantic violations and a
delayed P600 (onset: 1,000 msec) was elicited to syntactic violations. These results, although by no means exhaustive in the discussion of resource-capacity models, give evidence that reduced
verbal working memory capacities might go hand in hand with a
temporal deficit.
In-line measures of syntactic processing
using event-related brain potentials
Marta Kutas and Jonathan W. King
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La
Jolla, CA 92093-0515. kutas@cogsci.ucsd.edu king@cogsci.ucsd.edu
Abstract: Scalp-recorded event-related potential (ERP) measures of
reading and listening have been proved more sensitive to the time course
of syntactic processing than the chronometric and behavioral data described by Caplan & Waters. ERP studies using sentences containing relative clauses indicate that there are individual differences in syntactic processing that appear at the earliest theoretically relevant time points and
are attributable to working memory operations.
Leaving aside a discussion of Caplan & Water’s (C&W’s) specific
proposal that there are two distinct verbal working memory systems, we note that C&W base much of their argument on
chronometric and behavioral data from the processing of sentences containing subject-relative (SR) and object-relative (OR)
clauses. The focus on the contrast between SR and OR clauses is
clear; arguments concerning limitations in working memory capacity permeate the experimental literature on this topic. The decision to focus on reaction time measures, however, is less clear to
us. Although a host of measures show consistent chronometric differences between the two sentences, these effects are generally
observed substantially after the point in the sentence where they
would be expected owing to a difference in working memory storage demands per se. In the following examples,
OR: The reporter who the senator harshly attacked admitted the error.
SR: The reporter who harshly attacked the senator admitted the error.
the noun phrase “the senator” in the OR sentence marks the beginning of the differential working memory load between the two
sentence types. However, RT differences are not seen until the
end of the relative clause or even later, on the main clause verb.
Thus, one might question the sensitivity of the chronometric measures on which C&W base their arguments, at least if monotonic
increases in memory load lead to monotonic increases in processing time, as many assume.
There is, however, one measure of brain processing that shows
differential activity for OR versus SR sentences starting precisely
where one would expect from the usual working memory arguments, namely, event-related potentials (ERPs). While C&W
miscite who found out what and when, they do note that the processing of OR sentences is associated with a greater negativity
than the processing of SR sentences. This electrophysiological
pattern has been observed repeatedly during word-by-word reading in English (King & Kutas 1995) and German (Mecklinger et
al. 1995; Münte et al. 1997) as well as in connected speech (Müller
et al. 1997). This ERP difference between subject and object relatives is large and is more pronounced over anterior electrode
sites; with visual materials, it is also left-lateralized. (Indeed, it is
this topographic characteristic of the data that C&W highlighted
when referring to ERP results in their section on the anatomical
locus of verbal working memory processes.) But, to emphasize the
point, the electrophysiological data show a much earlier point of
divergence. Moreover, King and Kutas (1995) found that the
Commentary/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
ERPs to the main-clause verb in both SR and OR sentences
showed a greater left anterior negativity than the second verb in
sentences that did not contain relative clauses. Although tempted,
we will refrain from arguing from this pattern of effects that the
insensitivity of the behavioral reaction time data suggests they are
more likely to reflect “post-integrative” effects.
C&W also claim that there is no (behavioral) evidence of an interaction between sentence complexity and working memory capacity (WMC), and, because there is no evidence that sentence
complexity has larger effects on low-WMC subjects, C&W conclude that RT data from the SR–OR contrast support their proposal for a verbal working memory subsystem specialized for syntactic analysis. Setting aside their interpretation of the behavioral
data, we would like to point out that the ERP studies cited above
also include notable demonstrations of individual differences in
syntactic processing. In both written and spoken English, good
comprehenders show large ERP differences between SR and OR
sentence types, whereas poorer comprehenders show almost none
(see, e.g., King & Kutas 1995; Müller et al. 1997). In good comprehenders, subject relatives elicit a frontal positivity, whereas object relatives show more negative deflections from this pattern. In
poor comprehenders, the responses elicited by both the SR and
the OR sentences are almost identical to each other and also identical to the response elicited by OR sentences in good comprehenders. It is likely that these good comprehenders would score
higher on many (possibly language specific) WMC measures than
would poor comprehenders. More sensitive measures thus do
show the kind of individual differences C&W failed to find. Additionally, however, we find that good and poor comprehenders
show ERP differences even on simple transitive sentences (Kutas
& King 1996), data that are not predicted by any of the existing capacity theories, because these sentence types require far less capacity to process.
Whatever the ultimate subdivisions of verbal working memory
turn out to be, we doubt that psycholinguists can afford to ignore
the temporal information and processing perspective provided by
sentence-level ERP measures. At present, although interesting
and provocative, the C&W proposal awaits an exact empirical test.
Accounting for the fine structure of syntactic
working memory: Similarity-based
interference as a unifying principle
Richard L. Lewis
Department of Computer and Information Science and Center for Cognitive
Science, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210.
rick@cis.ohio-state.edu
www.cis.ohio-state.edu/~rick
Abstract: A promising approach to more refined models consistent with
the Caplan & Waters hypothesis is based on similarity-based interference,
a general principle that applies across working memory domains. This may
explain both the fine details of syntactic working memory phenomena and
the gross fractionation for which Caplan & Waters have found evidence.
Detailed models of syntactic processing that embody similarity-based interference fare well cross-linguistically.
Caplan & Waters (C&W) present a compelling case for specialization within verbal working memory. They conclude their target article by looking ahead to additional specification of the hypothesis
based on better developed models of working memory. In this
commentary I describe some work that takes up the challenge to
develop more refined working memory models. I will first discuss
briefly some of the phenomena that must be accounted for by a
more detailed theory. I will then argue two theoretical points:
first, even though working memory may be quite specialized, there
are general principles that apply across all kinds of working memory. Second, one of those general principles, similarity-based interference, may explain both the fine details of syntactic working
memory phenomena and the gross fractionation and specialization
for which C&W have found evidence (Lewis 1993; 1996b; 1998).
C&W start their discussion of memory demands in sentence
comprehension with the notoriously difficult English double
center-embedded relative clause (their sentence [1]) and go on to
cite a few of the computational models and linguistic metrics that
have been developed to account for the problem with such embeddings. What they did not mention is how surprisingly difficult
it has been to produce models and metrics that are empirically adequate. Though it is true that there is a “remarkable degree of similarity in the measurements” that some of the models produce
(sect. 1), the vast majority of these measurements do not fare well
when considered against a broad range of embeddings cross-linguistically. Furthermore, there has been little independent psychological motivation for the proposed memory structures (e.g.,
stacks, lookahead buffers) and their associated limitations (see
Lewis 1996 for a review).
As an example of the kind of empirical hurdle faced by any theory of syntactic working memory, consider a fact established by
Cowper (1976) and Gibson (1991) in their seminal work: a metric
based on the amount of center-embedding does not account for
many difficulty contrasts in English and other languages. Consider
sentences (1a) and (1b):
1a. That the food that John ordered tasted good pleased him (Cowper
1976; Gibson 1991).
1b. Der Bauer, der die Kuh, die schlechte Milch gab schlachtete ist
krank.
the farmer who the cow which bad milk gave killed is sick.
“The farmer who killed the cow which gave bad milk is sick” (Hawkins
1994).
Though both constructions involve double center-embedding of
sentential structures (and sentence [1b] even involves centerembedding of relative clauses), neither causes the comprehension
difficulty associated with the classic example cited in the target article.
Although increasing center-embedding certainly increases difficulty, another important observation is that increasing the similarity of the embedded constituents increases difficulty, and making constituents more distinct or dissimilar in some way helps
processing. This generalization is an old one; it goes back to Miller
and Chomsky’s (1963) original self-embedding metric and has
been noted several times since (e.g., Bever 1970; Kuno 1974).
Why is this observation significant? Similarity-based interference is a principle that holds true of working memory in general. Starting with the early work of Baddeley (1966) and Conrad
(1963), which identified a special system relying on phonological
codes and covert rehearsal, evidence has accumulated for a wide
range of distinct working memory types subject to selective, typespecific interference. The verbal versus visual-spatial distinction is
the best known (Baddeley & Hitch 1974; Logie et al. 1990), but
there is also evidence for distinct codes for kinesthetic memory
(Williams et al. 1969), odor (Walker & John 1984), and sign language (Poizner et al. 1981), to name a few. The robust result across
domains is that, when to-be-remembered items are followed by
stimuli that are similar along some dimensions, the original items
are more quickly forgotten (Shulman 1970; Waugh & Norman
1965).
Crucially, similarity-based interference operates within major
categories as well. The most familiar within-category effect is the
phonological similarity effect. Ordered recall of phonologically
similar lists of words, consonants, or nonsense trigrams is worse
than with dissimilar lists (Baddeley 1966; Conrad 1963; Wickelgren 1965). Related effects show up in immediate memory for
American Sign Language (Poizner et al. 1981) and visual orientation (Magnussen et al. 1991).
Building on these results, and the work cited earlier by Gibson
(1991), Cowper (1976), and others, I have hypothesized that
similarity-based interference as a general principle applies to syntactic working memory as well. Lewis (1993; 1996b) described a
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
105
References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
line psycholinguistic processing and one or more resource
pools that may be used for other verbally-mediated tasks.
How fine a fractionation of resource systems will be needed
to account for capacity limitations in language processing
and other verbally mediated cognitive functions is not
known at this point (Waters & Caplan 1996c).
References
Letters “a” and “r” appearing before authors’ initials refer to target article
and response, respectively.
Abelson, R. A. & Prentice, D. (1997) Contrast tests of interaction hypotheses.
Psychological Methods 2:315–28. [AM]
Abney, S. P. (1989) A computational model of human parsing. Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research 18:129–44. [aDC]
Allik, J. & Bachmann, T. (1983) How bad is icon? Behavioral and Brain Sciences
6:12–13. [AT]
Allik, J. & Help, T. (1985) Comments on S. T. Parker’s “A social-technological
model for the evolution of language.” Current Anthropology 26:626. [AT]
Almor, A., Kempler, D., MacDonald, M. C., Andersen, E. S. & Tyler, L. K. T.
(1998) Why do Alzheimer’s patients have difficulty with pronouns? Working
memory, semantics, and pronouns in Alzheimer’s disease (submitted).
[MHC]
Baddeley, A. D. (1966) Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of
acoustic, semantic, and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 18:362–65. [RLL]
(1976) The psychology of memory. Basic Books. [aDC]
(1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press. [aDC, RAM]
(1990a) The development of the concept of working memory: Implications and
contributions of neuropsychology. In: Neuropsychological impairments of
short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T. Shallice. Cambridge University Press.
[aDC]
(1990b) Human memory: Theory and practice. Allyn and Bacon. [AT]
(1993) Working memory and conscious awareness. In: Theories of memory, ed.
A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway & P. E. Morris. Erlbaum.
[aDC]
(1995) Working memory. In: The cognitive neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga.
MIT Press. [aDC, AT]
Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. & Spinnler, H. (1991) The
decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 114:2521–42.
[aDC]
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (1974) Working memory. In: The psychology of
learning and motivation, vol. 8: Advances in research and theory, ed. G. A.
Bower. Academic Press. [arDC, MJK, RLL, JNT]
(1994) Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology
8:485–93. [aDC]
Baddeley, A. D., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S. D. & Spinnler, H. (1986)
Dementia and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 38A:608–18. [aDC]
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Dronkers, N., Pizzamiglio, L., Wulfeck, B., Hernandez,
A., Juarez, L. & Marangolo, P. (1994) Grammatical deficits in patients without
agrammatism. Brain and Language 47:400–402. [EB]
Baum, S. (1991) Sensitivity to syntactic violations across the age span: Evidence
from a word monitoring task. Journal of Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics
5:317–28. [aDC]
Bayles, K. A. (1982) Language function in senile dementia. Brain and Language
16:265–80. [aDC]
Berndt, R., Mitchum, C. & Haendiges, A. (1996) Comprehension of reversible
sentences in “agrammatism”: A meta-analysis. Cognition 58:289–308.
[aDC]
Berwick, R. C. & Weinberg, A. (1984) The grammatical basis of linguistic
performance: Language use and acquisition. MIT Press. [aDC]
Bever, T. (1970) The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In: Cognition and the
development of language, ed. J. R. Hayes. Wiley. [RLL]
Blackwell, A. & Bates, E. (1995) Inducing agrammatic profiles in normals. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience 7:228–57. [EB, rDC]
Blanken, G., Dittmar, J., Haas, J. C. & Wallesch, C. W. (1987) Spontaneous speech
in senile dementia and aphasia: Implications for a neurolinguistic model of
language production. Cognition 27:247–74. [aDC]
Boland, J. E. & Cutler, A. (1996) Interaction with autonomy: Multiple output
models and the inadequacy of the great divide. Cognition 58:309–20. [NJP]
Brown, R. G. & Marsden, C. D. (1988) Internal versus external cues and the
control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 111:323–45. [aDC]
122
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
(1991) Dual task performance and processing resources in normal subjects and
patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 114:215–31. [aDC]
Buckner, R. & Petersen, S. (1996) What does neuroimaging tell us about the role
of prefrontal cortex in memory retrieval? Seminars in the Neurosciences
8:47–55. [RCM]
Budd, D., Whitney, P. & Turley, K. J. (1995) Individual differences in working
memory strategies for reading expository text. Memory and Cognition
23:735–48. [PW]
Caplan, D. (1983) A note on the ‘word-order problem’ in agrammatism. Brain and
Language 20:155–65. [HHHJK]
(1985) Comments on Fodor’s The modularity of mind: A neo-Cartesian
alternative. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:6–7. [aDC]
(1987) Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology: An introduction. Cambridge
University Press. [aDC]
Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Waters, G. S. (in press) Effects of syntactic structure and
propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience. [aDC]
Caplan, D., Baker, C. & Dehaut, F. (1985) Syntactic determinants of sentence
comprehension in aphasia. Cognition 21:117–75. [arDC]
Caplan, D. & Futter, C. (1986) Assignment of thematic roles to NPs by an
agrammatic aphasic. Brain and Language 27:117–35. [rDC, HHHJK]
Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1986) Language deficits and the theory of syntax: A
reply to Grodzinsky. Brain and Language 27:168–77. [rDC]
(1988) Disorders of syntactic comprehension. MIT Press/Bradford Books.
[rDC, HHHJK]
Caplan, D., Hildebrandt, N. & Makris, N. (1996) Location of lesions in stroke
patients with deficits in syntactic processing in sentence comprehension.
Brain 119:933–49. [aDC]
Caplan, D., Hildebrandt, N. & Waters, G. S. (1994) Interaction of verb selectional
restrictions, noun animacy and syntactic form in sentence processing.
Language and Cognitive Processes 9:549–85. [arDC]
(1997) Determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasic patients in
sentence- picture matching tasks. Journal of Speech Language and Learning
Research 40:542–55. [aDC]
Caplan, D. & Waters, G. (1990) Short-term memory and language comprehension:
A critical review of the neuropsychological literature. In: Neuropsychological
impairments of short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T. Shallice. Cambridge
University Press. [arDC, MJK, AM]
(1995) Aphasic disturbances of syntactic comprehension and working memory
capacity. Cognitive Neuropsychology 12:637–49. [aDC]
(1996) Syntactic processing in sentence comprehension under dual-task
conditions in aphasic patients. Language and Cognitive Processes 11:525–51.
[arDC, HHHJK]
Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. (1988) The role of working memory in language
comprehension. In: Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert
A. Simon, ed. D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky. Erlbaum. [aDC]
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A. & Shell, P. (1990) What one intelligence test
measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive
Matrices Test. Psychological Review 97:404–31. [AM]
Carpenter, P. A., Miyake, A. & Just, M. A. (1994) Working memory constraints in
comprehension: Evidence from individual differences, aphasia, and aging. In:
Handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. M. Gernsbacher. Academic Press. [AW]
Case, R. (1985) Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. Academic Press.
[JNT]
Case, R., Kurland, M. & Goldberg, J. (1982) Operational efficiency and the growth
of short term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
33:386–404. [JNT]
Cattell, R. B. (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical
experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54:1–22. [MJK]
Chapman, R. & Miller, J. (1984) SALT: Systematic analysis of language transcripts.
University of Wisconsin Press. [SK]
Cheung, H. & Kemper, S. (1992) Competing complexity metrics and adults’
production of complex sentences. Applied Psycholinguistics 13:53–76. [SK]
Chomsky, N. (1957) Syntactic structures. Mouton. [aDC]
(1981) Lectures on government and binding. Foris. [aDC]
(1986) Knowledge of language. Praeger. [aDC]
(1993) The minimalist program. MIT Press. [aDC]
(1997) Language and mind: Current thoughts on ancient problems. Pesquisa
Linguistica 3:4. [ZB]
Christensen, A. L. (1974) Luria’s neuropsychological investigation: Text.
Munksgaard. [aDC]
Christiansen, M. H. (1996) Recursive sentence structure in connectionist
networks. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual CUNY Conference on
Sentence Processing, CUNY, New York. [aDC]
Cohen, J. (1978) Partialed products are interactions; partialed powers are curve
components. Psychological Bulletin 85:858–66. [AM]
(1983) The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement
7:249–53. [AM]
References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
(1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edition.
Erlbaum. [AM]
Cohen, L. & Mehler, J. (1996) Click monitoring revisited: An on-line study of
sentence comprehension. Memory and Cognition 24:94–102. [aDC]
Conrad, R. (1963) Acoustic confusions and memory span for words. Nature
197:1029–30. [RLL]
Conway, A. R. A. & Engle, R. W. (1996) Individual differences in working memory
capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory 4:577–90.
[MJK]
Conway, A. R. A., Tuholski, S. W. & Engle, R. W. (1998) Individual differences in
working memory: Insights from the counting span task. Paper presented at
the Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
[MJK]
Cowper, E. A. (1976) Constraints on sentence complexity: A model of syntactic
processing. Ph. D. thesis, Brown University. [RLL]
Cronbach, L. J. & Snow, R. E. (1977) Aptitudes and instructional models: A
handbook for research on interactions. Irvington. [PW]
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. (1980) Individual differences in working memory
and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:450–66.
[aDC, MJK, JNT, MW]
Daneman, M. & Merikle, P. M. (1996) Working memory and language
comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin and Review 3:422–33.
[aDC, AW]
Daneman, M. & Tardif, T. (1987) Working memory and reading skill re-examined.
In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. M.
Coltheart. Erlbaum. [rDC]
Davis, C. A. & Ball, H. E. (1989) Effects of age on comprehension of complex
sentences in adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32:143–50.
[aDC]
Della Sala, S. & Logie, R. H. (1993) When working memory does not work: The
role of working memory in neuropsychology. In: Handbook of
neuropsychology, vol. 8, ed. F. Boller & J. Grafman. Elsevier. [RAM]
Demonet, J. F., Fiez, J. A., Paulesu, E., Petersen, S. E. & Zatorre, R. J. (1996) PET
studies of phonological processing: A critical reply to Poeppel. Brain and
Language 55:352–79. [rDC]
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co. [AT]
DeRenzi, E. & Vignolo, L. (1962) The token test: A sensitive test to detect
receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain 85:665–78. [aDC, SAK]
Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B. & Dronkers, N. (1998) Deficits in the
interpretation of complex sentences in aphasic patients and normals under
adverse processing conditions. University of California, San Diego, Center for
Research in Language. [EB]
Dominey, P. F., Venre-Dominey, J., Brousolle, E. & Jeannerod, M. (1997)
Analogical transfer is effective in a serial reaction time task in Parkinson’s
disease: Evidence for a dissociable sequence learning mechanism.
Neuropsychologia 35:1–9. [rDC]
Emery, O. B. (1985) Language and aging. Experimental Aging Research
(Monograph) 11:3–60. [aDC]
(1986) Linguistic decrement in normal aging. Language and Communication
6:47–64. [aDC]
Engle, R. (1995) Overview of working memory. Paper presented at the Conference
on Cognitive Aging, Atlanta. [aDC]
Engle, R. W., Cantor, J. & Carullo, J. J. (1992) Individual differences in working
memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18:972–92.
[PW]
Engle, R. W. & Conway, A. R. A. (1998) Working memory and comprehension. In:
Working memory and thinking, ed. R. Logie & K. Gilhooly. Psychology Press.
[MJK]
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J. & Tuholski, S. W. (in press a) Individual differences in
working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention,
general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In: Models of
working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control,
ed. A. Miyake & P. Shah. Cambridge University Press. [MJK]
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E. & Conway, A. R. A. (in press b)
Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent
variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [MJK]
Eslinger, P. J. & Damasio, A. R. (1985) Severe disturbance of higher cognition after
bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology 35:1731–41. [AM]
Farah, M. J. (1994) Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A
critique of the “locality” assumption. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17:43–61.
[PW]
Feier, C. D. & Gerstman, L. J. (1980) Sentence comprehension abilities
throughout the adult life span. Journal of Gerontology 35:722–28. [aDC]
Ferreira, F. (1992) Linguistic processing is modular. Talk presented at the
University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Conference, Milwaukee, Wisconsin.
[aDC]
Ferreira, F., Anes, M. D. & Horine, M. D. (1996) Exploring the use of prosody
during language comprehension using the auditory moving window technique.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25:273–90. [aDC]
Ferreira, F., Henderson, J. M., Anes, M. D., Weeks, P. A., Jr. & McFarlane, D. K.
(1996) Effects of lexical frequency and syntactic complexity in spoken
language comprehension: Evidence from the auditory moving window
technique. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
Cognition 22:324–35. [aDC]
Fiez, J., Raife, E., Balota, D., Scheartz, J., Raichle, M. & Peterson, S. (1996) A
positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal
information. Journal of Neuroscience 16:808–22. [rDC, RCM]
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press. [arDC, NJP, PW]
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. (1974) The psychology of language: An
introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. McGraw Hill.
[MW]
Fodor, J. D. & Inoue, A. (in press) Attach anyway. In: Reanalysis in sentence
processing, ed. F. Ferreira & J. D. Fodor. Kluwer. [FF]
Ford, M. (1983) A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity
throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
22:203–18. [aDC]
Forster, K. I. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language
processor. In: Sentence processing, ed. W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker.
Erlbaum. [aDC]
Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J. & Mehler, J. (1980) Monitoring around the relative
clause. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:328–37. [aDC]
Frazier, L. (1985) Syntactic complexity. In: Natural language parsing:
Psychological, computational, and theoretical perspectives, ed. D. R. Dowty,
L. Karttunen & A. M. Zwicky. Cambridge University Press. [SK]
(1990) Exploring the architecture of the language-processing system. In:
Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational
perspectives, ed. G. T. M. Altmann. MIT Press/Bradford Books. [aDC]
Frazier, L. & Clifton, C. (1996) Construal. MIT Press. [arDC]
Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. (1982) Making and correcting errors during sentence
comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous
sentences. Cognitive Psychology 143:178–210. [aDC]
Freidrich, F. J. (1990) Multiple phonological representations and verbal short-term
memory. In: The neuropsychology of short-term memory, ed. T. Shallice & G.
Vallar. Cambridge University Press. [aDC]
Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A. & von Cramon, D. Y. (in press) First-pass versus
second-pass parsing processes in a Wernicke’s and a Broca’s aphasic:
Electrophysiological evidence for a double dissociation. Brain and Language.
[SAK]
Frick, R. W. (1995) Accepting the null hypothesis. Memory and Cognition
23:132–38. [AM]
Friston, K. J., Price, C. J., Fletcher, P., Moore, C., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Dolan,
R. J. (1996) The trouble with cognitive subtraction. NeuroImage 4:97–104.
[KJF]
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993) Working memory and language.
Erlbaum. [aDC, AM]
Gibson, E. (1991) A computational theory of human linguistic processing: Memory
limitations and processing breakdown. Ph. D. thesis, Carnegie Mellon
University, CMU-CMT-91–125. [RLL]
(in press) Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition.
[aDC, EG]
Gibson, E., Schutze, C. & Salomon, C. (1996) The relationship between the
frequency and the processing of complex linguistic structures. Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research 25:59–92. [SK]
Gibson, E. & Thomas, J. (1996) The processing complexity of English centerembedded and self-embedded structures. In: Proceedings of the NELS 26
workshop on language processing: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, ed. C.
Schutze. MIT Press. [aDC]
Gick, M. L., Craik, F. I. M. & Morris, R. G. (1988) Task complexity and age
differences in working memory. Memory and Cognition 16:353–61.
[aDC]
Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E. & Ivry, R. (1995) Functional mapping of sequence
learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7:497–510.
[aDC]
Grafton, S. T., Mazziota, J. C., Presty, S., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Phelps,
M. E. (1992) Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined
with regional cerebral blood flow and PET. Journal of Neuroscience
12:2542–48. [aDC]
Grober, E. & Bang, S. (1995) Sentence comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease.
Developmental Neuropsychology 11:95–107. [aDC]
Grodzinsky, Y. (1990) Theoretical perspectives on language deficits. MIT Press.
[HHHJK]
Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., Vernon, G. & Hurtig, H. I.
(1991) Sentence comprehension and praxis deficits in Parkinson’s disease.
Neurology 41:1620–28. [aDC]
Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S. & Hurtig, H. (1992) Sentence
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
123
References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and memory.
Brain and Language 42:347–84. [aDC]
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H. & Phillips, S. (1998) Processing capacity defined by
relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and
cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(6):803–65. [GA]
Hart, S. (1988) Language and dementia: A review. Psychological Medicine
18:99–112. [aDC]
Hawkins, J. A. (1994) A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge
University Press. [RLL]
Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, K. & Schindler, A. G. (1985) Language disintegration in
dementia: Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language 25:117–33.
[aDC]
Hildebrandt, N., Caplan, D. & Evans, K. (1987) The man “left” without a trace: A
case study of aphasic processing of empty categories. Cognitive
Neuropsychology 4:257–302. [aDC]
Hitch, G. J. & Baddeley, A. D. (1976) Verbal reasoning and working memory.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28:603–21. [JNT]
Horn, J. L. (1980) Concepts of intellect in relation to learning and adult
development. Intelligence 4:285–317. [MJK]
Horn, J. L. & McArdle, J. J. (1992) A practical and theoretical guide to
measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental Aging Research
18:117–44. [SK]
Humphreys, L. G. & Fleishman, A. (1974) Pseudo-orthogonal and other analysis of
variance designs involving individual-differences variable. Journal of
Educational Psychology 66:464–72. [AM]
Illes, J. (1989) Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production
dissociate three forms of neurodegenerative disease: Alzheimer’s,
Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s. Brain and Language 37:628–42. [aDC]
Irigaray, L. (1973) Le langage des dements. Mouton. [aDC]
Irle, E. (1987) Lesion size and recovery of function: Some new perspectives. Brain
Research Reviews 12:307–20. [AT]
(1990) An analysis of the correlation of lesion size, localization and behavioral
effects in 283 published studies of cortical and subcortical lesions in old-world
monkeys. Brain Research Reviews 15:11–213. [AT]
Johnson, M. (1996) Resource sensitivity in grammar and processing. Paper
presented at the Ninth Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence
Processing, CUNY, New York. [aDC]
Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Awh, E., Minoshima, S. & Monton, M. A. (1993) Spatial
working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363:623–25. [aDC]
Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension:
Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review
99(1):122–49. [GA, arDC, EG, MJK, HHHJK, AM, MW, PW]
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A. & Keller, T. (1996) Working memory: New frontiers of
evidence. Psychological Review 103:773–75. [aDC]
Just, M. A. Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F. & Thulborn, K. R. (1996b)
Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274:114–16.
[aDC]
Kahneman, D. (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall. [AW]
Kane, M. J. & Engle, R. W. (1988) Full frontal fluidity: Working memory capacity,
attention, general fluid abilities, and the prefrontal cortex (submitted).
[MJK]
Kapur, N. (1996) Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-behaviour research. A
critical review. Brain 119:1775–90. [AT]
Kemper, S. (1986) Imitation of complex syntactic constructions by elderly adults.
Applied Psycholinguistics 7:277–87. [aDC]
(1987) Life-span changes in syntactic complexity. Journal of Gerontology 42:23238. [Sk]
(1988) Geriatric psycholinguistics: Syntactic limitations of oral and written
language. In: Language, memory, and aging, ed. L. L. Light & D. M. Burke.
Cambridge University Press. [aDC, SK]
Kemper, S., Anagnopoulos, C., Lyons, K. & Heberlein, W. (1994) Speech
accomodations to dementia. Journal of Gerontology 49:223–29. [aDC]
Kemper, S., Kynette, D. & Norman, S. (1992) Age differences in spoken language.
In: Everyday memory and aging, ed. R. West & J. Sinnott. Springer-Verlag.
[SK]
Kemper, S., Kynette, D., Rash, S., O’Brien, K. & Sprott, R. (1989) Life-span
changes to adults’ language: Effects of memory and genre. Applied
Psycholinguistics 10:49–66. [SK]
Kemper, S., LaBarge, E., Ferraro, F. R., Cheung, H., Cheung, H. & Storandt, M.
(1993) On the preservation of syntax in Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence from
written sentences. Archives of Neurology 50:81–86. [aDC]
Kemper, S. & Rash, S. (1988) Practical aspects of memory: Current research and
issues. Cambridge University Press. [aDC]
Kemper, S., Snowdon, D. A. & Greiner, L. H. (1996) Linguistic ability across the
life span: Findings from the Nun study. Paper presented at the Cognitive
Aging Conference, Atlanta. [aDC]
Kempler, D., Curtiss, S. & Jackson, C. (1987) Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer’s
disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30:343–50. [aDC]
124
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
Kemtes, K. A. & Kemper, S. (1996) Younger and older adults’ working memory
and on- line processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences. Paper presented
at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta. [aDC]
(1998) Age-based construct validation of working memory. Unpublished
manuscript. [SK]
Kilborn, K. (1991) Selective impairment of grammatical morphology due to
induced stress in normal listeners: Implications for aphasia. Brain and
Language 41:275–88. [EB, rDC]
King, J. & Just, M. A. (1991) Individual differences in syntactic processing: The
role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30:580–602.
[aDC, PW]
King, J. W. & Kutas, M. (1995) Who did what and when: Using word- and clauselevel ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 7(3):376–95. [MK]
Kintsch, W. & Keenan, J. M. (1973) Reading rate and retention as a function of the
number of the propositions in the base structure of sentences. Cognitive
Psychology 5:257–74. [SK]
Kirshner, H. S. (1982) Language disorders in dementia. In: The neurology of
aphasia, ed. H. S. Kirshner & F. R. Freeman. Swets and Zeitlinger. [aDC]
Kluender, R. & Kutas, M. (1993) Subjacency as a processing phenomenon.
Language and Cognitive Processes 8:573–633. [aDC]
Kolk, H. H. J. & van Grunsven, M. F. (1985) Agrammatism as a variable
phenomenon. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2:347–84. [HHHJK]
Kolk, H. H. J. & Weijts, M. (1996) Judgements of semantic anomaly in agrammatic
patients: Argument movement, syntactic complexity and the use of heuristics.
Brain and Language 54:86–135. [HHHJK]
Kontiola, P., Laaksonen, R., Sulkava, R. & Erkinjuntii, T. (1990) Pattern of
language impairment is different in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-infarct
dementia. Brain and Language 38:364–83. [aDC]
Koster, J. (1975) Dutch as an SOV language. Linguistic Analysis 1:111–36.
[HHHJK]
Krueger, L. E. (1994) Mind your p’s and q’s when accepting or rejecting the null
hypothesis. Paper presented at the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, MO.
[aDC]
Kuno, S. (1974) The position of relative clauses and conjunctions. Linguistic
Inquiry 5:117–36. [RLL]
Kutas, M. & King, J. W. (1996) The potentials for basic sentence processing:
Differentiating integrative processes. In: Attention and performance XVI, ed.
I. Ikeda & J. L. McClelland. MIT Press. [MK]
Kynette, D. & Kemper, S. (1986) Aging and the loss of grammatical forms.
Language and Communication 6:65–72. [SK]
Lalami, L., Marblestone, K., Schuster, S., Andersen, E., Kempler, D., Tyler, L. &
MacDonald, M. (1996) On-line vs. off-line sentence processing in
Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at the Meeting of Cognitive Aging,
Atlanta. [aDC]
Lee, L. (1974) Developmental sentence analysis. Northwestern University Press.
[SK]
Lees, A. J. & Smith, E. (1983) Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s
disease. Brain 106:257–70. [aDC]
Lehto, J. (1996) Are executive function tests dependent on working memory
capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A:29–50. [AM]
Lewis, R. L. (1993) An architecturally-based theory of human sentence
comprehension. Ph. D. thesis, School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon
University (CMU-CS- 93–226). [RLL]
(1996a) A theory of grammatical but unacceptable embeddings.Unpublished.
[aDC]
Lewis, R. L. (1996b) Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two
(or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research
25:93–115. [SK, RLL]
(1998) Working memory in sentence processing: Retroactive and proactive
interference in parsing. Talk presented at CUNY 98, New Brunswick, NJ.
[RLL]
Lieberman, P., Friedman, J. & Feldman, L. S. (1990) Syntax comprehension in
Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 178:360–66.
[aDC]
Light, L. L. (1990) Interactions between memory and language in old age. In:
Handbook of the psychology of aging, 3rd edition, ed. J. E. Birren & K. W.
Schale. Academic Press. [aDC, AW]
Logie, R., Zucco, G. M. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990) Interference with visual shortterm memory. Acta Psychologica 75:55–74. [RLL]
Luria, A. R. (1974) Neiropskhologiya pamyati. Narusheniya pamyati pri lokal
‘nykh porazheniakh mozga. [Neuropsychology of memory. Memory
disturbances in local brain lesions]. Pedagogika. [AT]
MacDonald, M. C. (1997) Language and Cognitive Processes: Special Issue on
Lexical Representations and Sentence Processing 12:121–399. [aDC]
MacDonald, M. C. & Christiansen, M. H. (1998) Individual differences without
working memory: A reply to Just & Carpenter and Waters & Caplan.
(submitted). [MHC, NJP]
References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992) Working memory
contraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology
24:56–98. [arDC]
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994) The lexical
nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101:676–703.
[aDC, AM]
Magnussen, S., Greenlee, M. W., Asplund, R. & Dyrnes, S. (1991) Stimulusspecific mechanisms of visual short-term memory. Vision Research
31:1213–19. [RLL]
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987) Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition.
Cognition 25:71–102. [aDC]
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. & Tyler, L. K. (1987) Against modularity. In: Modularity in
knowledge representation and natural language understanding, ed. J. L.
Garfield. MIT Press. [NJP]
Martin, R., R. (1995) Working memory doesn’t work: A critique of Miyake et al.’s
capacity theory of aphasic comprehension deficits. Cognitive
Neuropsychology 12:623–36. [RCM]
Martin, R. & Feher, E. (1990) The consequences of reduced memory span for
comprehension of semantic versus syntactic information. Brain and Language
38:1–20. [aDC, SAK]
Martin, R. C. & Romani, C. (1994) Verbal working memory and sentence processing:
A multiple component view. Neuropsychology 8:506–23. [rDC, RCM]
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. & Yaffee, L. (1994) Language processing and working
memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and
semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language 33:83–111. [RCM]
Mattys, S. L. (1997) The use of time during lexical processing and segmentation: A
review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 4:310–29. [AW]
Maxwell, S. E. & Delaney, H. D. (1993) Bivariate median splits and spurious
statistical significance. Psychological Bulletin 113:181–90. [AM]
Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D. & Dill, C. A. (1984) Another look at ANCOVA
versus blocking. Psychological Bulletin 95:136–47. [AM]
McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1987a) The double dissociation of shortterm memory for lists and sentences. Brain 110:1545–64. [RAM]
(1987b) Understanding: A function of short-term memory? Brain 110:1565–78.
[RAM]
(1990a) Cognitive neuropsychology: A clinical introduction. Academic Press.
[RAM]
(1990b) Short-term memory: A phenomenon in search of a function? In:
Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory, ed. G. Vallar & T.
Shallice. Cambridge University Press. [RAM]
McClelland, G. H. (1997) Optimal design in psychological research. Psychological
Methods 2:3–19. [AM]
McClelland, G. H. & Judd, C. M. (1993) Statistical difficulties of detecting
interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin 114:376–90. [AM]
Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K. & Friederici, A. D. (1995)
Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An
analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition 23(4):477–94.
[MK]
Miller, G. A. & Chomsky, N. (1963) Finitary models of language users. In:
Handbook of mathematical psychology, vol. II, ed. R. R. Bush & E. Galanter.
Wiley. [RLL]
Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. & Just, M. (1994) A capacity approach to syntactic
comprehension disorders: Making normal adults perform like aphasic
patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology 11:671–717. [EB, aDC, AM]
Morris, R. G. (1984) Dementia and the functioning of the articulatory loop system.
Cognitive Neuropsychology 1:143–57. [aDC]
(1986) Short-term forgetting in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
Cognitive Neuropsychology 3:77–97. [aDC]
(1987) Articulatory rehearsal in Alzheimer type dementia. Brain and Language
30:351–62. [aDC]
Morris, R. G. & Baddeley, A. D. (1988) Primary and working memory functioning
in Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology 10:279–96. [aDC]
Morris, R. G., Craik, F. I. M. & Gick, M. L. (1990) Age differences in working
memory tasks: The role of secondary memory and the central executive
system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42A:67–86. [aDC]
Morris, R. G., Gick, M. L. & Craik, F. I. M. (1988) Processing resources and age
differences in working memory. Memory and Cognition 16:362–66. [aDC]
Müller, H. M., King, J. W. & Kutas, M. (1997) Event-related potentials elicited by
spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research 5(3):193–203. [MK]
Münte, T. F., Schwirtz, O., Wieringa, B. M., Matzke, M. & Johannes, S. (1997)
Electrophysiology of complex sentences: Word and sentence-level ERPs.
EEG-EMG-Zeitschrift für Elektroenzephalographie, Elektromyographie und
verwandte Gebiete 28(1):11–17. [MK]
Natsopoulos, D., Katsarou, Z., Bostantzopoulo, S., Grouios, G., Mentanopoulos, G.
& Logothetis, J. (1991) Strategies in comprehension of relative clauses in
Parkinsonian patients. Cortex 27:255–68. [aDC]
Naucler, N., Wulfeck, B. & Bates, E. (1998) A developmental study of complex
sentence interpretation abilities (Technical Report CND-9803). University of
California, San Diego, Project in Cognitive and Neural Development. [EB]
Navon, D. (1984) Resources – A theoretical soup stone. Psychological Review
91:216–34. [rDC, MHC]
Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I. & Garrett, M. F. (1991)
Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related
brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3:151–65. [aDC]
Nõmm, M. (1996) The development of short-memory span: Further evidence on
the contribution of long-term memory. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Tartu
University. [AT]
Norman, S., Kemper, S., Kynette, D., Cheung, H. & Anagnopoulous, C. (1991)
Syntactic complexity and adults running memory span. Journal of
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 46:346–51. [aDC]
Obler, L. K., Fein, D., Nicholas, M. & Albert, M. L. (1991) Auditory
comprehension and aging: Decline in syntactic processing. Applied
Psycholinguistics 12:433–52. [aDC]
Pearlmutter, N. J. & MacDonald, M. C. (1992) Plausibility and syntactic ambiguity
resolution. In: Proceedings of the 14th Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science Society. Erlbaum. [NJP]
(1995) Individual differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic ambiguity
resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 34:521–42. [arDC, NJP]
Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E. & Evans, A. (1993) Functional activation of
the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory
tasks. Neurobiology 90:878–82. [arDC]
Phillips, C. B., Zeki, S. & Barlow, H. B. (1984) Localization of function in the
cerebral cortex. Past, present and future. Brain 107:327–61. [KJF]
Plaut, D., McClelland, J., Seidenberg, M. & Patterson, K. (1996) Understanding
normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular
domains. Psychological Review 103:56–115. [rDC]
Poizner, H., Bellugi, U. & Tweney, R. D. (1981) Processing of formational, semantic,
and iconic information in American sign language. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7:1146–59. [RLL]
Posner, M. I., Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T. & Raichle, M. E. (1988) Localization of
cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240:1627–31. [aDC]
Potter, M. C. (1993) Very short-term conceptual memory. Memory and Cognition
21:156–61. [AW]
Price, C. J. & Friston, K. J. (1997) Cognitive conjunction: A new approach to brain
activation experiments. NeuroImage 5:261–70. [KJF]
Rakic, P. (1988) Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241:170–76. [aDC]
Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Head, D., Dupois, J. H. & Acker, J. D. (1998)
Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: Evidence from structural MRI.
Neuropsychology 12:95–106. [AW]
Restle, F. (1970) Theory of serial pattern learning: Structural trees. Psychological
Review 77:481–95. [AT]
Roberts, R. M. & Corkin, S. (1998) What do working memory tasks measure?
Manuscript under review, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT.
[EG]
Roberts, R. M., Hooker, C. & Gibson, E. (1997) Sentence memory capacity and
working memory. Poster presented at the 10th CUNY Sentence Processing
Conference, Santa Monica, CA, March 1997. [EG]
Rochon, E. & Saffran, E. M. (1995) A semantic contribution to sentence
comprehension impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented to the
Academy of Aphasia, San Diego. [aDC]
Rochon, E., Waters, G. S. & Caplan, D. (1994) Sentence comprehension in
patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language 46:329–49. [aDC]
Rosenberg, S. & Abbeduto, L. (1987) Indicators of linguistic competence in the
peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults. Applied
Psycholinguistics 8:19–32. [SK]
Rosenthal, R. R. & Rosnow, R. L. (1985) Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons
in the analysis of variance. Cambridge University Press. [AM]
Salthouse, T. A. (1991) Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Erlbaum. [aDC]
Schneider, W. (1993) Varieties of working memory as seen in biology and in
connectionist/control architectures. Memory and Cognition 21:184–92. [AT]
Schneider, W. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977) Controlled and automatic human
information processing I.: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological
Review 84:1–66. [aDC]
Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S. M. & Saffran, E. M. (1979) Dissociations of language
function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language 7:277–306. [aDC]
Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M. & Marin, O. S. M. (1980) The word order problem
in agrammatism I: Comprehension. Brain and Language 10:249–67. [RAM]
Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (1989) A distributed, developmental model
of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96:523–68. [rDC]
Shah, P. & Miyake, A. (1996) The separability of working memory resources for
spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125:4–27. [aDC]
Shallice, T. & Burgess, P. (1993) Supervisory control of action and thought
selection. In: Attention: Selection, awareness, and control, ed. A. Baddeley &
L. Weiscrantz. Clarendon Press. [MJK]
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
125
References/Caplan & Waters: Working memory and sentence comprehension
(1996) The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of
behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London
351:1405–12. [HHHJK]
Shallice, T. & Warrington, E. K. (1970) Independent functioning of the verbal
memory stores: A neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology 22:261–73. [RAM]
Sherman, J. C., Schweickert, J., Growdon, J. & Corkin, S. (1988) Sentence
comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Paper presented at the 18th Annual
Meeting of the Society of Neuroscience, Toronto. [aDC]
Shiffrin, R. M. & Schneider, W. (1977) Controlled and automatic human
information processing II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a
general theory. Psychological Review 84:127–90. [aDC]
Shulman, H. (1970) Similarity effects in short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin
75:399–414. [RLL]
Smith, S. (1989) Syntactic comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease.Paper presented
at the Academy of Aphasia conference, Santa Fe, New Mexico [aDC]
Snowdon, D. A., Kemper, S. J., Mortimer, J. A., Greiner, L. H., Wekstein, D. R. &
Markesbery, W. R. (1996) Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function
and Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of
the American Medical Association 275:528–32. [aDC]
Squire, L. R. & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991) The medial temporal lobe memory system.
Science 253:1380–86. [aDC]
Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: Extension of Donder’s
method. Acta Psychologia 30:276–315. [KJF]
Stevens, K., Kempler, D., Andersen, E. & MacDonald, M. C. (1996) Preserved use
of semantic and syntactic context in Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at
the Meeting of Cognitive Aging, Atlanta. [aDC]
Stine, E. A. L. (1995) Aging and the distribution of resources in working memory.
In: Age differences in word and language processing, ed. P. Allen & T.
Bashore. North-Holland. [SK]
Stowe, L. (1998) The cognitive function of the inferior frontal lobe in sentence
comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience S126. [rDC]
Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Rauch, S. (1996) Localization of syntactic
comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language
52:452–73. [aDC]
Sturt, P. & Crocker, M. W. (1996) Monotonic syntactic processing: A crosslinguistic study of attachment and reanalysis. Language and Cognitive
Processing 11:449–94. [arDC]
Swinney, D. & Love, T. (1998) Language input rate as a parameter on structural
processing. Proceedings of the 11th Annual CUNY Conference on Human
Sentence Processing, New Brunswick, NJ, March 1998. [MW]
Swinney, D. & Zurif, E. (1996) Syntactic processing in aphasia. Brain and
Language 50:225–39. [aDC]
Swinney, D., Zurif, E., Prather, P. & Love, T. (1996) Neurological distribution of
processing resources underlying language comprehension. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience 8:174–84. [aDC]
Tabor, W., Juliano, C. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1997) Parsing in a dynamical system: A
attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints
in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 12:211–71. [arDC]
Tabossi, P., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., McRae, K. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994)
Semantic effects on syntactic ambiguity resolution: Evidence for a constraintbased resolution process. In: Attention and performance XV, ed. C. Umilta &
M. Moscovitch. MIT Press. [NJP]
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K. & Sedivy, J. (1995) The
interaction of visual and verbal information in spoken language
comprehension. Science 268:1632–34. [PW]
Taylor, A. E., Saint-Cyr, J. A. & Lang, A. E. (1986) Frontal lobe dysfunction in
Parkinson’s disease. Brain 109:845–86. [aDC]
Tim, P. A. & Wingfield, A. (1993) Is speech special? Perception and recall of
spoken language in complex environments. In: Adult information processing:
Limits on loss, ed. J. Cerella, J. Rybash, W. Hoyer & M. L. Commons.
Academic Press. [AW]
Tirre, W. & Pena, C. (1992) Investigation of functional working memory in the
reading span test. Journal of Educational Psychology 84:462–72. [arDC]
Titchener, E. B. (1908) Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and
attention. Macmillan. [AW]
Tomoeda, C. K., Bayles, K. A., Boone, D. R., Kaszniak, A. W. & Slauson, T. J.
(1990) Speech rate and syntactic complexity effects on the auditory
comprehension of Alzheimer patients. Journal of Communication Disorders
23:151–61. [aDC]
Towse, J. N. (1998) On random generation and the central executive of working
memory. British Journal of Psychology 89(1):77–101. [JNT]
Towse, J. N. & Hitch, G. J. (1995) Is there a relationship between task demand and
storage space in tests of working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology 48A:108–24. [rDC, JNT]
Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J. & Hutton, U. (1998) A re-evaluation of working memory
capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language 39:195–217. [JNT]
126
BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1999) 22:1
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Garnsey, S. M. (1994) Semantic influence on
syntactic processing: Use of thematic information in syntactic disambiguation.
Journal of Memory and Language 33:265–312. [aDC, NJP]
Turner, M. & Engle, R. W. (1989) Is working memory capacity task dependent?
Journal of Memory and Language 28:127–54. [aDC, MJK]
Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz,
W. J. & Pinker, S. (1997) A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that
the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical
rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 9:266–76. [ZB, rDC]
Vallar, G. & Shallice, T., eds. (1990) Neuropsychological impairments of short-term
memory. Cambridge University Press. [aDC]
Waldstein, R. & Baum, S. (1992) The influence of syntactic and semantic context
on word monitoring latencies in normal aging. Journal of Speech Language
Pathology and Audiology 6:217–22. [aDC]
Walenski, M., Figueroa, S. & Swinney, D. (1997) The sentence memory task: A
methodological investigation. Proceedings of the 10th Annual CUNY
Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Santa Monica, CA, March 1997.
[MW]
Walker, H. A. & John, E. E. (1984) Interference and facilitation in short-term
memory for odors. Perception and Psychophysics 36:508–14. [RLL]
Wanner, E. & Maratsos, M. (1978) An ATN approach to comprehension. In:
Linguistic theory and psychological reality, ed. M. M. Halle, G. Miller & J.
Bresnan. MIT Press. [aDC]
Warrington, E. K. (1975) The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:637–57. [SAK]
Warrington, E. K., Logue, V. & Pratt, R. T. C. (1971) The anatomical localisation of
selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia
9:377–87. [RAM]
Waters, G. & Caplan, D. (1996a) The capacity theory of sentence comprehension:
A reply to Just and Carpenter. Psychological Review 103:761–72. [aDC]
(1996b) Processing resource capacity and the comprehension of garden path
sentences. Memory and Cognition 24:342–55. [aDC, AM]
(1996c) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to
reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
49:51–79. [arDC]
(1997) Working memory and on-line sentence comprehension in patients with
Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 26:377–400.
[aDC]
(submitted) The effect of a digit load on syntactic processing in subjects with
high and low working memory capacity. [aDC]
Waters, G., Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1987) Working memory and written
sentence comprehension. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology
of reading, ed. M. Coltheart. Erlbaum. [arDC]
(1991) On the structure of verbal short-term memory and its functional role in
sentence comprehension: Evidence from neuropsychology. Cognitive
Neuropsychology 8(2):81–126. [arDC]
Waters, G. S., Caplan, D. & Rochon, E. (1995) Processing capacity and sentence
comprehension in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Cognitive
Neuropsychology 12:1–30. [aDC, AM]
Waters, G. S., Rochon, E. & Caplan, D. (in press) Task demands and sentence
comprehension in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Brain and
Language. [aDC]
Waugh, N. C. & Norman, D. A. (1965) Primary memory. Psychological Review
72:89–104. [RLL]
Wickelgren, W. (1965) Acoustic similarity and retroactive interference in shortterm memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 4:53–61.
[RLL]
Williams, H. L., Beaver, W. S., Spence, M. T. & Rundell, O. H. (1969) Digital and
kinesthetic memory with interpolated information processing. Journal of
Experimental Psychology 80(3):530–36. [RLL]
Wright, R. E. (1981) Aging, divided attention, and processing capacity. Journal of
Gerontology 36(5):605–14. [aDC]
Yngve, V. (1960) A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of
the American Philosophical Society 104:444–66. [SK]
Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E. & Gjedde, A. (1992) Lateralization of
phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science 256:846–49.
[rDC]
Zeki, S., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Friston, K. J., Kennard, C. & Frackowiak,
R. S. J. (1991) A direct demonstration of functional specialisation in human
visual cortex. Neuroscience 11:641–49. [KJF]
Zurif, E., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Solomon, J. & Bushell, C. (1993) An on-line
analysis of syntactic processing in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and
Language 45:448–64. [aDC]
Zurif, E., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Wingfield, A. & Brownell, H. (1995) The
allocation of memory resources during sentence comprehension: Evidence
from the elderly. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24:165–82. [aDC]
Download