A sample of a multi-step approach to cross-cultural research design. Step 1. Describe a problem (an issue) you have to investigate. Review the scholarly literature on the topic. You may use popular journals, magazines, and newspapers for additional references. Check available sources in the language of the country or countries you examine, if necessary. Step 2. Identify your research goal, i.e. explain what you want to achieve as a result. Then introduce one or several hypotheses for your study. You can use at least two strategies; (a) inductive: you collect data first, and then make a conclusion about the studied samples; (b) deductive: you select a hypothesis first; then you collect data to demonstrate or reject the selected hypothesis. Step 3. Identify and describe the research sample of your study: groups of people, newspaper reports, children’s drawings, texts, etc. Step 4. Choose or design a methodology for your project. Make sure that your method does not violate research ethics. Refer to your local Human Subjects Review Board for approval. Put together a schedule (time-table) for your project. Step 5. Conduct a pilot study, a preliminary exploration of the method to see how your methodology works and whether there are any obstacles to data collection. Step 6. Collect research data. Step 7. Interpret you data using statistical procedures. Step 8. Present the results and analyze them critically in a report. Step 9. In your report, suggest where and how your data should be or could be used (i.e., in education, counseling, advertisement, conflict-resolution, etc.) Source: Shiraev E.and Levy, D. Cross-Cultural Psychology. (2007). Second Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon Two Research Strategies in Cross-Cultural Studies Comparativist strategy: Researchers focus primarily on similarities and differences in certain statistical measures in a sample of ethnic, religious, or national group. Example: Are major symptoms of alcoholism similar in European and Asian populations? Application-oriented strategy: Researchers attempt to establish the applicability of research findings obtained in one ethnic, religious, or national group to other ethnic, religious, or national groups. Example: If a certain method of treatment of depression in women is proven effective in Great Britain and the United States, will this method be similarly effective in Japan or China? Source: Shiraev E.and Levy, D. Cross-Cultural Psychology. (2007). Second Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon Generic rules that used for successful translation in cross-cultural studies The translation process from the beginning ought to be conducted by bilinguals, i.e., by people proficient in both languages. They should conduct back-translation: initially, they translate the original version of the method and then transfer this version back into the original language. Then both versions are compared. It is quite beneficial to have several people do the translation so that there will be several versions of it. These versions can then be compared and converted into one. Both versions of a questionnaire can be administered on the same bilingual individuals. If the investigator gets similar results on both versions, this is a good indicator that the translation was conducted successfully. Source: Shiraev E. and Levy, D. Cross-Cultural Psychology. (2007). Second Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon A case in point. A 2003 study, conducted in Finland, found that Finnish women who had cosmetic breast implants were three times more likely to commit suicide than the general population. These results were similar to findings from a comparable study of Swedish and American women conducted by the National Cancer Institute. The question is why? A common response of the media after the publication of the Finnish study was that breast implants caused a high risk of suicidal behavior. Are there other explanations? Option 2: The high suicide rate might be a function of the problems, discomfort, pain, or serious regrets that occur in some women months and years after their surgery. Option 3: It is also possible that the high suicide rate reflects the psychological makeup of women who seek implants. In other words, women who want to have breast implants, as a group, might be more likely than all other women to have specific psychological problems than the general population. Source: Kaufman (2003).