THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 384:109–126 (1997) Local Circuit Neurons of Macaque Monkey Striate Cortex: IV. Neurons of Laminae 1–3A JENNIFER S. LUND* AND CHARLES Q. WU Department of Visual Science, Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London EC1V 9EL, United Kingdom ABSTRACT We continue our Golgi studies (Lund [1987] J. Comp. Neurol. 257:60–92; Lund et al. [1988] J. Comp. Neurol. 276:1–29; Lund and Yoshioka [1991] J. Comp. Neurol. 331:234–258) of the organization of local circuit, largely g-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-containing neurons in macaque monkey visual cortex, area V1, with this account of the local circuit neurons lying in layers 1 and 2/3A. These layers receive intrinsic interlaminar excitatory and inhibitory relays from layers 3B, 4A, 4B, and 5. We describe seven varieties of local circuit neurons with somata within layers 1-2/3A, and we compare the lateral scale of spread of the axons and dendrites of these neurons with the size of the columnar connectional patch domains made by the laterally spreading axon collaterals of pyramidal neurons within the superficial layers (Lund et al. [1993] Cerebral Cortex 3:148–162). We conclude from this comparison that all of the neurons have dendritic fields that are limited to single patch domains. Furthermore, only two of the seven local circuit neuron varieties have sufficient axon spread to influence territory beyond single domains, reaching into neighboring territory likely to differ in function from that occupied by their dendrites. We have identified descending projections from particular varieties to layers 3B, 4A, 4B, and 5 and to the white matter. We discuss the contributions that these interneurons may make to function within the superficial cortical layers, and we summarize our overall conclusions, so far, from our set of studies on interneurons within area V1 of the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 384:109–126, 1997. r 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Indexing terms: vision; inhibition; interneuron; visual cortex; primate This study represents a further step in our investigation (Lund, 1987; Lund et al., 1988; Lund and Yoshioka, 1991) of the morphology of local circuit neurons in the macaque monkey primary visual cortex (area V1 or striate cortex) as seen in Golgi-rapid impregnations (Lund, 1973). In our previous studies of layers 3B–6, we have documented the presence in each layer of several different forms of these neurons, which generally contain g-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and are presumed to be inhibitory (Ribak, 1978; Houser et al., 1984). We have shown that their axons make a local or laterally extended intralaminar arbor and, in many cases, also make very specific interlaminar projections. Some of these local circuit neuron projections follow the patterns of projections made by different populations of pyramidal and spiny stellate neurons. The local circuit neurons also provide feedback pathways and links between different channels of information flow. We have pointed out that the lateral spread of the axons of the interneurons, and, thus, their spatial relationships to the topography of function across the cortex, may be an important element in determining their different roles. r 1997 WILEY-LISS, INC. Below, we briefly review anatomical features of layers 1–3A that are either shared or unique to particular divisions; it is clear that this territory is divided both vertically and laterally into a number of different compartments, and this will bear on our later discussion of possible functional roles of the interneuron populations described. The laminar numbering scheme we have used for V1 cortex is shown in Figure 1. Layer 1 is sparse in cells (at least 80% of which are GABAergic; Fitzpatrick et al., 1987) and contains almost no somata or basal dendrites of pyramidal neurons, although it does contain a rich plexus of apical dendritic processes of pyramidal cells with so- Grant sponsor: National Eye Institute; Grant number: EY10021; Grant sponsor: Medical Research Council; Grant numbers: G9203679N, G9408137. *Correspondence to: Professor Jennifer S. Lund, Department of Visual Science, Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, 11-43 Bath Street, London EC1V 9EL, United Kingdom. E-mail: j.lund@ucl.ac.uk Received 22 October 1996; Revised 16 January 1997; Accepted 28 February 1997 110 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 1. Cresyl violet-stained (A) and cytochrome oxidase (CO)reacted (B) 40 µm sections from parafoveal macaque monkey V1 cortex to illustrate the laminar numbering scheme used in this study. The border between layers 3A and 3B is placed halfway between the upper border of 4B and the lower border of layer 1. The border between layers 3A and 2 is placed halfway between the base of layer 1 and the top of layer 3B. In B, arrows indicate the positions of two CO-rich blob regions. Scale bar 5 200 µm. mata in underlying layers down to layer 5B (see Fig. 10B). There are direct extrinsic afferents to layer 1 from the intercalated layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN; Fitzpatrick et al., 1983; Lachica and Casagrande, 1992) and from nonspecific thalamic nuclei and other subcortical regions (see Tigges and Tigges, 1985; Herkenham, 1986). Layer 1 shares input from the pulvinar with layers 2 and 3A (Ogren and Hendrickson, 1977; Rezak and Benevento, 1979). Feedback pathways from extrastriate cortex also enters layers 1 and 2 (Rockland and Virga, 1989; Rockland and Van Hoesen, 1994; Rockland et al., 1994). Layers 2 and 3A are rich in somata and basal and apical dendritic processes of pyramidal neurons (about 80% pyramids, 20% GABAergic interneurons; Fitzpatrick et al., 1987); the region does not appear to receive much in the way of thalamic afferents; projections of the LGN intercalated layers to the blobs [cytochrome-oxidase (CO)rich regions] concentrate their terminal fields deeper in layer 3B (Fitzpatrick et al., 1983; Lachica and Casagrande, 1992). Importantly, layers 2/3A are not innervated directly by relays of excitatory spiny stellate neurons from layer 4C, which distinguishes the region from underlying layer 3B—a major recipient of these relays (Blasdel et al., 1985; Fitzpatrick et al., 1985; Yoshioka et al., 1994). Dendritic processes of layer 3B pyramidal neurons, driven by these direct thalamic inputs, rise up into overlying layers 2/3A and 1 and express a rich CO content, so that the blob zone extends up into layer 1; many other dendritic and axonal processes from both interneurons and pyramidal cells of all layers except 4C enter the neuropil of the 2/3A region (summarized in Fig. 10B). In this study, although it was clear that layer 1 differed in its interneuron constituents from layers 2 and 3A, no sharp difference in interneuron varieties were found between layers 2 and 3A. There are a number of differences between the two regions; the cell packing density in layer 2 can appear greater than in layer 3A in tissue stained for cytoarchitecture; layer 3A, rather than layer 2, is a princi- LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX pal source of efferent projections to area V2 (Rockland and Pandya, 1979), and layer 3A does not share feedback terminations from visual cortical areas V2, V4, and TEO with layers 1 and 2 (Rockland and Van Hoesen, 1994; Rockland et al., 1994). Despite their differences, layers 1-2/3A share input from laterally spreading, rising relays from layer 3B pyramids together with input from pyramids in layer 5. These inputs are reinforced by laterally spreading collaterals from the axons of the pyramids of the 2/3A layer itself; these pyramidal neuron connections form a laterally running, lattice-like continuum of patchy terminal fields in the superficial layers, which largely (70% match) link regions of common function (Rockland and Lund, 1983; Malach et al., 1993; Yoshioka et al., 1996). This connectional continuum runs without apparent interruption across the borders of at least two fixed-place blob and interblob compartments; in these compartments lie segregated populations of efferent neurons; pyramids in the blob regions relay to thin, CO-rich stripe compartments, and pyramids in the interblob regions project to CO-poor stripe compartments of V2 (Livingstone and Hubel, 1984). There is no clear evidence that blobs and interblobs have different interneuron populations, although the level of neurotransmitter in axon terminals and other features of the neuron biochemistry may differ between these regions, most probably due to different activity levels in the thalamic-driven blob zones compared with interblob territory (see, e.g., Fitzpatrick et al., 1987). The current Golgi study provides a morphological description of the varieties of interneurons encountered in our Golgi impregnations together with measurements of the lateral spread of their axonal and dendritic arbors as well as any interlaminar projections observed. Possible relationships of the inhibitory neurons to lattice functions and intrinsic feed-forward inhibition are discussed, and comparison is made with interneurons of other cortical areas and different species. A preliminary report of this study has appeared earlier (Lund and Wu, 1993). MATERIALS AND METHODS The reader is referred to our earlier papers (Lund, 1973; Lund, 1987; Lund et al., 1988; Lund and Yoshioka, 1991) for details of the Golgi method and our general approach to tissue analysis. An illustration of our laminar numbering system for area V1 is given in Figure 1; layers 2/3A occupy the upper half of the cortical depth between the top of layer 4B and the base of layer 1 (these boundaries are usually recognizable in the Golgi-rapid impregnations), the lower half comprising layers 3B and 4A; layer 2 and layer 3A each occupy half of this upper territory. Tissue from the same series of infant and adult macaque monkeys (M. rhesus and M. nemestrina) has been used in the current study, as it was in our earlier studies, and an extensive search was made by light microscopy through many sections (cut at 90 µm thickness) of a wide age range of animals for repeated examples of morphologically distinct cell types. Partial drawings were made of nearly every interneuron encountered until it was clear that examples of neurons of similar morphology were regularly encountered; then, detailed drawings were prepared of the most complete impregnations of five to ten individual cells of that variety. These drawings then served as the template against which 111 other cells were compared to see whether they resembled or differed from that variety. If cells appeared to differ in morphology from these template drawings of distinct varieties, perhaps appearing as intermediate forms between two varieties or a new variety, then detailed drawings were made of these individual cells for comparison with the sets of neurons already placed in distinct categories. In this way, we placed each neuron encountered into a particular category. Very occasionally, we failed to make any matches to a single cell encountered (the wide-arbor cell described in the following section falls into that category); however, examples of most varieties were regularly encountered, with at least two or three examples in each of the seven to ten blocks sectioned from each hemisphere, despite interanimal variation in frequency of impregnation of particular varieties. In some cases, our categories were clearly those of other workers on cortical anatomy (e.g., chandelier, basket, and neurogliaform cells). In some cases, we could not make a clear distinction between two varieties described by others (double-bouquet and bipolar), and we placed these neurons in a single class. In other cases, our varieties had not been described previously by others in a manner that clearly matched our sample; in these cases, however, we were able to match the variety quite well to similar forms in macaque prefrontal cortex in our own studies (columnar, medium-arbor, and simple beaded varieties; Lund and Lewis, 1993). We believe that each of our varieties is distinctly different in morphology from the others, without intermediate forms. However, this does not preclude the existence of additional forms that we have failed to impregnate or the existence of differences in single varieties other than those revealed in Golgi impregnations. It should be noted that Golgi impregnations are very much more successful in juvenile animals than in adults (where myelination of the axons makes impregnation difficult), and our illustration of cell types are from young animals in which both axon and dendrites of single cells are impregnated; however, the same cells are present in the Golgi preparations from adults, with only minor differences in axon and dendritic form but less complete in impregnation of the entire cell. In young animals, the dendrites of the local circuit neurons may bear many small, thorn-like protrusions or spines, and this feature will be noticeable in the illustrations of local circuit neurons from juvenile animals illustrated in this study; in the adult, the dendrites become smooth or only sparsely spined, with some variation between individual cells of even single varieties within single animals in the degree to which these dendritic appendages are shed. Dimensions of cells, as measured in the Golgi-rapid preparations, were compared with dimensions of connectional patterns seen in V1 frozen tissue sections reacted for biocytin, as reported in other studies (Lund et al., 1993; Yoshioka et al., 1996). These preparations have very similar shrinkage factors; the tissue for both is taken from brains perfused with the same 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer, and the Golgi-rapid osmium stage prevents further shrinkage. RESULTS In our search for interneurons, we examined all areas of V1, from the foveal representation to the far periphery. We could find no evidence for particular varieties of interneu- 112 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 2. A: Layer 1 neuron, variety 1:1. Axon arbor is on the right, dendritic field is on the left. B: Chandelier neuron, variety 2/3A:1. Axon arbor is on the right, dendritic arbor is on the left. Camera lucida drawing of Golgi-rapid impregnations from a 5-week-old M. nemestrina. For both A and B, the axon is drawn separately from the dendritic field for clarity. Large arrowheads indicate offset axons. Both cells are from the outer operculum of area V1. In this and subsequent figures, the laminar boundaries are indicated by small arrowheads, and numbers indicate each laminar identity. Scale bar 5 50 µm. rons being restricted to peripheral or central visual field representations, with the one reservation that only one example was found of the wide-arbor basket neuron variety (see below) in layer 2-3 (in the parafoveal cortex). Examples of the other varieties were found at all eccentricities. they were not impregnated in the tissue from postnatal ages examined in this study. A single morphological type of interneuron is regularly impregnated with somata in layer 1 of our infants and more mature tissue. Layer 1 neurons Layer 1 of at least the juvenile monkey may contain Cajal-Retzius neurons (Marin-Padilla, 1984). Although these neurons are impregnated in our prenatal material, Layer 1: Variety 1 This neuron (Fig. 2A) has a dendritic field spread of 250–300 µm, some portion of which may spread into layer 2. Its axon rapidly divides on emerging from the soma to form a dense field of highly convoluted, beaded (presumed LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX 113 Fig. 3. A,B: Columnar cells, variety 2/3A:2. The axon field is drawn separately (in each case, to the left of the dendritic field) from the dendritic field for each neuron for clarity. Golgi-rapid impregnations from a 5-week-old M. nemestrina. Both cells are from the outer operculum of area V1. Scale bar 5 50 µm. to be sites of synapses) processes within layer 1, with a spread of approximately 300–350 µm. Occasionally, the soma may lie in layer 2, but the axon rises to establish the bulk of its arbor in overlying layer 1. Layer 2/3A neurons The first two cell types we describe, the chandelier neuron and the columnar cell, are also found with somata in the deeper regions of the superficial cortex, layers 4B, 4A, and 3B, and their axons and dendrites flow vertically across all laminar boundaries between the base of 4B and the base of layer 1. We have also observed chandelier neurons in upper layer 4C-a and in layer 5 (Lund, 1987; Lund et al., 1988). 2/3A variety 1: Chandelier cells. Whereas the dendrites of the more superficially placed neurons may cross into layer 1, their axons do not enter layer 1; this limitation of the axon arbor distribution is consistent with the distribution of their synaptic targets, the initial axon segments of pyramidal cells (Somogyi, 1977), which do not occur in layer 1. The axon field is generally centered around the dendritic field and spreads laterally no more than 300 µm: generally, 150–250 µm wide. The characteristic vertically arrayed, synaptic cartridges occur evenly distributed across the axon field. The dendritic field is more tightly focused than the axon arbor, with the dendrites forming a narrow columnar field under 100 µm wide (Somogyi, 1977; Peters, 1984b; see Fig. 2B). 2/3A variety 2: Columnar cells. The cell bodies of these neurons (around 17 µm in diameter) are generally within layer 3A, i.e., the lower half of the 2/3A zone, and only rarely as high as layer 2 (Lund and Yoshioka, 1991; see Fig. 3). The dendrites form a narrow columnar field, averaging about 80 µm in diameter. Roughly equal numbers of dendrites extend into territory above and below the soma, and the more superficial segments may enter layer 1. The axon emerges as a stout trunk on the pial side of the soma and rises to the base of layer 1 without entering it. The axon gives off laterally running beaded collaterals along its length, and the terminal field forms a column around 250–350 µm wide; the dilations, which are assumed to be sites of synaptic contact, are distributed fairly evenly, if sparsely, within the axon arbor. One or two stout trunks also descend from the initial axon trunk, and collateral branches can be given off in layers 3B, 4A, and 4B, whereas the trunks continue downward at least as far as layer 5B. The main bulk of the axon collaterals are found on a level with and to the pial side of the soma position. 2/3A variety 3: Wide-arbor basket cells. Although many examples have been found of the other varieties of cells described in this Golgi study, only one example of this cell type in layer 2/3A has been found (Jones and Hendry, 1984; see Fig. 4). Because its lateral axonal spread is the largest of any Macaque monkey Golgi-impregnated interneuron we have found so far in area V1, we include its 114 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 4. Wide-arbor basket neuron, variety 2/3A:3. The axon is drawn separately to the right of the dendritic field for clarity. This neuron was the only one of its type that we encountered in our Golgi-rapid impregnations of layer 2/3A. From a 3-week-old M. nemestrina in the outer operculum of V1. Scale bar 5 100 µm. description here, because, if it is a consistent part of the neuropil, then it may exert great impact on the range of direct, laterally spreading inhibition in the superficial cortex. The soma (25 µm in diameter) lies at the 2/3A border. The bulk of the dendrites of this cell form a columnar arbor largely within a zone 100 µm wide above the soma in layers 1-2/3A. The axon emerges basally and descends at least as far as layer 5B. Beaded collaterals are given off this trunk around the dendritic field to establish a local dense arbor about 400 µm wide centered around the soma; in addition, a few stout, lateral trunks travel more distantly to about 580 µm from the soma in layer 2/3, giving off short, lateral, beaded terminal branches. The long lateral trunks distribute as separated ‘‘arms,’’ giving unevenness in terminal distribution around the cell; one of these trunks was traced into layer 1, where it traveled at least 1.5 mm from the soma, emitting sparse, lateral branches to layer 2. There was no clear evidence of patching in the distribution of its axon terminals. 2/3A variety 4: Neurogliaform or spider-web cells. These cells (Jones, 1984; see Fig. 5) are found occasionally in superficial layers 2/3A in our impregnations. They are identified particularly by their numerous but short dendrites (dendritic field approximately 150 µm wide) and their fine, locally distributed, densely branched axon field (about 200 µm wide). 2/3A variety 5: Cells with simple beaded axons. The vertically oriented dendrites form an arbor within layers 1–3A about 100 µm wide or less (Fig. 6). The axon generally emerges apically and forms an interweaving cascade of slender, beaded processes adjacent to and somewhat wider (200–250 µm) than the dendritic field but with little intrusion on layer 1. Stout axon trunks begin a descent to deeper layers, but their impregnation ceases in our material within layer 3B (perhaps because of myelination), and, so far, the destination of these descending trunks not been traceable in our material. 2/3A variety 6: Medium-arbor neurons. This cell type has not been found in layers 3B-4B but is a regularly occurring component of layer 2/3A (Fig. 7). The soma generally lies in layer 3A; the dendrites (see Fig. 7A, dendrites drawn together with the axon) are largely vertically oriented, with a total arbor width 125–150 µm, and are distributed within layers 3A-2 with little intrusion into layer 1. The robust axon emerges basally from the soma and rapidly divides into recurrent, spreading branches of linear, beaded terminal processes with a total spread of 500–700 µm; the axon resembles that of basket neurons but does not form pericellular basket arrays. The whole arbor has an inverted cone shape that is widest near the top of layer 2, with almost no intrusion into layer 1, within which terminals appear evenly distributed. 2/3A variety 7: Cells with narrowly focused, vertically oriented axon fields (double-bouquet or bipolar cells). The somata can lie at any level in layers 2/3A (Somogyi and Cowey, 1984; Peters, 1984a; Figs. 8, 9). The dendrites of these neurons are mainly vertically oriented with occasional laterally spreading branches, and the main dendritic field is around 100 µm wide. The dendrites of our impregnated cells are only occasionally truly bipolar in origin (i.e., arising from two dendritic trunks, arising from apical and basal sides of the soma; see Fig. 9A); more usually, the cells are multipolar. The dendrites can show slight intrusion into layer 1, but the bulk of the dendritic field lies in layers 2/3A. The axon emerges from any point on the soma and ascends or descends, quickly dividing into multiple, vertically oriented, fine collaterals that together form a narrow column. On the pial side of the soma, the axon trunks gives off multiple collaterals to form a closeknit column of interweaving, beaded processes, often with long side spines, that spreads through layers 2 and 1 (see Fig. 8). In layers 3A-3B, the arbor is largely restricted to a column of fine, vertical processes with prominent, spinelike side processes. The axon arbor width in layer 1 reaches up to 200 µm; in layer 2, occasional processes can reach up to 300 µm laterally, but the majority of processes are focused within a diameter of 100 µm; in deeper regions, the column of fine axon processes is generally less than 50 µm wide. In our impregnations, the descending axon LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX 115 most superficial region, layers 1–3A, of the macaque visual cortex; Figure 10A summarizes these findings, and Figure 10B summarizes other axonal and dendritic contributions that are made to layers 1–3A from neurons in other layers of V1. Relationship of interneurons to superficial layer pyramidal neuron connectional patch system Fig. 5. Neurogliaform (spider-web) neuron, variety 2/3A:4, from layer 2 of the calcarine floor region of V1 in a 5-week-old M. nemestrina; Golgi-rapid impregnation. The axon is drawn separately below the dendritic field for clarity. Scale bar 5 50 µm. arbors of single cells have reduced to no more than a single process passing below layer 4A, which then descends as far as layer 5 and occasionally to the white matter. Others (see Somogyi and Cowey, 1984) have reported a skein of fine axon collaterals from single cells forming a narrow column reaching to layer 5. We have been unable to make a clear distinction between bipolar and double-bouquet cells on the basis of different axon morphology; there is some variability in the morphology of the axons of these cells in our impregnations; the majority appear to have prominent side spine morphology, as described above, but others are finer and less decorated with appendages (see the cells in Fig. 9). It is therefore probable that this variety contains subclasses. DISCUSSION We have provided above a brief description of the interneurons observed in our Golgi preparations of the In the absence of any clear indication from other published studies that different classes of interneurons occupy the blob or interblob positions or are restricted to particular parts of the visual field representation in area V1, we will assume that all classes of interneurons that we have described are spread relatively evenly across the lateral extent of cortex. The absolute density of particular varieties is likely to vary on the basis of findings from immunocytochemical labeling of calcium-binding proteins and other biochemical labels for interneurons populations (DeFelipe, 1993); but, where electron microscopic (EM) studies have examined the issue (see, e.g., Somogyi et al., 1982; Kisvarday et al., 1993), there is overlap between axons of individual cells of the same variety on single target neurons. The degree of overlap in both vertical (pia to white matter) and lateral dimensions of the cortical sheet may well differ between varieties, given their different dimensions and densities. The scale of the laterally spreading, patchy lattice of pyramidal neuron connections (Rockland and Lund, 1983; Blasdel et al., 1985; Lund et al., 1993; Yoshioka et al., 1994, 1996; see Fig. 11A) in relation to the axon and dendritic arbor sizes of the local circuit neurons is of particular relevance with regard to how the superficial layer neuropil may function. Considering the fact that the patch system is a continuum and that each patch is only definable in terms of its connections to particular (perhaps unique) offset points of similar size, match in scale between the spread of a local circuit neuron’s axon and the pyramidal neuron connectional patch size will mean that any single interneuron of that variety will occupy the center of a patch, exerting a direct effect through the entire width of the single patch, i.e., over an area likely to be territory of similar function. If the axon is wider than the connectional patch size, then the interneuron activity will additionally affect neurons in territory of function unlike that surrounding the cell’s own dendritic field, even when the cell lies centered in the patch (Malach et al., 1993; Yoshioka et al., 1996; see Fig. 12, top). In addition, if the interneuron axon arbor is narrower than the pyramidal neuron connectional patch, then the cell can exert a narrowly focused effect even within a patch boundary. The influence of interneurons with somata offset to different degrees from the patch center on activity within the patch will gradually wane with distance of their offset with a geometry dependent upon the lateral diameter of their axon. The pyramidal neuron patchy connections build in depth to form connectional columns (about 250–300 µm diameter) that reach from the base of 4B to the top of layer 1 (Rockland and Lund, 1983). The relationship of these anatomical connectional columns to the ocular dominance ‘‘columns’’ (actually slabs) and orientation columns defined functionally in layers 4B-1 by Hubel and Wiesel (1972, 1977; anatomical ocular dominance columns are only seen in layer 4C) has recently been explored by optical imaging 116 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 6. Neuron with simple beaded axon arbor (axon to the left, dendritic field to the right); variety 2/3A:5, from area V1 in the floor of the calcarine fissure in a Golgi-rapid impregnation of an 8-week-old M. nemestrina. Scale bar 5 50 µm. (Malach et al., 1993; Yoshioka et al., 1996); there is no simple link between these superficial layer anatomical connections and functionally defined columns. All of the local circuit neurons we have described within layers 1–3A, as well as those in layers 3B-4B, have dendritic fields no wider than that of a single pyramidal neuron connectional column. Single pyramidal neuron dendritic fields and thalamic input ‘‘blobs’’ are also about 250 µm wide (Fig. 10B shows the size of the blob width, with the other neurons of Fig. 10 drawn to scale). Most of the interneurons described have an axon arbor that matches or is narrower than that of the dimensions of single connectional columns (i.e., exert their maximum effect within a column of cells with reasonably similar function). Only two varieties, the wide- and medium-arbor cells (varieties 2/3A.3 and 2/3A.6), have axon spreads that would enable them to influence laterally offset territory immediately surrounding but outside their local connectional patch, i.e., territory with functions unlike the region occupied by their dendritic field. Modulation beyond the patch diameter. Both of the neuron varieties with wider axon arbors (varieties 2/3A.2 and 2/3A.6) have no axon terminals within layer 1, suggesting that they are unlikely to terminate on pyramidal neuron apical dendrites; although their axons do not form pericellular terminal arrays, their general resemblance to basket neurons (whose axons are known to contact the proximal segments of pyramidal neuron dendrites and their somata; Kisvarday, 1992) suggests that they may have similar contact sites on pyramidal neurons of layers 2 and 3A. If the wide-arbor interneuron 2/3A:3 is a constant part of the superficial layer neuropil (despite our failure to impregnate it regularly in our Golgi preparations), it could represent a counterpart of the deeper wide-arbor basket neurons of layers 3B-4B (varieties 4B, 4A, and 3B:3 of Lund and Yoshioka, 1991). Our one example has an axon spread in layer 2/3A with significant amounts of terminals over an area of about 780 µm in diameter, i.e., much the same spread as the axons of deeper basket cells in layers 4B-3B. We proposed in a previous study (Lund et al., 1993) that these wide arbors might help reinforce the discontinuous pattern of connectivity of the pyramidal neurons in the superficial layers by inhibiting pyramidal neurons in a territory that surrounds any active point across the cortical sheet. It is, however, more likely that the single, wide-arbor neuron we have found in layer 2/3 represents an odd outlier in a system that predominantly occupies layers 4B-3B (Lund and Yoshioka, 1991). Much more LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX Fig. 7. A: Medium-arbor neuron, variety 2/3A:6. Note that axon and dendrites are drawn superimposed in their in vivo relationship. B: Axon of medium arbor neuron, variety 2/3A:6 (soma and dendritic 117 field not impregnated; open arrow indicates axon initial segment). Both A and B are from Golgi-rapid impregnations of the outer operculum of V1 in a 5-week-old M. nemestrina. Scale bar 5 50 µm. 118 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 8. Two double-bouquet neurons, variety 2/3A:7, from the outer operculum of V1 (A; axon to left, dendrites to right) and from V1 in the roof of the calcarine fissure (B; axon to right, dendrites to left). Both A and B are from 5-week-old M. nemestrina, Golgi-rapid impregnations. Scale bar 5 50 µm. regularly encountered are the medium-arbor cells with axon arbors widening as they rise from layer 3A to layer 2, with no entry into layer 1. The dimensions of their axon spread (500–700 µm) also could provide inhibition in a surrounding territory to the pyramidal connectional patch system (see diagram in Fig. 12, bottom). Although the wide-arbor axons include terminals in the home territory of the basket neuron dendritic field, we suggest that their inhibitory impact on pyramidal neurons is always balanced against excitatory feed-forward input from lower layers. If the basket neuron and local pyramids are receiving heavy direct excitation, then we suggest that the local basket neuron contacts have little effect; the inhibitory effect of the basket neuron is hypothesized to grow as the synapses lie farther and farther away from the center of the activated patch, where the stimulus and its resultant excitation becomes less and less effective. The balance between weight of excitation and inhibition is suggested to be the key element in the activity patterns of the superficial layers, and the shift toward net inhibition should lie at about 125 µm from the center of an activated patch. The question remains: Why, if these medium- and wide-arbor cells fulfill a similar function of surround inhibition, is there not a single form of neuron providing it? The answer may lie in the source of afferents to the medium- and wide-arbor neurons; if each is driven by the same sources of rising interlaminar afferents as the local pyramids, then the deep, wide-arbor layer 4B-3B neurons should receive input from afferents rising from layer 4C and, perhaps, from thalamic afferents to blobs; the more superficial medium-arbor cells should receive input from rising projections from layer 3B, 4A, and 4B pyramidal neurons. This stratification of afferents and wide- and medium-arbor cells should enable the interneurons to have the same temporal relationships to afferents at different depths as the local pyramids. It is known from work in cat (Kisvarday et al., 1993) that basket neurons with a morphology similar to that of the layer 3B-4B cells in monkey also contact each other. As one patch system becomes active, the wide- and medium-arbor cells within the active patches may not only inhibit pyramidal neuron activity in surround territory, they may also prevent inhibition of their own patch areas by inhibiting the surrounding neurons of like kind (see Fig. 12, bottom). There is a difference in anatomical detail of the axon arbors of the medium- and wide-arbor cells that may suggest a slight difference in target. The 2/3A.3 wide-arbor neuron variety, like the layer 4B-3B basket cells, has short, terminal-side branches that closely resemble similar processes on the basket neurons of cat. These processes have been shown by EM to terminate on pyramidal neuron proximal dendritic segments and somata (Kisvarday et al., 1993). The axons of medium-arbor cells have longer linear beaded processes that have a very similar trajectory to pyramidal neuron dendrites. It is possible, therefore, that the medium-arbor cell axons contact dendritic branches of LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX 119 Fig. 9. A,B: Two double-bouquet neurons, variety 2/3A:7. Both A and B are from the outer operculum of area V1; the axon for each appears to the right of the dendritic field; Golgi-rapid impregnations from 5-week-old M. nemestrina. Note the bipolar form of cell A and compare the axon morphology of these neurons with those shown in Figure 7; despite some differences in morphology, we were unable to make a clear distinction into bipolar and double-bouquet classes based on either axonal or dendritic morphology. Scale bar 5 50 µm. the superficial pyramids; the basal or proximal apical branch dendrites would be the most likely targets, because the medium-arbor cell axons do not enter layer 1. Local modulation across a patch width. Local circuit neuron candidates exist where single neurons could exert inhibition across a pyramidal neuron connectional column width: the layer 1 neuron (variety 1:1), the columnar neuron (variety 2/3A:2), the chandelier neuron (variety 2/3A:1), and the simple beaded axon cell (variety 2/3A:5). Each of these neurons has axon arbors with a lateral spread similar to that of a single connectional patch. Whereas the axons of individual columnar neurons extend their arbor vertically, individual layer 1, chandelier, and simple beaded axon cells restrict the vertical extent of their axons; the chandelier and columnar neurons have a continuous distribution in depth, with narrowly focused columnar dendritic fields and axon arbors that cross all boundaries in depth of layers 4B-2. Chandelier cells are also found in layers 4C-a, 5A, and 5B (Lund, 1987; Lund et al., 1988). Their axons are known to provide GABAergic synapses to pyramid initial axon segments (Somogyi, 1977). The origins of their excitatory and inhibitory inputs are unknown. Clearly, these cells could form a crucial gate to the pyramidal neuron (as well as some populations of spiny stellate neuron) outputs. Douglas and Martin (1990) have suggested on the basis of theoretical modeling that the chandelier neurons may work in synergy with basket neuron synapses on the somata of the same pyramidal cells; they suggest that the axon initial segment inhibition increases the threshold for action potential discharge, whereas the basket neuron synapses control the suprathreshold discharge. Nothing is known of the interrelations between the simple beaded cells and other cells of the 2/3A layer. The absence of their axon collaterals in layer 1 suggests that their target is not pyramidal neuron apical dendrites or the distal regions of dendrites of other local circuit neurons of the same depth, most of which intrude on layer 1; this leaves pyramidal neuron somata or other interneuron somata and proximal dendritic segments as possible targets. Their presence in layer 2/3A and their apparent 120 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Fig. 10. A: Diagram summarizing the varieties of local circuit neurons found in layers 1-2/3A of area V1 in our Golgi impregnations of infant and mature macaque monkeys. All varieties except 2/3A:3 occurred with regularity in our impregnations from a variety of ages. B: Diagram summarizing findings from our previous studies (Lund, 1987; Lund et al., 1988; Lund and Yoshioka, 1991). The diagram shows the pyramidal neuron (five cells to the left of the diagram) and local circuit neuron (seven numbered varieties to the right of the diagram) projections from deeper layers to layers 1-2/3A of area V1. At the upper left, the lateral dimensions of a single blob territory is drawn to scale with the lateral extent of axon and dendritic fields of the neurons diagrammed in A and B. absence in layer 3B may indicate a special role at this depth in the cortex. Martin et al. (1989) have recorded and filled intracellularly with horseradish peroxidase a layer 1 neuron in the cat visual cortex with morphology similar to our variety 1:1 neurons. Its axon contacts and dendritic inputs were examined by EM, and it was concluded that it both projected to and received input from pyramidal neurons (whose apical dendrites and axon processes make up a major part of the layer 1 neuropil). Functionally, the neuron was a simple, orientation-selective but nondirectional neuron. Because layer 1 has afferents from a number of extrinsic sources as well as lateral projections from pyramids in the superficial layers, these layer 1 local circuit neurons may share these inputs with the pyramidal neurons and govern the responsiveness of their apical LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX Fig. 11. Neuron of layer 4B, variety 4B:5 (for further illustration of this neuron type, see Lund and Yoshioka, 1991). This neuron makes a rising, axonal projection specifically to layers 1-2/3A. Golgi-rapid impregnation from the outer operculum of V1 of a 3-week-old M. nemestrina. Scale bar 5 50 µm. dendrites to these various afferents. The dimensions of the axons of the layer 1 cells suggest that this modulation is across a patch width. Local modulation within patch boundaries. It is known that orientation preference can change through 90° 121 Fig. 12. Top: Patchy connections of pyramidal neurons in the superficial layers of macaque V1 (pial surface to the top, base of layer 3 to the bottom). The scale of change in orientation preference across the cortex is shown along the top; the pyramidal neurons are drawn to scale with the change of orientation preference. Pyramids centered in patches marked A respond best to stimulus orientations near the vertical; those centered in patches marked B respond best to stimulus orientations near horizontal. The ‘‘stepping’’ connectivity can start at any point in the orientation sequences across the cortex, but interconnected patches center on regions of common orientation preference; the individual connectional patch size does not grow larger than about 250 µm in diameter. Bottom: Superficial layer medium- and widearbor basket neuron connections. The cortex is oriented as in the diagram above; we propose that entering excitatory afferents (arrows entering base of center column A) drive both pyramidal neurons and wide-arbor inhibitory basket cells in layer 3B; layer 3B pyramidal neurons send rising excitation to pyramidal neurons and mediumarbor basket neurons in layers 2/3A. The wide-arbor and mediumarbor basket neurons of center column A have axon arbors that reach into neighboring B columns of orientation preference, unlike those of column A. We propose that, in the B columns, the A basket neurons inhibit both pyramidal neurons and like-kind basket neurons. This architecture is presumed to be a continuum across the cortex, and it implies that only one set of patches can be fully active at any one time, because their activity will suppress surrounding, unlike territory. Electron microscopic (EM) studies of synaptology have demonstrated, in at least the cat V1, that wide-arbor basket neurons have terminals on both pyramidal neuron somata and proximal dendritic segments as well as on other basket neurons of the same type (Kisvarday et al., 1993); EM studies have also shown that the lateral connections of pyramidal neurons target mainly pyramidal neurons and, as well, interneuron surfaces (Rockland, 1985; McGuire et al., 1991). Other relationships hypothesized in this figure need to be confirmed. across a lateral distance equivalent to a connectional patch. The double-bouquet or bipolar cells (variety 2/3A:7) have vertical axon arbors that are narrower than the pyramidal neuron connectional column and, thus, can exert influence within this functional gradient; such cells have been frequently associated with the calcium-binding proteins calbindin and calretinin and may also contain CCK or tachykinin as well as GABA (DeFelipe et al., 1990; 122 J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU DeFelipe, 1993; Lund and Lewis, 1993; Condé et al., 1994; Yan et al., 1995). It is clear that these neurons may vary widely in the amount of these substances in the cell and its processes; the level can vary depending on species, on age, on cortical area or subcompartments within single areas, and on activity level of the cell (Hendry and Carder, 1993). There is reason to believe that there may be different types of these neurons with forms that are closer to bipolar morphology with different synaptic relationships (Peters, 1984a; Somogyi and Cowey, 1984); in monkey, however, the synapses of double-bouquet cells appear to have symmetric (type 2; generally associated with axons of GABAergic cells) morphology; they synapse about equally on spines (probably those of pyramidal neurons) and on dendritic shafts, which may belong to other local circuit neurons (DeFelipe et al., 1990). The narrow arbor of individual double-bouquet cell axons may enable a very fine scale of modulation across the cortical map, even if the neurons themselves are continuously distributed. Another candidate for intracolumnar modulation is the neurogliaform cell (variety 2.3A:4). We have previously found neurogliaform cells in layer 4B (variety 4B:4 of Lund and Yoshioka, 1991), and they have been described as a common component of the cerebral cortex (Fairén et al., 1984; Jones, 1984) in general. These cells should not be confused with small local circuit neurons of layer 4C, which we have called variety 4Cb:1 (Lund, 1987) and which we believe to be the same as the ‘‘clewed’’ cells of Valverde (1971). The neurogliaform cells have much finer axons than this layer 4C neuron variety, and their axons lack its large diameter dilations. There is, however, a resemblance between neurogliaform cells and the layer 1 neuron (variety 1:1), but, again, the layer 1 neuron has somewhat larger diameter boutons on its axon processes than the neurogliaform cells. Different sets of feed-forward, local circuit neuron axon projections accompany the specific relays of different populations of layer 4C spiny stellate neurons to layers 4B, 4A, and 3B (Lund, 1987; Lund and Yoshioka, 1991). Because layer 4B has pyramidal neuron projections to layer 3B and above, and layer 3B pyramids project to layers 1-2/3A, it might be expected that there would be feed-forward inhibitory relays accompanying these excitatory projections. Martin et al. (1989) observed GABAergic contacts on the soma and proximal dendrites of the cat layer 1 cell they studied. Inhibition of these layer 1 local circuit neurons, if it produces disinhibition of the pyramidal neuron apical dendritic arbors in layer 1, could be a means of bringing the layer 1 inputs into the sum of events affecting the firing of the parent pyramidal neurons; the source and drive for the inhibitory synapses on these layer 1 local circuit neurons is therefore of some importance. Figures 10B and 13A show the sources of inhibition we have been able to find that enter layer 1 and that might cause disinhibition of pyramids via inhibition of layer 1 GABAergic cells; local circuit neurons in layers 5A (variety 5A:4), 4B (variety 4B:5), perhaps 4A (variety 4A,3B:9), and double-bouquet cells of 2/3A (variety 2/3:5) are all candidates for such action. The interneurons with rising columnar axons (varieties 4B, 4A, 3B, 2/3A:2; see Fig. 11) form a continuum of axon projections and dendritic arbors from layer 4B upward to the base of layer 1 and these could provide one source of feed-forward inhibition to layers 2/3. The axons of the deeper columnar cells have sparser terminal arbors near the top of layer 3A, whereas more superficial columnar cells in 2/3A, have a robust axon at the same level, but Fig. 13. A: Diagram illustrating possible sources of rising ‘‘feedforward’’ columnar inhibition (black dots are numbered interneurons in layers 4A-3B, 4B, and 5A) directed at local circuit neurons in layers 1–3A (double bouquet, solid oval; chandelier, open squares; layer 1, solid dots, 1.1), which, themselves, provide direct inhibition to pyramidal neurons. The action of these rising, inhibitory connections is suggested to be disinhibitory, allowing the pyramids to respond to rising excitatory afferents (not shown) from the same deeper layers. The columnar neurons (double squares) are suggested to inhibit chandelier cells (see B). For further discussion, see text. B: Diagram of proposed intracolumnar, ‘feed-forward’ disinhibition of pyramidal neurons involving columnar and chandelier local circuit neurons. In this circuit, the columnar cells (double squares) are suggested to inhibit chandelier neurons (single squares), thereby disinhibiting the pyramidal neurons and allowing them to respond to incoming excitation. Note that the rising intracolumnar excitation provided by pyramids is suggested to contact both columnar cells and other pyramids. Laterally running pyramidal neuron collaterals of the patch system, on the other hand, are suggested to contact chandelier neurons and pyramids, not the columnar cells. We have proposed earlier (Lund et al., 1995) that, as the weight of this lateral input increases, it will have a net inhibitory effect (via the chandelier cells) on the pyramidal neurons. It should be noted that, in addition to spiny stellate neurons (open circles), local circuit neurons send projections out of divisions of 4C; these could inhibit the chandelier neurons controlling the layer 4B and 3B pyramids (Lund, 1987), i.e., disinhibiting the pyramidal neurons. For further discussion, see text. C: Model for directional surround suppression of nondirectional layer 2-3 neuron responses. Although neurons of layers 2-3 are not direction selective in the macaque monkey, surround stimulation of the same orientation preference and direction of motion as a center stimulus suppresses responses to the center stimulus (Allman et al., 1990); the opposite direction of motion in center and surround preserves the response to the center stimulus. In layer 4B, however, cells can have true direction selectivity to center stimulus motion; here again, surround stimulation matched in direction and orientation is suppressive. In this model, layer 4B pyramids in the cortex activated by the surround send laterally traveling excitation to those center pyramidal and chandelier neurons (open squares) of similar direction and orientation specificities. When the weight of the surround input rises sufficiently, the chandelier neuron inhibition begins to suppress 4B local pyramidal neurons. The 4B pyramids provide excitatory drive to 4B columnar neurons (double squares), whose rising axons inhibit superficial layer chandelier neurons. If the 4B pyramids are prevented from firing by matching center and surround stimulation, then activation of the local columnar cells is prevented; in the absence of inhibition, the layer 2-3 chandelier cells remain active and suppressive as the surround input from layer 2-3 pyramids reaches the layer 2-3 center cells. On the other hand, if the layer 4B pyramids are activated by a center stimulus of one direction of motion and the surround input is of the opposite direction, then the surround input is ineffective in silencing these center-driven, directionally specific 4B pyramids; their activity drives columnar cells that inhibit chandelier cells in layers 2-3, allowing the nondirectional layer 2-3 pyramids to continue to respond to lateral input from the surround of the same orientation but of opposite direction. Although there is EM evidence for the contacts between the chandelier neuron axon and pyramidal neuron axon initial segments (Somogyi et al., 1982), for layer 1 neuron to pyramidal neuron contacts, at least in the cat (Martin et al., 1989), and for lateral pyramidal neuron connections to both pyramids and interneurons (Rockland, 1985: McGuire et al., 1991), other synaptic relationships hypothesized in the diagrams of this figure need to be confirmed. Contributors to feed-forward inhibition of layers 1–3A LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX Figure 13 123 124 sparse arbors in layers 3B and 4B. Given their similarity of form at all depths, these neurons probably have a common function throughout the depth of the 4B-to-base-1 column. The diameter of their columnar axon fields (250–350 µm) is close to that of the patches of lattice-like pyramidal neuron axon terminals, so they should provide inhibition to elements within columns of common function. Because the columnar cell axons do not intrude into layer 1, their target is unlikely to be pyramidal neuron apical dendrites. Below, we suggest that a possible target may be the somata and proximal dendritic segments of chandelier neurons. It is clear that there are neurons of columnar class with dendrites restricted to layer 4B (variety 4B:2; see Fig. 10B), whereas, more superficially, there are columnar cells whose dendrites cross between layers 3B and 2/3A. The layer 4B columnar cells with dendrites restricted to that layer are likely to be driven by activity restricted to layer 4B (because more superficial pyramids do not send terminal arbors down into layer 4B). The more superficial columnar cells can receive input from axons terminating through the depth of the superficial layers, i.e., their input is not restricted to axons stratified to any particular depth (e.g., to axon projections out of layer 4C that target layer 3B). We suggest, therefore, that the columnar interneurons could be one target of local pyramidal neuron axon arbors at each level in the column, acting as a ‘‘within column’’ inhibitory modulator but emphasizing upward projection with the bulk of their axon, i.e., feed-forward function, within the column width. In layer 3B-2, the narrow focus of their dendritic fields (approximately 80 µm diameter field in layer 2/3, much narrower than the local pyramidal neuron dendritic arbors of approximately 250 µm diameter) suggests that the cells may have very specifically tuned response properties, because they occupy such a narrow territory within the laterally changing gradients of orientation specificity, as well as other properties that are characteristic of these superficial layers. This tight tuning should be reflected in the inhibition that they exert. Apart from the columnar interneurons, one other candidate exists for feed-forward inhibition to layers 1-2/3A: This is variety 4B:5 (see Fig. 10B), which we have already discussed in relation to contacts on layer 1 cells, and it also provides terminals to layers 2/3A. It was described previously by Lund and Yoshioka (1991), and an example is illustrated in Figure 11 from a better impregnated neuron. The narrow columnar axon arbor rising from layer 4B clearly favors layers 1 and 2/3A as a terminal zone. Another feed-forward interneuron projection comes from a cell located in layer 4A/base of layer 3B, variety 4A-3B:9 (Lund and Yoshioka, 1991; see Fig. 10B), which terminates in at least layers 2/3A and 3B. The narrow focus of the axon arbors of these two varieties of neurons within layers 2-3 suggests that they exert an effect on a scale less than the connectional patch width. They could both synapse upon the vertically oriented dendritic arbors of other interneurons in layers 1–3A and have a disinhibitory role. In the case of the layer 4B neuron, the clear focus of the axon terminals to within layers 1–3A suggests postsynaptic elements that are restricted to this territory. Figure 13A suggests possible targets for these feed-forward GABAergic projections (layer 1 neurons, double-bouquet cells, and chandelier cells), all of which would lessen inhibition on the pyramidal neurons in the same column. J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Functional considerations and hypothetical circuits Although it has been clear from the earliest EM studies of cerebral cortex that interneurons are contacted by type 2 GABAergic synapses, the specific cell types linked by these connections have only been identified in very few instances (e.g., basket-to-basket neuron contacts; Kisvarday et al., 1993). The difficulty lies in labeling and identifying both pre- and postsynaptic cells prior to examination by EM. It will therefore take considerable effort to determine whether our suggested circuitry is anatomically correct. The answers may come from physiological identification and cell-filling experiments rather than from purely anatomical investigations, especially if clear hypotheses are built up concerning what physiological properties the interneurons are expected to display. We feel, therefore, that it is useful to make some suggestions for circuitry involving the interneurons we have described. Although these circuits may not be accurate, they involve principles of organization that must be considered in cortical function. By suggesting specific circuitry, we may prompt a hunt to either confirm or refute these particular circuits on the basis of predicted physiological properties, and this should advance our understanding of cortical function. Because this study suggests that feed-forward inhibition to layers 1 and 2/3A is a possible function of several neuron varieties, and feed-forward inhibition has been seen in our earlier studies of layer 4C interneurons, the question might be asked what function such inhibition might have. One function could be to release tonic inhibition on the targets of the interlaminar, rising relays of feed-forward excitation, allowing the postsynaptic targets to respond to the feed-forward excitation; tonic inhibition on pyramidal neurons could be exerted by either basket or chandelier neurons. It is known that basket neurons contact and presumably disinhibit one another in the cat (Kisvarday et al., 1993), but nothing is known of the origins of inhibition on chandelier neurons; inhibition of the chandelier neurons is unlikely to come from other chandelier neurons, given the cartridge-like form of their axon terminal arbors. One possible function for the columnar cells, therefore, might be to act as inhibitory controllers of the chandelier neurons, ensuring that local pyramidal neurons are released from their inhibition under appropriate conditions, e.g., when the column is active. The excitatory drive for the chandelier neurons could be from laterally spreading, pyramidal neuron axon collaterals. In a previous study, we suggested a simple model by which surround stimulation may change the firing properties of cortical pyramids to stimuli presented in their classical receptive field (Lund et al., 1995); the model postulated the existence of a local circuit neuron that acts as a ‘‘symbiotic’’ inhibitory companion to pyramidal cells and that suppresses the pyramidal neuron output when driven by high levels of excitatory input from laterally offset patches of pyramidal neurons; here, we suggest that this symbiotic companion is the chandelier neuron and that its activity is also governed by feed-forward intracolumnar cell inhibition. A diagram of this suggested relationship between columnar neuron, chandelier neuron, and pyramids, together with their afferents, is shown in Figure 13B. The columnar neurons in layer 4B that project to the superficial layers could be one route by which the property of directionally specific surround modulation (Allman et al., 1990) could be brought LOCAL CIRCUIT NEURONS OF MONKEY VISUAL CORTEX to bear on the nondirectional neurons of layers 2-3. A model circuit diagram is shown in Figure 13C, where surround motion contradirectional to center-field stimulus motion is permissive to superficial layer pyramid firing, whereas the same direction of motion in center and surround would veto superficial layer pyramid activity. Columnar neurons as well as chandelier neurons are found in superficial layers of other regions of the cortex, e.g., prefrontal cortex (Lund and Lewis, 1993), suggesting that the function of both cells may be universally applicable. Their relays also pass to layer 5B below, perhaps representing another step in the feed-forward disinhibitory path. In layer 5, interneurons (variety 5B:2; see Lund et al., 1988; see also Fig. 11B) are found that may be the counterparts of these superficial cells; they have both a local arbor in layer 5B and a robust arbor in layer 2/3A, and these, again, may be the local controllers of chandelier neurons in layer 5 as well in the superficial layers. The question of what is feed-forward or feedback clearly becomes a problem to interpret in the case of layer 5; thus, for the moment, it may be better to think of the layer 5 interactions with the superficial layers as parts of a single process (because their pyramids provide reciprocal connections) with lateral connectivity in the superficial layers and, to a more limited extent, also playing an important role within layer 5B. We have not observed chandelier neurons in layer 6 of V1 (Lund et al., 1988), but this may have been because of the vagaries of our Golgi impregnations; however, the variety 5B:2 cells, which have been suggested to control the chandelier neurons, also do not contribute more than the weakest axon collaterals to layer 6. Comparative aspects It interesting to note that all of the interneurons described in this study are found in superficial layers of other, nonvisual regions of the macaque monkey (e.g., prefrontal cortex; Lund and Lewis, 1993) as well as occurring in many other mammals, including a variety of marsupials (Lund et al., 1994; Tyler et al., 1996). We conclude from this commonality of occurrence that the mammalian cortex, in at least its superficial layers, has a universal, basic set of components that can serve as a substrate either for processing the very first stages of sensory input or for functions that are much closer to the cognitive aspects of behavior ascribed to specific regions, such as those of the frontal pole. It was also found that, in all areas of the cortex examined in the primate (Levitt et al., 1993; Lund et al., 1993) and in the visual cortex in many different mammals (including monotremes and marsupials; Dann and Buhl, 1995; Tyler et al., 1996), the superficial layer pyramidal neurons exhibit a discontinuous pattern of lateral connectivity. The interneurons, therefore, form an integral part of this universal cortical pattern of organization that apparently has remained little changed over its evolutionary history. CONCLUSIONS This study brings to an end our detailed examination of the interneurons of the macaque primary visual cortex as seen in Golgi preparations. Despite their apparent complexity in terms of the variety of different forms in each lamina and their intricate patterns of interlaminar projections, there is an impression of economy of organization, in that each form appears to have a particular role, which is 125 defined by its axon morphology, lateral spread, and specific patterns of intra- and interlaminar projections. It is interesting to consider that, because the GABAergic neurons in the cortex are only about one-quarter or less of the total neuron population (Fitzpatrick et al., 1987), the frequency of any one variety will be quite low; a sparse representation may allow patterns of activity within any one population that can sculpt activity across the much larger population of pyramidal neurons. The scale of patterned connections across the cortex, therefore, may be constrained more by the activity of the interneuron populations than by innate specificity of connections between particular pyramidal neurons. In particular, the interplay of wide-arbor axon interneurons’ veto and disinhibition by interneurons with narrower diameter axons could help to create the connectional column system prenatally (Yoshioka, 1994). The excitatory interconnections would then tend to foster commonality of response patterns in interconnected columns. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Stephen Griffiths and Kesi Sainsbury are thanked for their expert technical assistance. Christopher Tyler’s critical appraisal of the paper was much appreciated. LITERATURE CITED Allman, J.M., F. Miezen, and E. McGuiness (1990) Effects of background motion on the responses of neurons in the first and second cortical visual areas. In G.M. Edelman, W.E. Gall, and M.W. Cowan (eds): Signal and Sense: Local and Global Order in Perceptual Maps. New York: Wiley-Liss, Inc., pp. 131–142. Blasdel, G.G., J.S. Lund, and D. Fitzpatrick (1985) Intrinsic connections of macaque striate cortex: Axonal projections of cells outside lamina 4C. J. Neurosci. 5:3350–3369. Condé, F., J.S. Lund, D.M. Jacobowitz, K.G. Baimbridge, and D.A. Lewis (1994) Local circuit neurons immunoreactive for calretinin, calbindin D-28k or parvalbumin in monkey prefrontal cortex: Distribution and morphology. J. Comp. Neurol. 341:95–116. Dann, J.F., and E.H. Buhl (1995) Patterns of connectivity in the neocortex of the Echidna (Tachyglossus aculeatus). Cerebral Cortex 5:363–373. DeFelipe, J. (1993) Neocortical neuronal diversity: Chemical heterogeneity revealed by colocalisation studies of classic neurotransmitters, neuropeptides, calcium binding proteins and cell surface molecules. Cerebral Cortex 3:273–289. DeFelipe, J., S.H.C. Hendry, T. Hashikawa, M. Molinari, and E.G. Jones (1990) A microcolumnar structure of monkey cerebral cortex revealed by immunocytochemical studies of double bouquet cells. Neuroscience 37:655–673. Douglas, R.J., and K.A.C. Martin (1990) Control of neuronal output by inhibition at the axon initial segment. Neural Comp. 2:283–292. Fairén, A., J. DeFelipe, and J. Regidor (1984) Nonpyramidal neurons. In E.G. Jones and A. Peters (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 5. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex: General Account. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 201–254. Fitzpatrick, D., K. Itoh, and I.T. Diamond (1983) The laminar organisation of the lateral geniculate nucleus and the striate cortex in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Neuroscience 3:673–702. Fitzpatrick, D., J.S. Lund, and G.G. Blasdel (1985) Intrinsic connections of macaque striate cortex: Afferent and efferent connections of lamina 4C. J. Neurosci. 5:3329–3349. Fitzpatrick, D., J.S. Lund, D.E. Schmechel, and A.C. Towles (1987) Distribution of GABA-ergic neurons and axon terminals in macaque striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 264:73–91. Hendry, H.C., and R.K. Carder (1993) Neurochemical compartmentation of monkey and human visual cortex: Similarities and variations in calbindin immunoreactivity across species. Vis. Neurosci. 10:1109– 1120. Herkenham, M. (1986) New perspectives on the organization and the evolution of nonspecific thalamocortical projections. In E.G. Jones and 126 A. Peters (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 5. Sensory-Motor Areas and Aspects of Cortical Connectivity. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 403–446. Houser, C.R., J.E. Vaugn, S.H.C. Hendry, E.G. Jones, and A. Peters (1984) GABA neurons in the cerebral cortex. In E.G. Jones and A. Peters (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 2. Functional Properties of Cortical Cells. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 63–90. Hubel, D.H., and T.N. Wiesel (1972) Laminar and columnar distribution of geniculo-cortical fibers in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 146:421–450. Hubel, D.H., and T.N. Wiesel (1977) Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex. Proc. R. Soc. London [Biol.] 198:1–59. Jones, E.G. (1984) Neurogliaform or spiderweb cells. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 409–418. Jones, E.G., and S.H.C. Hendry (1984) Basket cells. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 309–336. Kisvarday, Z.F. (1992) GABAergic networks of basket cells in the visual cortex. In R.R. Mize, R. Marc, and A.M. Sillito (eds): Progress in Brain Research. Mechanisms of GABA in the Visual System. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 385–405. Kisvarday, Z.F., C. Beaulieu, and U.T. Eysel (1993) Network of GABAergic large basket neurons in cat visual cortex (area 18): Implication for lateral disinhibition. J. Comp. Neurol. 327:398–415. Lachica, E.A., and V.A. Casagrande (1992) Direct W-like geniculate projections to the cytochrome-oxidase (CO) blobs in primate visual cortex: Axon morphology. J. Comp. Neurol. 319:141–158. Levitt, J.B., D.A. Lewis, T. Yoshioka, and J.S. Lund (1993) Topography of the Pyramidal neuron intrinsic connections in macaque monkey prefrontal cortex (areas 9 and 46). J. Comp. Neurol. 338:360–376. Livingstone, M.S., and D.H. Hubel (1984) Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 4:309–356. Lund, J.S. (1973) Organisation of neurons in the visual cortex, area 17, of the (Macaca mulatta) macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 147:455–496. Lund, J.S. (1987) Local circuit neurons of macaque monkey striate cortex: I. Neurons of laminae 4C and 5A. J. Comp. Neurol. 257:60–92. Lund, J.S., and D.A. Lewis (1993) Local circuit neurons of developing and mature macaque prefrontal cortex: Golgi and immunocytochemical characteristics. J. Comp. Neurol. 328:282–312. Lund, J.S., and Q. Wu (1993) Local circuit neurons in the superficial layers of macaque monkey striate cortex. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 19:1445. Lund, J.S., and T. Yoshioka (1991) Local circuit neurons of macaque monkey striate cortex: III. Neurons of laminae 4B, 4A, and 3B. J. Comp. Neurol. 331::234–258. Lund, J.S., M.J. Hawken, and A.J. Parker (1988) Local circuit neurons of macaque monkey striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 276:1–29. Lund, J.S., T. Yoshioka, and J.B. Levitt (1993) Comparison of intrinsic connectivity in different areas of macaque monkey cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 3:148–162. Lund, J.S., A. Harman, and L. Beazley (1994) Common cell types and patterns of laminar distribution in marsupial and eutherian visual cortex identify components essential for neocortical function. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 20:311. Lund, J.S., Q. Wu, P.T. Hadingham, and J.B. Levitt (1995) Cells and circuits contributing to functional properties in area V1 of macaque monkey cerebral cortex: Bases for neuroanatomically realistic models. J. Anat. (London) 187:563–581. Malach, R., Y. Amir, M. Harel, and A. Grinvald (1993) Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vitro targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 90:10469–10473. Marin-Padilla, M. (1984) Neurons of layer I: A developmental analysis. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex. Vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 447–478. J.S. LUND AND C.Q. WU Martin, K.A.C., M.J. Friedlander, and V. Alones (1989) Physiological, morphological, and cytochemical characteristics of a layer 1 neuron in cat striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 282:404–414. McGuire, B.A., C.D. Gilbert, P.K. Rivlin, and T.N. Wiesel (1991) Targets of horizontal connections in macaque primary visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 305:370–392. Peters, A. (1984a) Bipolar cells. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. I. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 381–408. Peters, A. (1984b) Chandelier cells. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 361–380. Rezak, M., and L.A. Benevento (1979) A comparison of the organisation of the projections of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, the inferior pulvinar and adjacent lateral pulvinar to primary visual cortex (area 17) in the macaque monkey. Brain Res. 167:19–40. Ribak, C.E. (1978) Aspinous and sparsely-spinous stellate neurons in the visual cortex of rats contain glutamic acid decarboxylase. J. Neurocytol. 7:461–478. Rockland, K.S. (1985) Intrinsically projecting pyramidal neurons of monkey striate cortex: An EM-HRP study. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 11:17. Rockland, K.S., and J.S. Lund (1983) Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 216:303–318. Rockland, K.S., and D.N. Pandya (1979) Laminar origins and terminations of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 179:3–20. Rockland, K.S., and G.W. Van Hoesen (1994) Direct temporal-occipital feedback connections to striate cortex (V1) in the macaque monkey. Cerebral Cortex 4:300–313. Rockland, K.S., and A. Virga (1989) Terminal arbors of individual ‘‘feedback’’ axons projecting from V2 to V1 in the macaque monkey: A study using immunohistochemistry of anterogradely transported Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J. Comp. Neurol. 285:54–72. Rockland, K.S., K.S. Saleem, and K. Tanaka (1994) Divergent feedback connections from areas V4 and TEO. Vis. Neurosci. 11:579–600. Somogyi, P. (1977) A specific axo-axonal interneuron in the visual cortex of the rat. Brain Res. 136:345–350. Somogyi, P., and A. Cowey (1984) Double bouquet cells. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex. I. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 337–360. Somogyi, P., T.F. Freund, and A. Cowey (1982) The axo-axonic interneuron in the cerebral cortex of the rat, cat and monkey. Neuroscience 7:2577–2607. Tigges, J., and M. Tigges (185) Subcortical sources of direct projections to visual cortex. In A. Peters and E.G. Jones (eds): Cerebral Cortex: Visual Cortex, Vol. 3. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 351–378. Tyler, C., J. Lund, S. Dunlop, A. Harman, J. Dann, R. Lund, and L. Beazley (1996) Essential and archaic anatomical features of mammalian visual cortex. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 22:1611. Valverde, F. (1971) Short axon neural subsystems in the visual cortex of the monkey. Int. J. Neurosci. 1:181–197. Yan, Y.-H., J.F.M. Van Brederode, and A.E. Hendrickson (1995) Transient colocalisation of calretinin, parvalbumin and calbindin-D28K in developing visual cortex of monkey. J. Neurocytol. 24:825–837. Yoshioka, T. (1994) Development of clustered lateral connectivity in macaque visual cortex. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 20:1109. Yoshioka, T., J.B. Levitt, and J.S. Lund (1994) Independence and merger of thalamocortical channels within macaque monkey primary visual cortex: Anatomy of interlaminar connections. Vis. Neurosci. 11:467– 489. Yoshioka, T., G.G. Blasdel, J.B. Levitt, and J.S. Lund (1996) Relation between patterns of intrinsic lateral connectivity, ocular dominance and cytochrome oxidase-reactive regions in macaque monkey striate cortex. Cerebral Cortex 6:297–310.