Perceptual Distortion and Stress Instructor: Anita McCrary Perceptual Distortion and Stress COURSE DESCRIPTION: Course prerequisites include: Situational Awareness: A Primer for SUDs Counselors; Sleep, Memory, and Recovery; and Breathing and Anxiety: Nature’s Chill Pill. The course describes the neurological pathways underlying language acquisition theory and how the pathways assist in creating perceptual distortion. The course uses Roger Brown’s grammatical morphemes and B.F. Skinner’s behavioral autoclitics to explicate the psychosomatic morphology of situational and environmental triggers or cues. COURSE GOAL: Upon completion of the course, a clinician will understand the neurological mechanics of trigger/cue acquisition. The course builds on previous courses discussing situational awareness, sleep and respiration, affirming their reciprocal effect on idiosyncratic perception and behavior. Completion of the course will also introduce brief therapy enhancements for depression: SUDs, GAD, and PTSD. Upon receiving licensure, a clinician acquires hundreds of new words and terminology specific to the therapeutic environment. Clinicians spend hundreds of hours in internships learning how to convey empathetic intent through words and mannerisms and how behavioral paraphrasing affects a client’s behavior. How a clinician paraphrases these words (Segal, 1975) convey a word’s intent, consciously or unconsciously. Just as students of the mind must rehearse new words within proper context to solidify meaning, stress inoculation demands rehearsal of semeiotic sign recognition to control cortisol release. Over thirty years ago, E.F. Segal (1975) characterized his behavioral paraphrasing as semeiotic signs (vocalization + mannerisms) initiating an idiosyncratic interpretation. Segal’s semeiotic approach (Fisch, 1986 and Pierce, 1991) combines Roger Brown’s morphemes and B.F. Skinner’s autoclitics to explain the proverb ‘beliefs become thoughts; thoughts become words; words become behavior” (Gold, 2004). Segal further suggests perception of any stimuli depends on the interpretation of the recipient. Studies suggest verbal behavior shapes the nonverbal performance (behavior) of the individual and the performance of others through the use of reinforcement contingencies. A person uses reinforcement contingencies to evaluate, or boot strap, the intrinsic value of another’s behavioral paraphrasing. This syntactic force processes verbal syntax (morpheme combining; person, place, thing, or action), semantics (prosodic features; i.e. pitch, stress, etc.), and context (specific situation or circumstance) to delineate the intent of verbal behavior. Environmental and situational cues bootstrap meaning to verbal behavior the same way an infant uses syntax and vocal inflection to bootstrap the meaning of a word. Additionally, attaching emotional value to verbal behavior will prioritize the bootstrap and create robust cognitive-emotional reasoning. Experience and reason are said to be the two ultimate sources of intrinsic beliefs (Chisholm, 1957 & 1966). Mirror neurons quickly analyze complex stimuli and bootstrap the stimuli into priority for attentional tracking (de Wit, 2011). Grossberg’s Spectral Timing Model (1989) describes a conditional reinforcement learning theory (Adaptive Resonance Theory, or ART) to control trigger mirror neurons within the neural tissue surrounding the superior temporal sulcus (STS)1 . He proposes primitive motor reactions calibrate and recalibrate the bootstrap within the neural network2 and orchestrates adaptive sensory-motor control. Simply stated, ART supplies control of distractor cues by replacing them with more valuable bootstraps. Efference tells muscles what to do, while reafference bridges “low level nerve physiology and the high level science of behavior” (Von Holst, 1971, p. 68). Originating from movement across the retina and receptor impulses in the eye muscles, efference and reafference affirm the Yiddish proverb, ‘the eyes are the mirror to the soul.’ Mirror neurons enlist motor function to do the causal work of the central nervous system (Herley, 2001). Von Holst characterizes reafference as a bilateral division between the lower centers (primal needs) and higher centers (cognitive interpretation) of the statoreceptor nerve endings within the STS. Recent research (Iacoboni, 2001; Hein, 2008) indicates Holst’s bilateral division of the STS, not the nerve endings, is more likely the case. Iacoboni’s study shows high correlation with Von Holst’s and Vincent’s (2008) models of the frontoparietal control system (FPC) in attention3. To characterize Iacoboni’s analytical description of reafference: the eye sees IT, See The Need for Sleep, McCrary, 2013; referencing frontoparietal control system (Vincent, 2008) Ibid., learning combination lock (Wilson, 2003) 3 See Anxiety and Breathing: Nature’s Chill Pill (McCrary, 2013; Von Holst, 1971; & Vincent, 2008). 1 2 Perceptual Distortion and Stress the temporal lobe matches IT to default semeiotics (words, phrases, sentences, and signs), the STS adds somatosensory (including peripheral) information to IT, sends IT to the inferior frontal cortex where a copy of IT is sent back to the STS for storage (reafference), while the original IT travels to the CNS to force behavior. Hein asserts this brain region, “can support different cognitive operations depending on the task-dependent network connections” (Abstract, 2008). In other words, a person’s cognitive, physiological, and physical reaction to stimuli depends on the person’s pre-rehearsed bootstrapping. Stress Inoculation Optimal [threat coping] entails a number of factors, including accurate recognition of risks, effective management of emotions and problem solving, a focus on prosocial outcomes, and engagement in actions that have a reasonable chance of mitigation and adaptation (Daughtry, 2011, p. 272). A person’s intent may be wholesome in nature; however, any behavioral cue can hijack intent and subsequently, reaction. Given certain threatening circumstances, a person will instinctually retreat psychologically and physically because humans have an abhorrence to inflicting harm on another person, even on the battlefield. Hein’s research, however, hints at a way to control this evolutionary instinct, whereas brain plasticity enables a person to recalibrate default mindsets in the brain. To simplify further, consider the analogy of a home theatre system; the more wires connecting the different speakers, the better quality and robust the sound. A person can manipulate the syntactic and semantic default sound waves through the receiver (i.e. more bass, less treble, more rear speaker, less side speaker, etc.). The frontal cortex is the receiver in the prefrontal regions of the brain. Traveling over 300 days a year, Lieutenant Colonel Dave Grossman (Ret.) teaches the psychology of stress inoculation, situational awareness, and fear. He trains first responders, educators, civilian law enforcement and military personnel on establishing the default mindset of a paladin. Most famous for his metaphor, ‘civilians are sheep; criminals are wolves; the police, firefighter, soldier, EMT - he or she is the sheepdog’, Grossman uses animal myths corresponding to Cooper’s Color Code (1989) to characterize the perceptual mindsets of humanity. The Cooper Color Code represents perceptual awareness through sympathetic heart rate (bpm) zones. It also characterizes a person’s willingness to act (Grossman’s animal myths). Zone white (60-80 bpm) represents preoccupation, sleep, and sheep – oblivious or minimum acknowledgement of a possible threat. Yellow zone (80115 bpm) represents coordinating behavior to activity, driving, and migrating birds – navigating around a possible threat. As heart rate excelerates to the orange zone (100-120 bmp) the adrenal glands release cortisol and perceptual distortion begins; body extremities become cold and actions mimick a rodent’s – cognitively ready to escape impending threat. The red zone (115-145 bpm) represents high level performance, athletic endurance, and the raptor – mental and physical acknowledgment of an obvious threat. The optimal red zone, however, begins to diminish after thirty seconds (when cortisol floods the bloodstream), creates sleep deprivation, and requires three minutes to recharge (parasympathetic recovery). In zone black (145-175 bpm), attention and performance diminish significantly and represents a total domination of biological or psychological survival, irrational behavior, and the terrified puppy – scrambling and urinating because of the threat. Robert R. Whitesell’s pre-ASPCA experiment on stress duration and resiliency suggests repetitive cycling through yellow-to-red anxiety levels causes enervation exhaustion (and death to the rodents); however, innoculating a group to intermittent stressor increases the duration of stress tolerance exponentially (Whitesell, 1977; Burchfield, 1979). However, the Pike, et. al. (1997) study suggests prolonging chronic stress weakens a person’s ability to cope with acute psychological stressors. Note the differences in duration of stressors; chronic stress weakens sympathetic response, whereas intermittent stress allows for parasympathetic ascension and a return to homeostatis. Research also suggests a person’s dominant behavior traits (Anderson, 2003; Craig, 2007) may increase or decrease cortisol sensitivity and gender (Taylor, 2000) normally (but not always) dictates male testosterone fight-or-flight or female oxytocin tend-and-befriend. Perceptual Distortion and Stress Repetitive release of cortisol can slide a person into a profound state of physical and emotional exaustion. Ascendance of the parasympathetic system immediately following red zone enervation causes “incredibly powerful weariness and sleepiness” (Grossman, 1999, para. 4) and bolsters PTSD bootstrapping (Cohen, 2012). For vunerable populations (children, neurological and psychiatric disorders), the experience of high level symapthetic anxiety for thirty seconds can be an excruciatingly long and potentially detrimental length of time. Repeat cycles of sympathetic anxiety without significant recooperation (homeostasis) and prolonged exposure to cortisol can cause psychiatric atrophy, inhibiting synaptic plasticity in hippocampal dendrites (Sapolsky, 2000). This atrophy reveals itself in behavior through explicit memory loss (what day of the week/month is it?) and an increase in mild stress sensitivity (a ‘touchy’ emotional demeanor). Research suggests pathological hypersecretion (too much) and hyposecretion (too little) can damage hippocampus dentate neurogenesis. There are also implications of hippocampal dendrite atrophy Perceptual Distortion and Stress found in Cushing Syndrome, depression, schizophrenia, and Alzheimer disease (Henneman, 2009), and PTSD (2000). Atrophy can be reversed or minimized through innoculation rehearsal. Grossman borrows Bruce Siddle’s definition of combat stress, although here we characterize it as traumatic stress: “the perception of an imminent threat of serious [body or psychological] injury or death, or the stress of responsibility to protect another party from imminent serious injury or death, under conditions where response time is minimal” (1999, para. 17). He and Sally Dickerson report the most impactful stressors initiating cortisol release are: 1) the perception of a lack of control and 2) the social evaluative of an act (1999; Dickerson, 2004). This helps to explain the physiological response seen in phobias, stage fright, and interpersonal conflict. Although the typical perspective of the word ‘bootstrap’ may elicit negative perceptions of physical admonishment (‘I am going to take a bootstrap to your backside…’) and security (‘Fasten your bootstrap and hold on…’), both operationalize the cognitive effort involved in learning and mindset behavior. Grossman, a retired U.S. Army Ranger, describes the conditioning effort used in paramilitary training in Practicing to be Miserable (Grossman, 2006). The paramilitary model for mental and physical preparation for combat is apparent in many civilian treatments for anxiety and in critical incident response protocols. Most experts agree that simulation practice is the most effective way to bootstrap control of the sympathetic nervous system. CASE STUDY: Previous Trauma Exposure and PTSD Symptoms as Predictors of Subjective and Biological Response to Stress, Regehr, C., LeBlanc, V., Jelley, R.B., Barath, I., & Daciuk, J. 2007. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 52:10 Heart-rate Chart Perceptual Distortion and Stress Exam 1. Reinforcement contingencies assist a person to: a. delineate the intent of another person’s verbal behavior b. attach emotional value to verbal behavior c. orchestrate adaptive sensory-motor control d. all of the above 2. Efference and reafference begins: a. within the STS b. in the hippocampus c. in the eyes d. after a cue or trigger is perceived 3. Repetitive cycling through yellow-to-red anxiety levels cause: a. physical and mental exhaustion b. mirroring c. bootstrapping d. phobias, stage fright, and interpersonal conflict 4. One of the most impactful stressors initiating cortisol release is: a. training b. reafference c. an increase in heart rate d. Lack of control 5. According to research, hippocampal dendrite atrophy is caused by: a. testosterone b. cortisol 6. The Cooper Color Code represents perceptual awareness through: a. Cortisol levels b. mental and physical preparation a. heart rate b. chronic stress 7. Perceptual distortion occurs through: a. interpretation b. the hippocampus c. parasympathetic ascension d. evaluation 8. The paramilitary model for mental and physical preparation defines: a. mindset b. cognitive effort c. symapthetic anxiety d. athletic endurance 9. Stress inoculation… a. only works in military training b. filters memory loss c. causes psychiatric atrophy d. controls the release of cortisol 10. Cognitive retention of environmental and situational cues, verbal syntax and semantics describe: a. perception b. inoculation c. bootstrap d. parasympathetic ascension c. heart rate in the red zone d. trauma Perceptual Distortion and Stress References: Anderson, C. &. (2003). Human Aggression: A SocialCognitive View. In M. &. Hogg, the Sage Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 296-323). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Ayllon, T. &. (1964). Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 327-331. Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Burchfield, S. (1979). the Stress Response: A New Perspective. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41 (8), 661-672. Chisholm, R. (1957). Perceiving. Ithaca, NY: Cornell UP. Chisholm, R. (1966). Theory of Knoweldge. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Cohen, S. K. (2012). Post-Exposure Sleep Deprivation Facilitates Correctly Timed Interactions Between Glucocorticoid and Adrenergic Systems, which Attenuate Traumatic Stress Responses. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37, 2388-2404. Cooper, J. (1989). Principlesof Personal Defense. Boulder, CO: Paladin Press. Craig, I. (2007). The importance of stress and genetic variation in human aggression. Bioessays, 29, 227-236. Daughtry, T. &. (2011). The Psychological Impacts of Global Cliamte Change. American Psychologist, 66 (4), 265–276. de Wit, L. L. (2011). Investigating the Status of Biological Stimuli as Objects of Attention in Multiple Object Tracking. PLOS ONE, 6 (3). Dickerson, S. &. (2004). Acute Stressors and Cortisol Responses: A Theoretical Integration and Synthesis of Laboratory Research. Psychological Bulletin, 130 (3), 355391. Fisch, M. (1986). Peirce’s General Theory of Signs. In K. &. Ketner, Pierce, Semeiotic, and Pragmatism (pp. 321355). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Grossberg, &. S. (1989). Neural dynamics of adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during associative learning. Neural Networks, 2 (2), 79-102. Grossman, D. &. (2003, November). Practicing to be Miserable. Signalman, pp. 7-15. Grossman, D. &. (1999). Psychological Effects. In Kurtz (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, and Conflict (Vol. 3, p. 139). New York: Academic Press. Grossman, D. (1999). Psychological Effects of Combat: Physiological Arousal and Fear. In L. &. Kurtz, Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, and Conflict. New York: Academic Press. Hein, G. &. (2008). Superior temporal sulcus--It’s my area: or is it? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 (12), 2125-36. Henneman, W. e. (2009). Hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 72 (11), 999-1007. Herley, S. (2001). Perception And Action: Alternative Views. Synthese, 129 (1), 3-40. Hubner, M. A. (2008). The Effects of Praising Qualifying Autoclitics on the Frequency of Reading. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24 (1), 55-62. Iacoboni, M. K. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. PNAS, 98 (24), 13995-13999. Pierce, C. (1991). Peirce on Signs. (J. Hoopes, Ed.) The University of North Carolina Press. Pike, J. S. (1997). Chronic life stress alters symapthetic, neuroendocrine, and immune response to an acute psychological stressor in humans. Psychometric Medicine, 59 (4), 447-457. Sapolsky, R. (2000). Glucocorticoids and Hippocampal Atrophy in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 923-935. Segal, E. (1975). Psycholinguistics Discovers the Operant: A Review of Roger Brown’s A First Language:The Early Stages. Journal of the Experiemental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 149-158. Semendeferi, K. L. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5 (3), 272-276. Perceptual Distortion and Stress Taylor, S. K. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 441-429. Tosh, C. &. (2010). Modeling Perception with Artifical Networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Inc. Vincent, J. K. (2008). Evidence for a Frontoparietal Control System Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100 (6), 3328–3342. Von Holst, E. &. (1971). The Principle of Reafference: Interaction Between the Central Nervous System and the Periphrial Organs. In P. Dodwell, Perceptual Processing: Stimulus Equivilence and Pattern Recognition (pp. 41-72). New York: Meridith Corporation. Whitesell, R. H. (1977). Dynamic Aspects of Glucose Transport Modulation in Thymocytes*. The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 252 (10), 3533-3537. Wilson, J. &. (2003). The learning combination lock – an experiential approach to learning design. Journal of Eurpoean Industrial Training, 27 (2,3,4), 88-97. Perceptual Distortion and Stress