Running head: COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 1 Social Trait Judgment and Affect Recognition from Static Faces and Video Vignettes in Schizophrenia Lindsey Gilling McIntosh Thesis completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Honors Program in Psychological Sciences Under the direction of Professor Sohee Park Vanderbilt University May, 2011 Approved by _________________________________________ Date _______________________ COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Acknowledgements I would like to acknowledge and extend my heartfelt gratitude my mentor, Dr. Sohee Park, for introducing me to the world of schizophrenia research, and for her vital support and guidance throughout this project. I am also grateful to Heathman Nichols, Katy Thakkar, and Joel Peterman for their assistance in conducting the clinical interviews and making the data analysis considerably less stressful. 2 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Abstract Previous research has shown impairment in affect recognition in individuals with schizophrenia. Poor affect recognition has been associated with more severe clinical symptoms and poor social functioning. The current body of literature has not properly addressed more complex social judgments made by schizophrenics. The present study combined an affect recognition task with a social trait judgment task, using a combination of emotional faces and short video vignettes. Patients showed a general impairment in affect recognition. However, as a group, schizophrenic patients did not make significantly different judgments of trustworthiness, approachability, attractiveness, and intelligence, from either stimulus, relative to the control group. We found evidence for a positive bias in trait attribution to static faces in patients with more severe positive symptoms, particularly delusions. These findings suggest that the ability to integrate nonverbal cues for trait judgment is largely intact, but when there are few nonverbal cues to draw from in judgment formation, a positive bias in more symptomatic patients is shown for some types of judgments. Keywords: affect recognition, social judgment, social cognition, nonverbal behavior 3 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Introduction We unintentionally send salient nonverbal social cues from socially rich channels such as voice, facial expression, body posture, and gestures. These cues are often automatically decoded by others, and integrated in the formation of complex social judgments. We update these judgments as new cues become available, and use them to organize our own displayed behavior during social interaction (Funder, 1995). Healthy people are very good at this process, and it seems to result in mostly successful social interactions. However, research on people with schizophrenia generally suggests that patients are impaired in both the encoding and decoding of nonverbal social cues and that these social cognition deficits may contribute to poor social functioning (Mandal, Pandey & Prasad, 1998). Affect recognition impairment by schizophrenics is one of the most studied topics of social cognition in schizophrenia. Most of these studies have found that people with schizophrenia are impaired in judging the emotional state of others (Hooker & Park, 2002; Kohler et al., 2010). However, it takes much more than correctly identifying the emotion your social partner is displaying in order to successfully interact with them, and research on more complex judgments in schizophrenia is relatively underdeveloped. How do schizophrenic patients judge social traits such as trustworthiness, approachability, and intelligence of others relative to healthy people? If their judgments are abnormal, do these misjudgments contribute to the patient’s social dysfunction? Such questions have not been systematically addressed by the current body of literature on schizophrenia. The goal of this project was to address such gaps in the literature with the hope of gaining a better picture of the social judgment process in schizophrenia. 4 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Schizophrenia has a worldwide prevalence rate of about 1 to 2% and is characterized by positive symptoms such as delusions, hallucinations, and bizarre behavior, as well as negative symptoms such as anhedonia, alogia, and blunted affect (van Os & Kapur, 2009). In addition to these symptoms, schizophrenia patients exhibit cognitive deficits such as those of executive function and working memory (Gold & Harvey, 1993; Lee & Park, 2005). Although the etiology of schizophrenia is far from clear, research suggests a complex combination of genetic vulnerability, prenatal insult, and environmental factors may be the source. Poor social functioning is common among people with schizophrenia (Bellack et al., 1990), and even though schizophrenia is a debilitating and often isolating disorder, this social dysfunction usually predates the diagnosis, and as such is characteristic of the nonpsychotic prodrome (Jones et al., 1993; Yung & McGorry, 1996; Zigler & Phillips, 1960). Antipsychotic medication, the standard primary treatment for schizophrenia, does little to improve social dysfunction (Patterson & Leeuwenkamp, 2008). Social dysfunction often leads to social isolation, which could have more adverse effects on the patient than any of the symptoms on their own (Morrison & Bellack, 1987). Identifying the primary contributors to social dysfunction is thus the focus of much current research. An impairment in affect recognition has been found to be related with poor social functioning (Hooker & Park, 2002), and has been extensively researched in schizophrenia. Patients have been found to be impaired in both the identification and discrimination of affect from facial expressions (Kohler et al., 2010). In addition to a general impairment, studies have found evidence for specific impairment, particularly in the recognition of negative emotions such as anger and fear (Bigelow et al., 2006; Kohler et al., 2003; van’t Wout et al., 2007). Studies point to a failure to attend to salient face regions like the eyes by schizophrenics as a possible 5 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA explanation for these impairments, or a more basic perceptual impairment in face processing (Doop & Park, 2009; Loughland, Williams & Gordon, 2002). However, beyond deficits in detecting emotion from faces, schizophrenic patients are also impaired in gleaning emotion from social scenes, gestures, and postures, so it seems that errors in the basic perception of faces cannot fully account for affect recognition impairment (Bigelow, et al., 2006; Toomey et al., 2002). The nonverbal cues integrated and used in making judgments of affect can also be used in making more complex social judgments such as that of social traits. These judgments are considered more complex because the traits themselves are often descriptions of a behavior pattern or prediction (e.g. trustworthy, honest). In other words, social trait judgments move beyond a term that captures the mental state of another, to behavioral consequences. Research in healthy populations suggests that we have a normative ability to make surprisingly accurate interpersonal judgments from very few nonverbal cues (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992; 1993). These judgments are quick and automatic social assessments of personality and physical traits (Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997). We constantly make these judgments during natural social interaction (think: first impressions), but they can be studied under laboratory conditions using the presentation of ‘thin slices’ of nonverbal behavior (faces, content-filtered voices, video clips) from one second to five minutes of recorded, expressive nonverbal behavior (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992). Within this range, studies have shown that the judgments are surprisingly accurate at as little as one second and further exposure length serves mainly to increase confidence in the judgments made at zero acquaintance (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992). This fascinating research on thin slice judgments provides insight into how we form first impressions and a basis for social interaction. In the past decade, this research has begun to 6 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA transfer into the schizophrenia population, with an eye to understanding its potential role in social dysfunction. Many of these studies have used trustworthiness ratings of unfamiliar, neutral faces to index the ability for patients to make these more complex social judgments (Baas, Aleman et al., 2008; Baas, van’t Wout et al., 2008; Couture et al., 2008; 2010; Hall et al., 2004; Haut & MacDonald, 2010; Hooker et al., 2011; Pinkham et al., 2008). These studies have generally found evidence for no group difference or a positive bias in the attribution of trustworthiness. However, recent studies by Hooker et al., Haut and MacDonald, and Pinkham et al. have found that this relationship may be influenced by the symptom profile of the patient. These findings still do not provide a complete account of the complex social judgment process in schizophrenia. Only Haut and MacDonald (2010) and Hall et al. (2004) used traits other than trustworthiness, including attractiveness, approachability, distinctiveness, and intelligence, to assess social judgment processing. Hall et al. approached the study of complex social judgment by measuring accuracy of these trait attributions, and found that patients were less accurate. However, the results lacked a description of what kinds of mistakes patients were making. The same study also combined emotion recognition and trait judgment to show that the same patients were impaired on both tasks, but separating these social cognition tasks precludes the investigation of how trait judgment is influenced by the displayed emotion in the stimulus. These studies solely used pictures of faces for their stimuli, as the majority of studies on affect recognition have done. Researchers should also explore the use of more ecologically valid stimuli such as short video clips, which would present more nonverbal cues. Finally, studies on complex social judgment have not properly discussed implications for social functioning. If trait judgment is indeed spared in schizophrenia, then this could potentially be used in social cognition training. 7 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA The present study attempted to take many of these concerns into account. In addition to picture stimuli this study used short video clips. By combining the affect recognition and social trait judgment tasks into one, the present study was also designed to answer the question of how perceived affect influences these trait judgments. We hypothesized that patients would be impaired in affect recognition relative to controls, and would particularly struggle with the more numerous but subtle emotional cues in the video stimuli. While we predicted that patients would not make abnormal trait judgments, we expected symptom presentation to play an important role in the types of judgments made, so special attention was paid to this during data analysis. Method Participants Twenty-one schizophrenia outpatients (SZ) who met the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia were recruited from a psychiatric facility in Nashville, TN. All of the patients were taking antipsychotic medication at the time of testing with a mean chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalent dose of approximately 375 mg (S.D. 277). Symptoms were assessed with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Overall & Gorham, 1962), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, 1984a), and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen, 1984b). Twenty healthy control participants (HC) were recruited by advertisements in the community. Exclusion criteria were neurological disorders, current or history of substance abuse, history of severe head injury, or age over 60 years for both SZ and HC. Controls were excluded for Axis I disorder or a family history of schizophrenia. Patients were excluded for comorbid Axis I disorder. Intelligence was estimated using the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982). All subjects reported having normal or corrected-to- 8 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA normal vision. After providing a description of the experiment to the participants, written informed consent, approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Review Board, was obtained. Participants were paid for paid for their time. SZ and HC were matched for age, sex, and race. Although HC had a higher estimated IQ and more years of education, both groups were well within the normal IQ range, and on average SZ participants had completed one year of college. IQ and education were controlled for in between-group analyses, as described below. Demographic characteristics for the two groups are outlined in Table 1. In addition, a younger group of healthy participants comprising of 17 healthy students (STU) enrolled at Vanderbilt University were recruited. The student group had 2 males and 15 females. The mean age was 19.6 (S.D. 1.9). The students were mostly Caucasian (12 Caucasian, 3 African-American, 2 other). Students were given course credit for their participation. Design and Procedure All participants completed a computerized social judgment task comprised of questions designed to assess basic face perception, facial affect recognition, and social trait judgment abilities. The experiment was run using PsyScope (http://psy.ck.sissa.it/) on a Macintosh computer. The task was divided into two parts, which differed by type of stimuli used (picture or video), and were counterbalanced across subjects. Social questionnaires were also given and are described below. Stimuli. All stimuli used were taken from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database (KDEF; Lundqvist & Litton, 1998) and The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT; McDonald et al., 2002). Both KDEF and TASIT provide standardized stimuli designed depict six emotions and neutral. TASIT was designed to assess social perception in individuals 9 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA with traumatic brain injury but has been used in schizophrenic populations to assess aspects of social cognition (Horan et al., 2009; Kern et al., 2009). Fifty-four color images from KDEF were used, and there were a total of six actors (three male, three female). Forty-two of these images depicted six emotions (happy, surprised, sad, angry, disgusted, afraid) and neutral, and were oriented straight to the camera. Twelve additional images were used for the face recognition task, and were from the same six actors displaying neutral emotion oriented at a 45 degree angle away from the camera left or right. All 54 images are of Caucasian faces. Thirty-five 15s-long color video clips were adapted from Part 1 of TASIT versions A and B. Part 1 is an emotional evaluation section, and both versions have 28 scenes (between 15 and 40s long) in which a total of nine actors perform scenes depicting six emotions (happy, surprised, sad, angry, revolted, anxious) and neutral. All scenes in Part 1 of TASIT involve either one or two actors. Two-person scenes comprise the minority of total scenes, and always have a principal actor on which the scene focuses. Scenes were selected for use in the present study by choosing an equal number of scenes per emotion category while minimizing the number of actors selected. The final stimuli included a total of five principal actors (two men, three women) in the seven emotion conditions. In all of the video scenes used, the actors were Caucasian. The most scenes included in the social judgment task in which an actor was the principal actor was eight, and the least was five. One two-person scene was included for each emotion type. TASIT had been previously converted to DVD (AVI format). Using a video editing software (TrakAxPC; http://www.trakax.com/software/pc/), the selected scenes were edited down to 15s, and converted to MOV files. A 15s block was randomly selected from the middle 10 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA of each full-length scene. The audio from each scene was removed during editing, in order to control for the verbal content. To align the use of the two types of stimuli and to avoid confusing the participant, the emotion label “revolted” from the TASIT was relabeled “disgusted” in the social judgment task. “Anxious” remained as the label and answer choice for fear-related videos, and “afraid” for face stimuli. This will be addressed further in the discussion section. Twelve screenshots of just the actors’ faces during neutral scenes were used in the face recognition task. Social Judgment Task. For the pictures part of the task, subjects viewed 42 colored frontal photographic images of emotional and neutral faces, presented for 1s on a computer screen and made judgments about each individual face. The subjects were prompted by the computer to make a seven-forced-choice answer by mouse-click to identify the emotion. Then the subjects were asked to make social trait judgments on attractiveness, trustworthiness, approachability, and intelligence of the actor by choosing between four choices ranging from very negative to very positive (for example: “very unattractive”, “somewhat unattractive”, “somewhat attractive”, “very attractive”) again by mouse-click. The trait judgments selected for the present study were drawn from those used in the Hall et al. (2004) study. Next, subjects completed six face recognition trials in which they were instructed to choose the face from the bottom row that was the same as the face in the top row, but rotated. There was a trial for each actor of the face stimuli. The test faces were always of neutral emotion, and oriented facing center. The distractors and target images, also of neutral emotion, were oriented 45 degrees to the left or right of center. The two distractor images on each trial were the remaining two actors of the same gender used in the face stimuli. Afterward, the subjects were instructed to rate the same six neutral faces for age by choosing one of the answers on the screen (19 or younger, 20-29, 30-39, 40-49, 50-59, 60 or older). 11 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA For the videos portion, subjects viewed 35 colored video-clips presented on the computer screen for their duration, 15s, and made the same judgments as above for the principal actor in each clip. Afterward, they completed a face recognition task similar to the one described above. The test and target images came from a different scene, with the target image being the actor’s face angled roughly 45 degrees to the left or right of center. Because there were only two male principal actors in the video stimuli, distractor images of a secondary actor were added. Next, the subject indicated age of the video actors shown with neutral faces in the same manner as described above for the pictures task. Affect recognition accuracy was assessed for each subject by stimulus type and emotion condition. Face recognition accuracy was assessed for each subject by stimulus type. A subject was excluded from analyses if he or she scored 0% on the picture or video face recognition stimuli, signaling a complete inability to perceive basic facial features. Two schizophrenia patients and one healthy control were excluded. Their results and demographic information are not included in the reported results. Perceived age of each picture and video stimulus was averaged for each participant group, and not judged by accuracy. Questionnaires. Participants in the three groups completed the Peters et al. Delusions Inventory (Peters et al., 2004) to measure delusional ideation. The STU and HC groups also completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire to index psychosis proneness (Raine, 1991). Additionally, the SZ and HC groups completed a social functioning interview using the SFS (Birchwood et al., 1990). Average SPQ and PDI are reported in Table 1. In the STU group, mean total SPQ was 16 (S.D. 9.8) and total PDI was 55.5 (S.D. 46.4). 12 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Results Data Analysis Accuracy for the facial recognition control tasks was averaged for the two stimulus conditions (pictures/videos) to produce an average accuracy for face recognition. Age judgment was averaged for the two stimulus conditions. Mean accuracy for affect recognition was recorded for each participant by stimulus type and affect condition. After coding trait judgment responses from 1-4 where 1 is least positive and 4 is most positive, mean judgment rating for traits was calculated by stimulus type, trait, and affect condition. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS after checking for data entry errors. Control Tasks T-tests were performed to measure group differences on face recognition and age judgment. There was no significant group difference for face recognition accuracy (t(39)=1.52, p>0.1). Age judgment was likewise not significant, although there was a trend toward significant group differences (t(39)=1.79, p>0.05). Patients did not judge age significantly different than healthy controls. The results from these control measures suggest that the participant groups did not differ in their ability to process facial features. Affect Recognition Repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare groups on mean accuracies for affect, with stimulus type (pictures/videos) as the within-subjects variable and diagnostic group the between-subjects variable. IQ and education were entered as covariates. There was significant effect of group (F(1,37)=21.4, p<0.001), whereby the SZ group performed more poorly than HC 13 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA (Figure 1). Average accuracy was lower in the video stimulus condition for both groups but there was no significant main effect of stimulus type (F(1,37)=1.67, p>0.05). This was probably because the HC group still performed well on the videos task, and was reflected by a significant interaction between group and stimulus type (F(1, 37)=4.8, p<0.05). Contrast analyses (t-tests) identified specific affect recognition impairments (Figure 2). In the pictures task, patients were significantly impaired in recognizing neutral (t(39)=2.3, p<0.05) and afraid (t(39)=2.7, p=0.01). In the videos task, patients were impaired in recognizing sad (t(39)=2.8, p<0.01), angry (t(39)=3.1, p<0.01), disgusted (t(39)=3.0, p<0.01) and anxious (t(39)=3.9, p<0.001). There were no main effects of IQ or education on affect recognition performance. Individual performance on the pictures and videos task was correlated in the SZ group (r=0.53, p<0.05), but not in either the HC or STU groups. Finally, we investigated the relationship between affect recognition performance and symptom severity in the patients. We found that alogia (poverty of speech) from the SANS is negatively correlated with affect recognition performance in both the pictures task (rs=-0.57, p<0.01) and videos task (rs=-0.47, p<0.05). Trait Judgment A repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare mean trait judgment ratings, with stimulus type, trait, and affect condition as the within-subjects variables, and group as the between-subjects variable. As for affect recognition analysis, IQ and education were entered as covariates. There was a main effect of stimulus condition (F(1, 37)=6.0, p<0.05) and no stimulus by group interaction; both SZ and HC judging traits more favorably in the videos condition. There was no main effect of trait (F(3, 35)=1.9, p>0.1), and no significant trait by group 14 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA interaction, though it was trending (F(3, 35)=2.7, p=0.06). Importantly, there was no main effect of group on trait judgments (F(1, 37)= 1.9, p >0.1), unlike affect recognition performance. There were no main effects of IQ or education on trait judgment. Subsequent pairwise comparisons using Bonferroni correction showed that trustworthiness and approachability ratings were not significantly different from one another (p>0.1), which was likely responsible for the lack of a main effect of trait. Intelligence was the highest rated trait across conditions, then trustworthiness and approachability. Attractiveness was rated the lowest. There was a significant main effect of emotion condition for ratings made (F(2.02, 74.75)=4.6, p<.05), with less favorable trait ratings made during negative affect stimulus conditions, on average. Relationship to clinical symptoms: In the pictures task, attractiveness (rs=0.51, p<0.05), trustworthiness (rs=.47, p<0.05), and approachability (rs=0.68, p<0.001), but not intelligence (rs=0.2, p >0.1) ratings were positively correlated with patients’ positive symptoms indexed by total SAPS (Figure 3). The delusions subscore of SAPS was likewise correlated with trustworthiness (rs=0.48, p<0.05) and approachability (rs=0.46, p<0.05), so it is likely that the delusions score played a significant role in driving the larger SAPS correlations. No significant correlations were found with symptoms ratings and trait judgments made in the videos condition. Relationship to social measures: Attractiveness ratings in pictures (rs= -0.56, p<0.01) and videos (rs= -0.48, p<0.05) conditions were negatively correlated with the independence performance subscale of the SFS. No other correlations were found with social functioning. Relationship to psychosis proneness: No relationship was found with total SPQ or its subscales in the HC. However, total SPQ scores in this group were very low. In the STU group, 15 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA which had a higher mean SPQ, approachability ratings from pictures (rs=0.74, p<0.001) and videos (rs=0.49, p<0.05), trustworthiness ratings from pictures (rs=0.6, p<0.01) and intelligence ratings from pictures (rs=0.6, p<0.05) were correlated with self-reported experience of unusual perceptual events (Figure 4). Additionally, total SPQ was significantly correlated with approachability ratings of picture stimuli (rs=0.524, p<0.05) but not videos. Delusional ideation: In healthy controls, total PDI was negatively correlated with trustworthiness ratings from pictures (rs=-0.56, p<0.05) (Figure 5a). This was the opposite relationship found between the same variables in the patients (rs=0.46, p<0.05) (Figure 5b). No significant correlations were found in the STU group with delusional ideation. Affect recognition and trait judgment relationship: To investigate how the affect recognition and trait judgment tasks were related, we performed a median split on the affect recognition performances in pictures and videos for each participant in all three participant groups, and compared the average trait judgments for low and high performers. The only significant difference found was in the SZ group, where low performers on the video affect recognition task rated approachability in the videos task more favorably relative to the high performers (F(1,16)=6.2, p<0.05). Discussion The results from the affect recognition task are generally in line with much of the previous research in that schizophrenics exhibited both general and specific negative impairments (Bigelow et al., 2006; Kohler et al., 2010; van’t Wout et al., 2007). The impairment in affect recognition from the video stimuli adds to the relatively sparse literature on affect recognition beyond simple face stimuli. However, it is not clear if the worse performance by 16 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA schizophrenic participants in the videos task relative to the pictures task was the result of increased number of social cues, stimulus duration, decreased affective intensity, or some combination of these factors. Importantly, since the videos task is arguably closer to real world interpersonal situations than the pictures task, it seems likely that the performance on the video task may better reflect the patients’ performance in the outside world. Future studies should investigate this by manipulating specific stimulus parameters and measuring the gains or losses in affect recognition performance. The display of the affective cues in the videos condition seemed to be considerably less intense than in the pictures condition. It can be argued that this is a natural and necessary step toward more ecologically valid stimuli. However, the perception of “anxious” and “afraid” may differ by more than just expression intensity, but due to restraints posed by the chosen stimuli, these conditions were matched during analyses as theoretically similar affect conditions. As a precaution, a more conservative analysis without the anxious/afraid affect conditions was performed and did not yield different results of main effects, so it is unlikely that this significantly impacted the findings in this paper. The important finding of no group difference in the overall judgment of social traits is also in line with previous research (Baas, Aleman, et al., 2008; Couture et al., 2008; 2010; Haut & MacDonald, 2010; Hooker et al., 2011). Schizophrenic patients made similar complex social judgments as controls, across two types of stimuli, seven emotion conditions, and four traits. Few studies in schizophrenia and especially those focusing on social cognition find evidence for a spared cognitive process so this is an exciting finding. These findings point toward a dissociation in social judgments, where patients are impaired in judging affect but not social traits from the same stimuli. Functional imaging studies have found that the two processes rely on partially 17 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA nonoverlapping brain circuits (Heberlein et al., 2004), with emotion recognition activating somatosensory cortex and personality judgments activating frontal operculum (Heberlein & Saxe, 2005). Other imaging studies have implicated the amygdala in both processes (Adolphs, 2001). This study is the first to have an experimental paradigm designed to properly investigate this relationship in a behavioral task. Future studies, particularly those focusing on improving social cognition through training, should parse this relationship further. While both processes use nonverbal cues from the face regions, judgments of social traits, particularly physical traits such as attractiveness, are based more on size and spacing of facial features as well as skin complexion (Paunonen et al., 1999). Judgments of affect often focus within individual regions such as the shape of the mouth, lips, or eyebrows (Loughland, Williams & Gordon, 2002). Patients could potentially be retrained to form affect judgments from trait judgment-related facial cues, and improve their ability to recognize facial affect. The findings from the face recognition and age judgment control tasks lead us to conclude that basic perceptual processing of faces was not impaired in patients. While the group difference in the age judgment task approached significance, this is probably reflective of the task difficulty and not abnormal perceptual processing. In previous studies which used age judgment as a control, participants are typically asked to make a dichotomous forced-choice between “young” and “old” or “above 30” or “below 30” (Hall et al, 2004; Pinkham et al., 2008). In the present study, participants had to choose between six response options, making the task considerably more difficult. Furthermore, the finding of no group difference in the trait judgment of trustworthiness, approachability, attractiveness, and intelligence, suggests that basic perceptual processing was indeed intact. 18 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA While there was no group difference in trait judgment in the patient group, we found an important and possibly mediating relationship with positive symptoms. Patients with severe positive symptoms made more favorable ratings of attractiveness, trustworthiness, and approachability. A similar positive bias in trustworthiness judgment has been found for an entire patient group (Baas, van’t Wout et al., 2008) and in a subset of patients with higher disorganized symptoms (Hooker et al., 2011). This positive correlation with symptoms and trait judgment carries over to delusional ideation and trait judgment. Schizophrenics with higher delusional ideation were more favorable in their approachability judgments, whereas healthy controls with high delusional ideation tended to be more cautious in these judgments. Interestingly, healthy young people who report unusual perceptual experiences on a psychosis-proneness measure show a similar positive bias. This suggests perhaps that this bias is related more toward the disorder and vulnerability for the disorder than the presence of delusions and delusional ideation. Research in schizophrenia beyond social trait judgment has also found a similar positive bias. People with schizophrenia report higher preference ratings for everyday things, including hedonic judgment of food (Folley & Park, 2010) and smell (Cumming, Matthews & Park, in press; Doop & Park, 2006). However, this positive bias does not necessarily benefit the patient. For instance, more positively biased social traits of others during negative interpersonal situations misinform and perhaps underprepare the patient for adverse social interaction. This could increase the amount of objectively unsuccessful social interactions, where there are negative social consequences. Additionally, a positive bias in judging people seemingly sets high expectations for a given social interaction. These expectations, of course, will not always be met and in fact will not be met at a higher rate than someone who has lower expectations. This could lead to more subjectively unsuccessful social interactions. In this sense, a positive bias may 19 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA contribute to poor social functioning. However, the present study only found such a negative relationship with attractiveness ratings. Future studies should investigate this relationship in greater detail. Correlations between symptoms and delusional ideation in the pictures task were not also present in the videos task. That is, the patients with more severe positive symptoms did not show a positive bias in the videos condition. In the pictures condition, the same small number of nonverbal cues was presented briefly to the subject. In the videos condition, more nonverbal cues were presented to the participant, and new cues appeared as the video continued. Perhaps a positive bias in social trait judgment is most apparent when there are few social cues to draw upon in making these judgments, and as more and more cues are presented, the positive bias is masked by a normal ability to integrate these cues into trait judgments. This would explain why the patients with severe positive symptoms made normal judgments in the videos condition, but future studies could test this by having patients updating these trait judgments as more nonverbal cues are presented in a dynamic stimulus. This finding underlies the importance of using more ecologically valid stimuli to measure social cognition, and suggests that simple paradigms using just static faces are less useful in describing the judgment process during real-life interaction. By combining two processes of social judgment, expanding the stimuli used in order to approach similarity to real-life conditions, and exploring attribution of multiple traits, this study provides an intriguing way to understand the social judgment process in schizophrenia. The finding of a dissociation of affect recognition and social trait judgment could be useful in social cognition training efforts, and evidence for a positive bias in more positively symptomatic patients sheds light on this social trait judgment process in schizophrenia as well as a potential line of future research. 20 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA References Adolphs, R. (2001). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3(12), 469-479. Andreasen, N.C. (1984a) Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa. --- (1984b) Modified Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa. Ambady, N. & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431-441. Ambady, N. & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256-274. Baas, D., Aleman, A., Vink, M., Ramsey, N.F., de Haan, E.H.F., & Kahn, R.S. (2008). Evidence of altered cortical and amygdala activation during social decision-making in schizophrenia. NeuroImage, 40, 719-727. Baas, D., van’t Wout, M., Aleman, A., & Kahn, R.S. (2008). Social judgment in clinically stable patients with schizophrenia and healthy relatives: Behavioral evidence of social brain dysfunction. Psychological Medicine, 38, 747-754. Bellack, A.S., Morrison, R.L., Wixted, J.T., & Mueser, K.T. (1990). An analysis of social competence in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 809-818. Bigelow, N.O., Paradiso, S., Adolphs, R., Moser, D.J., Arndt, S., Heberein, A., Nopoulos, P., & Andreasen, N.C. (2006). Perception of socially relevant stimuli in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 83, 257-267. Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Cochrane, R., Wetton, S., & Copestake, S. (1990). The social functioning scale: The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 853-859. Couture, S.M., Penn, D.L., Addington, J., Woods, S.W., & Perkins, D.O. (2008). Assessment of social judgments and complex mental states in the early phases of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 100(1-3), 237-241. Couture, S.M., Penn, D.L., Losh, M., Adolps, M., Hurley, R., & Piven, J. (2010). Comparison of social cognitive functioning in schizophrenia and high functioning autism: More convergence than divergence. Psychological Medicine, 40(4), 569-579. 21 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Cumming, A., Matthews, N.M., & Park, S. (in press). Olfactory identification and preference in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. Doop, M.L. & Park, S. (2006). On knowing and judging smells: Identification and hedonic judgment of odors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 81, 317-319. Doop, M.L. & Park, S. (2009). Facial expression and face orientation processing in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 170(2), 103-107. Funder, D.C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652-670. Gold, J.M. & Harvey, P.D. (1993). Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 16(2), 295-312. Hall, J., Harris, J.M., Sprengelmeyer, R., Sprengelmeyer, A., Young, A., Santos, I.M., Johnstone, E.C., & Lawrie, S.M. (2004). Social cognition and face processing in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 169-170. Haut, K.M. & MacDonald, A.W. (2010). Persecutory delusions and the perception of trustworthiness in unfamiliar faces in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 178, 456-460. Heberlein, A.S., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2004). Cortical regions for judgments of emotions and personality traits from point-light walkers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1143-1158. Heberlein, A.S. & Saxe, R.R. (2005). Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 28, 770-777. Hooker, C. & Park, S. (2002). Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research, 112(1): 41-50. Hooker, C.I., Tulley, L.M., Verosky, S.C., Fisher, M., Holland, C., & Vinogradov, S. (2011). Can I trust you? Negative affective priming influences social judgments in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(1), 98-107. Horan, W.P, Kern, R.S., Shokat-Fadai, K, Sergi, M.J., Wynn, J.K., & Green, M.F. (2009). Social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia: An initial efficacy study of stabilized outpatients. Schizophrenia Research, 107, 47-54. Jones, P.B., Bebbington, P., Forester, A., Lewis, S.W., Murray, R.M., Russell, A., Sham, P.C., Toone, B.K., & Wilkins, S. (1993). Premorbid social underachievement in schizophrenia: Results from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 65-71. 22 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Kern, R.S., Green, M.F., Fiske, A.P., Kee, K.S., Lee, J., Sergi, M.J., Horan, W.P., Subotnik, K.L., Sugar, C.A., & Nuechterlein, K, H. (2009). Theory of mind deficits for processing counterfactual information in persons with chronic schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 39, 645-654. Kohler, C.G., Turner, T.H., Bilker, W.B., Brensinger, C.M., Siegel, S.J., Kanes, S.J., Gur, R.E., & Gur, R.C. (2003). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: Intensity effects and error pattern. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1768-1774. Kohler, C.G., Walker, J.B., Martin, E.A., Healey, K.M., & Moberg, P.J. (2010). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 1009-1019. Lee, J. & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 599-611. Loughland, C.M. Williams, L.M., & Gordon, E. (2002). Visual scanpaths to positive and negative facial emotions in an outpatient schizophrenia sample. Schizophrenia Research, 55, 159-170. Lundqvist, D. & Litton, J. E. (1998). The Karolinska directed faces. Karolinska Institute. Mandal, M.K., Pandey, R., & Prasad, A.B. (1998). Facial expressions of emotions and schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24(3), 399-412. McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., & Kinch, J. (2002). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 219–238. Morrison, R.L. & Bellack, A.S. (1987). Social functioning in schizophrenic patients: Clinical and research issues. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(4), 715-725. van Os, J. & Kapur, S. (2009). Schizophrenia. Lancet, 374, 635-645. Overall, J.E. & Gorham, D.R. (1962). The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Reports, 10, 799-812. Patterson, T.L. & Leeuwenkamp, O.R. (2008). Adjunctive psychosocial therapies for the treatment of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 100(1), 108-119. Paunonen, S.V., Ewan, K., Earthy, J., Lefave, S., & Goldberg, H. (1999). Facial features as personality cues. Journal of Personality, 67(3), 555-583. Peters, E., Joseph, S., Day, S., & Gary, P. (2004). Measuring delusional ideation: The 21-item Peters et al. delusions inventory. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(4), 1005-1022. 23 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Pinkham, A.E., Hopfinger, J.B., Pelphrey, K.A., Piven, J., & Penn, D.L. (2008). Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 99(1-3), 164-175. Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4): 555-564. Toomey, R., Schuldberg, D., Corrigan, P., & Green, M.F. (2002). Nonverbal social perception and symptomatology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 53(1-2), 83-91. van’t Wout, M., Aleman, A., Kessels, R.P.C., Cahn, W., de Haan, E.H.F., & Kahn, R.S. (2007). Exploring the nature of facial affect processing deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 150(3), 227-235. Young, A.R. & McGorry, P.D. (1996). The prodromal phase of first-episode psychosis: past and current conceptualizations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22(2), 353-370. Zebrowitz, L.A. & Collins, M.A. (1997). Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 203222. Zigler, E. & Phillips, L. (1960). Social competence and outcome in psychiatric disorder. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 264-271. 24 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Table 1: Participant demographics Figure 1: Performance on pictures and videos task in HC (gray) and SZ (black), with standard deviation bars. Significant group differences found in both tasks. Patients performed worse on videos task than pictures. Figure 2: (a) Performance on the pictures task by emotion condition in HC (gray) and SZ (black), with standard deviation bars. Specific group differences in neutral and afraid conditions. (b) Performance on the videos task by emotion condition in HC (gray) and SZ (black), with standard deviation bars. Specific group differences in sad, angry, anxious, and disgusted conditions. Figure 3: Positive correlations between increasing total SAPS score on x-axis and increasing favorability of trait judgment on y-axis. (a) Attractiveness ratings vs. total SAPS score (rs= 0.51, p<0.05). (b) Approachability ratings vs. total SAPS score (rs=0.68, p<0.001). (c) Trustworthiness ratings vs. total SAPS score (rs=0.47, p<0.05). Figure 4: Trait judgments made by STU vs. unusual perceptual experiences score of SPQ. (a) Approachability ratings from pictures (rs=0.74, p<0.001). (b) Approachability ratings from videos (rs=0.49, p<0.05). (c) Trustworthiness ratings from pictures (rs=0.6, p<0.01). (d) Intelligence ratings from pictures (rs=0.6, p<0.01). Figure 5: Trustworthiness judgment and delusional ideation. (a) Trustworthiness ratings from pictures vs. HC total PDI (rs=0.56, p<0.05) (b) Trustworthiness ratings from pictures vs. SZ total PDI (rs=0.46, p<0.05). 25 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Table 1 Schizophrenia patients Mean (S.D.) Healthy controls Mean (S.D.) N 21 20 -- Sex (Male/Female) 13/8 10/10 x2= 0.6, p> 0.1 Age (years) 40.6 (8.1) 38 (8.7) t= 1.0, p> 0.1 Education (years) 13.7 (2.6) 15.5 (2.3) t= 2.41, p= 0.02 NART IQ 101.1 (10.4) 107.2 (7.5) t= 2.12, p= 0.04 6 13 0 1 1 11 7 1 1 0 BPRS SAPS SANS 13.1 (7.4) 15.7 (8.6) 22.0 (15.5) -- -- Total PDI 104.3 (77.6) 37.4 (30.9) t= 3.59, p< 0.01 Total SPQ -- 8.7 (8.3) -- Significance Ethnicity Caucasian African American Hispanic Asian Multi-race x2= 6.0, p> 0.1 Clinical Symptoms 26 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Figure 1 27 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Figure 2 28 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Figure 3 (a) (b) (c) 29 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Figure 4 (a) (c) (b) (d) 30 COMPLEX SOCIAL JUDGMENT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Figure 5 (a) (b) 31