Journal of 10.1177/1043986204263778 Maruna / DESISTANCE Contemporary FROM Criminal CRIME Justice / May 2004 Desistance From Crime and Explanatory Style A New Direction in the Psychology of Reform SHADD MARUNA University of Cambridge Research on offender verbalizations traditionally focuses on the degree to which offenders accept responsibility (or blame) for their mistakes. This small study expands this cognitive perspective in criminology by incorporating basic findings from the psychological literature on attributions. Additionally, offender attributions or neutralizations are framed within a life course perspective. It was found that active offenders and desisting ex-offenders differ in terms of explanatory style. Compared to desisting ex-offenders, active offenders tend to interpret negative events in their lives as being the product of internal, stable, and global forces. On the other hand, active offenders were more likely to believe that the good events in their lives were the product of external, unstable and specific causes. These other dimensions of offender cognitions may be useful in understanding the psychological aspects of desistance from crime. Keywords: desistance from crime; explanatory style; attribution theory; neutralization theory C riminologists have long been interested in the explanations or “vocabularies of motive” used by offenders when accounting for their behaviors. Sykes and Matza (1957) argued that these verbalizations, when they are able to “neutralize” an individual’s sense of shame for an offense, can at least partially account for persistence in crime, although the evidence on this count has been mixed (see Agnew, 1994; Copes, 2003). More recently, it has been This research was funded by the Joseph Rowntree Foundation and the H. F. Guggenheim Foundation. Considerable thanks are owed to Dan Casullo, Rik Legault, Samantha Lundrigan, Louise Porter, and Jason Zeliph for their assistance in collecting and coding the data in this analysis. This article has also benefited from comments by Hart Blanton, Ros Burnett, Scott Cunningham, Stephen Farrall, Nathan Harris, Christopher Kierkus, Anna King, Tom LeBel, Alex Piquero, Ed de St. Aubin, Hans Toch, and Richard Wiebe. Substantial assistance from Christopher Peterson and Tony Ward was most especially appreciated. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, Vol. 20 No. 2, May 2004 184-200 DOI: 10.1177/1043986204263778 © 2004 Sage Publications 184 Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 185 argued that to successfully desist from crime, former offenders may need to make sense of their past lives of crime in specific ways (see, for example, Maruna, 2001). Braithwaite and Braithwaite (2001) describe this sort of sense-making for past lives of crime as a form of “shame management”: In the social-developmental literature, attributions of stability and intentionality in the context of wrongdoing have been associated with maladaptive outcomes (e.g., shame, anxiety, despair). In contrast, attributions that connote expectations of change for the better in the wrongdoer and avoid labeling individuals as possessing global personality deficits have been associated with positive outcomes (e.g., pro-social behavior, empathy, self-esteem). (p. 258) Certainly, explanatory accounts for past crimes are considered to be of paramount importance in the applied world of offender rehabilitation with the ascendancy of cognitive-based correctional treatment (e.g., Bush, 1995; McGuire, 1995). To date, however, there has been very little research that examines the relationship between the key dimensions of explanatory style and the process of rehabilitation. EXPLANATORY STYLE IN PSYCHOLOGY “Explanatory style” can be defined as a person’s tendency to offer similar sorts of explanations for different events in their life narrative (Peterson, Buchanan, & Seligman, 1995). According to this view, individuals spontaneously construct explanations (or attributions) for significant and unexpected life events (Ward, Hudson, Johnston, & Marshall, 1997), and these schematized interpretations are thought to be proximally responsible for the continuity of a person’s actions over time, through “reactive person-environment interactions” (Dodge, 1993). According to Bruner (1987), Eventually, the culturally shaped cognitive and linguistic processes that guide the self-telling of life narratives achieve the power to structure perceptual experience, to organize memory, to segment and purpose-build the very “events” of a life. In the end, we become the autobiographical narratives by which we “tell about” our lives. (p. 15) Considerable research suggests that processing biases seem to occur in three salient dimensions in this cognitive landscape: • Internality (“I am solely responsible for this good/bad event”) versus externality (“This event is someone else’s fault/responsibility”); 186 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 • Stability (“The cause is going to last forever”) versus instability (“The cause will be short-lived”); • Globality (“It is going to affect everything I do”) versus specificity (“It’s only going to influence this one thing”). Explanatory styles that are particularly skewed toward one of these extremes (e.g., narratives in which people hold themselves solely responsible for everything that happens to them) are thought to correlate with specific behavioral patterns. As such, considerable work in cognitive psychology focuses on working to change these thinking patterns as a means of changing the consequent behavior (see McGuire, 1995). According to Seligman (1991), “One of the most significant findings in psychology in the last twenty years is that individuals can choose the way they think” (p. 8). The best known application of this framework is in depression research and therapy. In their “revised helplessness theory” of depression, Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978) assign a central role to the influence of causal thinking and emphasize the unique contribution of each of these dimensions of explanatory style. The theory predicts that individuals who have an explanatory style that invokes substantially internal, stable, and global attributions for negative life events (and external, unstable, and specific attributions for positive events) will be most at risk for becoming depressed when faced with unfortunate circumstances like the loss of a job or a relationship break-up. Persons who use highly internal explanations for negative life events are thought to increase their risk for depression because of the threat this poses to their self-esteem. The stability-instability dimension of causal beliefs is thought to affect the chronicity of helplessness and depression following bad events. Finally, the globality-specificity of causal beliefs influences the “pervasiveness of deficits following bad events” (Peterson & Seligman, 1984, pp. 348-349). On the other hand, the theory suggests that mentally healthy individuals do tend to use highly internal, stable, and global attributions when accounting for good things that happen in one’s life. In Learned Optimism, Seligman (1991) summarizes these findings clearly: For nondepressives, failure events tend to be external, temporary, and specific, but good events are personal, permanent and pervasive. “If it’s bad, you did it to me, it’ll be over soon, and it’s only this situation. But if it’s good, I did it, it’s going to last forever, and it’s going to help me in many situations.” (p. 110) This “beneffectance” (Greenwald, 1980) is understood as “self-enhancing biases that distort appraisals in the positive direction” (Bandura, 1989, p. 1177). Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 187 These hypotheses have received substantial research support (Dodge, 1993) but do suffer from something like a “chicken-or-the-egg” dilemma. That is, it is not always clear if pessimistic thinking increases the risk of depression or if depression increases the risk of pessimistic thinking. McAdams (1994) writes, The truth probably lies with a little bit of both possibilities—cognition as both a cause and a result of the personality constellation we call depression, personality and cognition influencing each other, linked in an intricate web of mutual causation. (p. 511) HYPOTHESES There is little guidance in the literature concerning the relationship between criminality and the three key dimensions of explanatory style, yet there is some reason to believe that offender psychology might be similar to that of depression. Although criminal conduct is typically categorized as externalizing behavior, whereas depression is thought of as internalizing, recent research demonstrates considerable overlap between the occurrence of these two phenomena (Capaldi, 1992; McLeod & Shanahan, 1993; Quiggle, Garber, Panak, & Dodge, 1992). This seems to be particularly true of long-term, habitual offenders—the “great pretenders” (Shover, 1996) who recycle in and out of prisons, jails, and drug treatment facilities with great regularity (see, especially, the psychological portrait in Zamble & Quinsey, 1997). Whereas O’Connor, McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin (1992) attribute this to “a common genetic liability,” it is also clear that criminal behavior and depression share common sociological antecedents and risk factors (e.g., stressful life events, abusive pasts, low social-structural positions) (see de Coster & Heimer, 2001; Hoffman & Su, 1998). As such, it was hypothesized (based on the exploratory findings in Maruna, 2001) that active offenders might tend to differ from reformed former offenders in the same way that depressives differ from nondepressives. That is, offenders might view positive events in their lives as being the product of external, unstable, and specific causes (i.e., “lucky breaks”) and view negative events as the product of internal, stable, and global causes (e.g., “That’s just the type of person I am,” “Bad to the bone,” “Born to lose,” etc.). As such, we hypothesized that the process of desistance from crime, like recovering from depression (see Beck, 1963; Ellis, 1962), might involve adapting these thinking patterns. Desisting ex-offenders should therefore view positive events as the product of more internal, stable, and global causes (i.e., “ . . . because I am a good person deep down”) and negative events as the 188 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 product of more external, unstable, and specific causes (e.g., “That was just a phase I was going through,” or “That wasn’t the ‘real me,’it just happened”). THE STUDY Using data collected as part of the Liverpool Desistance Study (LDS) (see Maruna, 2001), the following analysis is a preliminary attempt to explore the role of explanatory style in the desistance process. The LDS sample provides a unique, although certainly not ideal, set of data for empirically exploring the relationship between explanatory style and desistance. The LDS involved life story interviews with more than 100 British citizens who had formerly spent time in prison for a variety of crimes—mostly drug-related and property offending. None had been in prison for murder, sexual assault, or manslaughter. The only individuals excluded from the study, however, were those who had no offending “career” to speak of (in that, they self-reported only one or two criminal episodes) or had a history of convictions for sex offending primarily. Both groups seemed to us too different from the other sample members—who might be considered petty career offenders or the “deskilled criminal Lumpenproletariat” (Shover, 1996)—to merit inclusion. The goal of the LDS research was to understand the psychological mindset that seemed to best support efforts to “go straight” and maintain a desistance from crime. Approximately 55 of the LDS participants have been classified as desisting from crime (based on self-reports). These are all individuals who were once long-term, habitual offenders, but who at the time of the interview had been crime-free and drug-free for more than a year, and it is important that they also reported having no plans for future involvement in criminal behavior. These were not individuals who had stopped offending in the past or had already desisted. As such, our interest was not in learning how this desistance happened (i.e., “What made them stop?”) as if this was something that occurred in the past tense. These were individuals who were desisting “before our eyes.” That is, the word desistance does not refer to a termination event that takes place at the time of a last offense. Desistance is the process of refraining or abstaining, in this case from illegal behavior. As such, the 55 persons being interviewed were categorized as actively desisting because they were actively engaged in this ongoing process of self-restraint and selfdefinition. The LDS was designed to better understand this psychology (for a more extensive discussion of sampling and the operational definition of desistance, see Maruna, 2001). On the other side, 34 of these interviewees have been classified as persisting or active in their criminal careers. They reported recent criminal activity and also admitted to explicit plans to continue selling drugs, robbing convenience stores, and so forth. It is important that, following Wright and Decker Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 189 (1994), the “active group” is made up of individuals who are actively involved with criminal behavior in the community and not drawn from a captive population (see Polsky, 1969). The desisting and the persisting samples were intended to mirror one another as closely as possible on static variables such as age, gender, types and number of crimes committed, age of criminal onset, and high school completion. The two samples were also generally indistinguishable in a test of their “Big Five” personality traits (see Maruna, 2001, for this analysis). The remainder of the other interviews we have conducted have been with less easily classifiable individuals, who either fall in the vast gray area between desisting and persisting or else could not be considered to be persistent or career offenders. CONTENT ANALYSIS Although still small, this collection of narrative profiles is large enough to facilitate some basic and tentative explorations of the role of social cognition in criminal behavior. Life story interviews (2 hours on average) with LDS participants were tape-recorded and transcribed, preserving the original language of the interviewees. These transcripts were then content analyzed using Peterson, Schulman, Castellon, and Seligman’s (1992) Content Analysis of Verbatim Explanations (CAVE) system. Based on Seligman’s theory of explanatory styles, CAVE is an innovative and well-established method for measuring the cross-event consistency in the explanations individuals provide for both positive and negative events in their lives. The CAVE system has been used in studies of depression, precursors of mental illness, and the success of presidential candidates. This previous research provides strong support for the construct validity of the CAVE technique, and coders trained by the system’s authors have been able to achieve interrater reliability levels exceeding .90 (Peterson et al., 1991, p. 386). The primary advantage of this coding scheme, however, is that it allows for the examination of the three key dimensions of causal explanations (internality-externality; stability-instability; and globality-specificity) within the everyday language and actual life stories of individuals. Although pencil-and-paper questionnaires have also been designed to gauge these dimensions of explanatory style, narrative methodologies have certain advantages over more tightly structured questionnaire measures (Emmons, 1999, p. 63). In particular, narrative analysis allows for a contextualization of these themes that is quite difficult to accomplish with standard screening instruments. According to Peterson (1992), “Idiographic research shifts attention away from abstract psychological variables and reorients it toward the lives of individuals” (p. 107). 190 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 Further, the systematic content analysis of verbal material has the advantage of eliciting more spontaneous and less artificial responses than selfreport questionnaires (Harvey, Weber, & Orbuch, 1990). As such, Peterson (1992) describes content analysis as a “particularly good assessment strategy, not a ‘second-best’ procedure” for assessing attributions and cognitive style (p. 379). The circumstances under which causal explanations are made without prompting are precisely those under which subjects are most likely to be mindful and thus most apt not to respond glibly or automatically as they might on a questionnaire. (p. 379) Nonetheless, the use of content analysis introduces numerous reliability problems, stemming from the multiple interpretations of subjective materials. As such, significant measures were taken in this analysis to protect against bias in the coding process. Two graduate students, blind to the hypotheses of this research, were trained in Peterson’s method for extracting attributions from transcribed interview material (see Peterson et al., 1991, pp. 383-386). A person’s attributional style is thought to differ depending on whether the explanation is provided for a positive event (“I got a job last week”) or a negative event (“I got fired last week”). Therefore, extracted attributions were divided into positive attributions and negative attributions at this stage. The events extracted were not all criminal acts. Explanatory style suggests that individuals are consistent in their accounting styles across a variety of acts. The only criteria for inclusion in this research, therefore, was that an act was either clearly a negative or positive event in the person’s life. This judgment was made by the student extractors based on the context surrounding the passage in the longer narrative transcript. These students were able to identify at least four negative life events and a minimum of three positive life events in every transcript using the rather conservative and specific criteria for extractions specified by Peterson and colleagues. The open-ended, life history interviews from which these transcripts were made differed considerably in length, ranging from 1 hour to more than 3 hours in many cases. As such, it is not unusual that the number of events mentioned would vary from transcript to transcript. Nonetheless, the mean number of negative and positive events extracted were not significantly different for the persisting and desisting groups, suggesting at a minimum that the measures are equally reliable. That is, were desisting narratives to contain 20 extracted events, whereas only 10 could be found for the persisting group, one might worry about systematic biases in comparability between the two samples. Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 191 Once these passages were extracted from the full-text transcripts, two additional graduate students, also blind to the hypotheses of this research, coded the explanations using Peterson’s content coding scheme. After a 2week process of training using pilot interview transcripts, the coders were provided with long lists of unidentified attributions, extracted from the larger context of the life story interview and randomized within and between participants. This list, therefore, took the following form: EVENT: A bad thing happened. EXPLANATION: Because I was misled by my peers. EVENT: A good thing happened. EXPLANATION: Because I worked hard to make it happen. Because of the precautions taken in randomizing the presentation of these anonymous passages, raters were not biased by previous ratings for the same subject and would have no way of easily connecting any series of passages. Most important, coders had no way of knowing whether the speaker of any particular passage was an active offender or a desisting ex-offender from the information they were given. Any passage that mentioned desistance or persistence in crime specifically and in the present tense (e.g., “That is what has kept me straight these last few years . . . ” or “which is why I am still selling drugs today”) was obviously excluded from the coding or else modified in such a way as to remove the reference to desistance/persistence. Coders rated each extracted attribution on three dimensions (internal, stable, and global) using a scale of 1 to 7, with 7 representing the highest possible score. Coders were instructed to rate each dimension independently of the other dimensions. Indeed, coders found it easiest to code all of the extractions on a single dimension (e.g., stability), then return to the beginning of the list and recode all of the same extractions on a different dimension. Their focus was not on the positive or negative events themselves but on the explanations for the events. The scores are meant to represent the speaker’s point of view and not the rater’s perspective on the particular event. Tables 3, 4, and 5 provide prototypical examples of the types of extracted explanations that received high scores (5 to 7 points) or low scores (1 to 3 points) on these three key dimensions. All of these responses are drawn from the actual LDS data. In coding the internal-external dimension (see Table 1), raters were trained to assign a score of 1 when the individual attributes blame or credit to someone or something completely external to the self. Ratings between 2 and 6 are assigned when explanations divide blame or credit between the self and others, with the higher numbers emphasizing one’s internal responsibility. A score of 7 is reserved for cases in which the individual attributes causality 192 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 TABLE 1 Internal-External Explanation Dimension Examples Negative internal explanations (scored 6 or 7) NEGATIVE EVENT: I never really spoke to my father (as a teen). EXPLANATION: Just because of the way I was. NEGATIVE EVENT: I got put in a battered wives’ home. EXPLANATION: Because I wasn’t a very good mother to my children. Positive internal explanations (scored 6 or 7) POSITIVE EVENT: They [the prosecution] never got no evidence on me. EXPLANATION: Cuz I was clever. POSITIVE EVENT: I got peace for the next few years [from those who had bullied me]. EXPLANATION: Because I had stood up to them. Negative external explanations (scored 1 or 2) NEGATIVE EVENT: I was mixing in with smackheads (addicts). EXPLANATION: Cuz they’re in the next cell. NEGATIVE EVENT: I just started to take drugs then. EXPLANATION: It was just where you lived, the thing to do. Positive external explanations (scored 1 or 2) POSITIVE EVENT: I just sailed by in prison. EXPLANATION: Me brothers had been in there, and they . . . had their reputation. POSITIVE EVENT: I don’t have much of a criminal record. EXPLANATION: ‘Cause my family is in the police. entirely to one’s own behavioral, physical, or mental characteristics (see Peterson et al., 1991, p. 387). The stability dimension (see Table 2) is intended to reflect the persistence or chronicity of an event’s cause (not the event itself). Raters were trained to take four characteristics of explanations into account when coding this dimension: verb tense, probability of recurrence, whether the cause is intermittent or continuous, and whether the attributed cause is characterological or behavioral in nature. A score of 1 would be reserved for an explanation that is based on something that occurred in the past, has no chance of happening again, was a sporadic event, and focuses on behavior rather than permanent traits of a person (e.g., “because I lost my first tooth that day”). On the opposite extreme, a score of 7 would be used for explanations that are in the present tense, have a strong likelihood of persistence, and focus on continuous traits (e.g., “because my family is cursed”). Finally, the global-specific dimension (see Table 3) reflects the extent to which a cause affects many aspects of a person’s life or just a few. According to Peterson and colleagues (1991), “This dimension often proves the most difficult to rate because there may not be enough information to indicate how widespread the effects of the cause might be” (p. 389). The coding manual Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 193 TABLE 2 Stable-Unstable Explanation Dimension Negative stable explanations (scored 6 or 7) NEGATIVE EVENT: They withdrew the [work] placement and said I couldn’t be employed by them. EXPLANATION: Because I am an ex-offender. NEGATIVE EVENT: My ex-partner tries to stop me from seeing my kid. EXPLANATION: Because she’s a spiteful cow. Positive stable explanations (scored 6 or 7) POSITIVE EVENT: Me mum and I get on great. EXPLANATION: She just knows me so well. POSITIVE EVENT: I stand by me mum. EXPLANATION: ‘Cause she’s just smothered me in love and it’s been like that all my life. Negative unstable explanations (scored 1 or 2) NEGATIVE EVENT: I don’t think I’m going to pass it [a qualifying test]. EXPLANATION: Because I haven’t fully applied myself to it. NEGATIVE EVENT: She won’t let me see my kids. EXPLANATION: Because I done the worst mistake possible in my life; I slept with a girl. Positive unstable explanations (scored 1 or 2) POSITIVE EVENT: I was running as well, you know, getting really fit. EXPLANATION: Because me medical [exam] was coming up. POSITIVE EVENT: I got off the heroin. EXPLANATION: ‘Cause my girlfriend, she was pregnant. suggests that raters think of a person’s life as involving two broad categories: achievement and affiliation. If an attributed cause is likely to have a strong effect on both dimensions (e.g., “because I’ve lost my will to carry on”), they are told to code it a 7. If the cause affects only one of the two aspects of one’s life, and only marginally so (e.g., “because I had to go to a conference that week”), it would be scored as a 1. In all, more than 1,250 separate attributions, an average of slightly more than 14 in each of the 89 interviews, were extracted and then double-coded by separate raters on all of the six key dimensions of explanatory style. This was a painstaking and highly labor-intensive process. In a measure of agreement, the two independent scorers achieved a correlation of .79 in their coding of these extractions. Large discrepancies between raters on a particular item were worked out in a conference between the two raters and the author. Most such disagreements were the result of one of the scorers inadvertently marking a 7, when intending to mark a 1 or vice versa. Remaining, smaller disagreements in coding (e.g., when the coders were one or two points away from each other) were settled by averaging the two coders’ scores. Final 194 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 TABLE 3 Global-Specific Explanation Dimension Negative global explanations (scored 6 or 7) NEGATIVE EVENT: I tried to kill myself. EXPLANATION: The drugs were just making me crazy. NEGATIVE EVENT: I went back to drugs after the accident. EXPLANATION: Because everything just fell apart for me. Positive global explanations (scored 6 or 7) POSITIVE EVENT: I’m going on another [college] course. EXPLANATION: Because I just want to do something positive with my life. POSITIVE EVENT: I’ve learned I’ve got to grow up now. EXPLANATION: [Because] I’ve got responsibilities and I’ve got to face life. Negative specific explanations (scored 1 or 2) NEGATIVE EVENT: I used to [skip school]. EXPLANATION: I wasn’t really interested in it. NEGATIVE EVENT: The last time I shoplifted was for food. EXPLANATION: Because I couldn’t afford to buy it. Positive specific explanations (scored 1 or 2) POSITIVE EVENT: I was the only one who stayed [with a certain training course]. EXPLANATION: I liked it; the others didn’t really understand it. POSITIVE EVENT: I am using Methadone now. EXPLANATION: I’m using that to balance myself out now. scores in the six dimensions represent an averaging of the ratings in each dimension across all of a participant’s extracted attributions. DATA ANALYSIS Once the data were coded in this way, we were able to ask whether the explanatory style of former long-term offenders desisting from crime was different from the style of former long-term offenders who were still persisting in criminal activities. The question being asked was in this sense explicitly correlational rather than causal. The hypothesis was that an optimistic explanatory style could help support and maintain efforts to stay crime free in a way that other cognitive mindsets could not. Logistic regression analysis was used to estimate the relationship between each of these dimensions of explanatory style and criminal activity status. The logistic regression procedures considered most appropriate for dichotomous dependent variables use maximum likelihood estimates of the parameters in the model (Long, 1997). Odds ratios of variables were calculated by raising the base of the natural logarithm to the power of the logit coefficient. For continuous predictors, such as the measures of explanatory style, the Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 195 TABLE 4 Pearson Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Explanatory Style Variables Desisting Group (N = 55) 1 1. Negative-internal 2. Negative-stable 3. Negative-global 4. Positive-internal 5. Positive-stable 6. Positive-global 2 3 4 — .228 .299* .218 .076 — .131 –.032 –.201 .037 — .239 .328 –.004 –.031 — .235 .369* .049 .173 –.089 –.099 .083 .032 5 6 .117 .042 .190 –.051 .058 .309* .166 .122 — .158 .290 — M SD 3.26 3.23 3.92 4.50 4.70 4.47 .80 .79 .71 .93 .77 .77 Persisting Group (N = 34) M SD 3.83* 1.35 3.76* .96 4.15 .77 4.06* 1.06 4.04* 1.11 3.81* .86 NOTE: Persisting group correlations are on the left; desisting group correlations are on the right. * p < .05. odds ratio indicates the change in the odds of an event occurring when the predictor increases by one unit, controlling for other variables in the model. FINDINGS Descriptive statistics for the independent variables are presented in Table 4 below. The mean desisting group scores differ from the persisting group on five of the six dimensions at a level that is statistically significant in means comparisons. All of the significant differences were in the hypothesized direction. The attribution variables were weakly correlated with one another, although not to the degree that the three dimensions seemed to be measuring the same construct.1 Table 5 presents the unstandardized logit coefficients and odds ratios for the logistic regression model of the main effects of explanatory style as predictors of desisting from criminal behavior. As hypothesized, internal, global, and stable explanations for negative life events were negatively associated with desistance from criminal behavior, and internal, global, and stable explanations for positive events were all positively associated with reform. Although all six measures had modest, zero-order correlations with criminal reform, the logistic regression indicates that only the three dimensions of positive explanations and the negative-internal dimension had unique and significant contributions (p < .05). All of the effects were in the expected direction. These results hold when variables such as age, marriage, gender, employment, age at first arrest, drug involvement, age at first conviction, and previous convictions are added to the model. This is almost certainly a result of the sampling procedures used. The range of ages and criminal histories of mem- 196 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 TABLE 5 Logistic Regression for Desistance From Crime Independent Variable Negative-internal Negative-stable Negative-global Positive-internal Positive-stable Positive-global Constant Model χ2 B –.8091 –.6564 –.6568 .7239 1.0296 .9991 –3.5394 39.40 S.E. .3172 .3614 .3932 .3440 .3811 .4035 3.0502 6 df p Value* .0107 .0693 .0948 .0353 .0069 .0133 .2459 .000 Exp(B) .4452 .5187 .5185 2.0624 2.7999 2.7158 * p values computed for one-tailed significance tests. bers in the Liverpool sample were purposefully restricted to ensure that the representatives in the desisting and persisting groups were drawn from the same types (generally, 25- to 35-year-old ex-convicts with extensive criminal records and drug use spanning at least a decade). To the extent that explanatory styles covary with demographic variables like age, race, and gender, this study might have also artificially restricted the range of attribution styles as well. SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION The findings from this exercise suggest an interesting, possible relationship between explanatory style and criminal desistance. First, negative-internal attributions are associated with persisting in criminal behavior. The odds ratio for negative-internal explanations is .45, meaning that the odds of being in the desisting group diminish by a factor of 1/.45 (or 2.22) for each unit increase in negative-internal explanations. This finding, which is largely consistent with the sociocognitive research on depression, suggests that someone might be less able to desist to the extent that negative events are seen as originating from internal sources (“This is just the way I am”)—especially when these are stable (“I’ve always been this way”) and global (“I fail at everything I do, no matter where I go”) characteristics. Examples of these explanations in the narratives of active offenders include the following: NEGATIVE EVENT: I wanted to get a job when I got out of prison, but I went back to crime. EXPLANATION: ‘Cause it’s the only way I know how. NEGATIVE EVENT: [My criminal activities] got worse and worse. EXPLANATION: Once you’ve had a taste of it, you want more. You want more and bigger and better things then. Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 197 On the other hand, the odds that a participant in this sample is in the desisting group more than double for each unit increase in their positive-internal, positive-stable, or positive-global explanations. One might speculate, therefore, that the more individuals are able to attribute positive life events to broad, long-lasting personal qualities (e.g., “Because I am a worthy individual”), the greater the odds may be that they will be able to stay crime free. Some examples of this sort of beneffectance or optimistic bias among desisting ex-offenders in the LDS sample include the following: POSITIVE EVENT: I love working with computers. EXPLANATION: Because it’s like I can be myself now. POSITIVE EVENT: I’d like to get a job doing counseling. EXPLANATION: Truthfully, I would, because I’d be good at it. I’m great with people. POSITIVE EVENT: I’m starting a (training) course, and I want to do it right. EXPLANATION: ‘Cause I’m pretty intelligent. Whether these explanations are objectively true or not (e.g., maybe the last speaker is not terribly intelligent in terms of some standardized test), these self-narratives seem to be the most supportive of efforts to maintain desistance from crime. They might therefore be seen as “positive illusions” (Taylor, 1989). These findings suggest that in our focus on offender neutralizations, criminologists might be missing a potent area for future research: offenders’ attributions for positive life events. Another implication of these results is that it may be useful to look beyond the internal-external dimension of locus of control in criminology. Starting with neutralization theory, criminologists have long been interested in whether offenders accept internal responsibility for their actions or blame these events on external factors. These findings suggest that equal attention might be paid to other dimensions of cognition related to controllability. In the LDS sample, the dimensions of stability and globality seem to be better stronger correlates than internality, and Guerra, Huesmann, and Zelli (1990) found precisely the same thing in their study of attributions and aggressive behavior. They conclude that their findings “shed some doubt on the utility of the locus of causality construct in understanding aggressive behavior among delinquents, but do suggest that investigations focused on controllability may prove useful” (p. 353). In fact, as explanatory style research has progressed in other areas of study, the internalityexternality dimension has become of less interest over the years. According to Peterson (2000), “It has more inconsistent correlates than do stability or 198 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 globality, it is less reliably assessed and there are theoretical grounds for doubting that it has a direct impact on expectations per se” (p. 48). NOTE 1. Of the three modest associations, two suggest a correlation between parallel explanations for positive and negative events within the samples. For example, in the active offender sample, participants who tend to blame stable forces for bad events seem more likely to also credit stable forces for good events. The same relationship is found with the globality dimension in the desisting sample, and this, of course, makes intuitive sense. At the same time, there are strong theoretical reasons (see Carver, 1989) for considering each dimension separately for negative and positive events in an exploratory study such as this. We do not know enough about the constellation of attribution styles of offenders to begin combining these dimensions into various constellations as is occasionally done in research on depression. REFERENCES Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87(1), 49-74. Agnew, R. (1994). The techniques of neutralization and violence. Criminology, 32, 555-580. Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175-1184. Beck, A. (1963). Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324-333. Braithwaite, J., & Braithwaite, V. (2001). Part four. In E. Ahmed, N. Harris, J. Braithwaite, & V. Braithwaite (Eds.), Shame management through reintegration. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bush, J. (1995). Teaching self-risk management to violent offenders. In J. McGuire (Ed.), What works: Reducing reoffending. New York: Wiley. Capaldi, D. M. (1992). The co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: Familial factors and general adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 277-300. Carver, C. S. (1989). How should multi-faceted personality constructs be tested? Issues illustrated by self-monitoring, attributional style, and hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 577-585. Copes, H. (2003). Societal attachments, offending frequency, and techniques of neutralization. Deviant Behavior, 24, 101-127. de Coster, S., & Heimer, K. (2001). The relationship between law violation and depression: An interactionist analysis. Criminology, 39, 799-836. Dodge, K. A. (1993). Social cognitive mechanisms in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 559-584. Maruna / DESISTANCE FROM CRIME 199 Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New York: L. Stuart. Emmons, R. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns. New York: Guilford. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603-618. Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L. R., & Zelli, A. (1990). Attributions for social failure and aggression in incarcerated delinquent youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 347-355. Harvey, J. H., Weber, A. L., & Orbuch, T. L. (1990). Interpersonal accounts: A social psychological perspective. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Hoffman, J. P., & Su, S. S. (1998). Stressful life events and adolescent substance use and depression: Conditional and gender differentiated effects. Substance Use and Misuse, 33, 2219-2262. Long, J. S. (1995). Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Maruna, S. (2001). Making good: How ex-convicts reform and rebuild their lives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Books. McAdams, D. P. (1994). The person (2nd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. McLeod, J. D., & Shanahan, M. J. (1993). Poverty, parenting, and children’s mental health. American Journal of Sociology, 58, 351-366. O’Connor, T. G., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (1992). Co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and antisocial behavior in adolescence: A common genetic liability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 27-37. Peterson, C. (1992). Explanatory style. In C. P. Smith & J. W. Atkinson (Eds.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis. New York: Cambridge (pp. 376-382). Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism. American Psychologist, 55, 44-55. Peterson, C., Buchanan, G. M., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1993). Explanatory style: History and evolution of the field. In G. M. Buchanan & M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Explanatory style. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Peterson, C., Schulman, P., Castellon, C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1993). The explanatory style scoring manual. In C. P. Smith & J. W. Atkinson (Eds.), Motivation and personality. New York: Cambridge. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91(3), 347-374. Polsky, N. (1969). Hustlers, beats, and others. Garden City: Doubleday. Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social informationprocessing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 13051320. Seligman, M.E.P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: Knopf. Shover, N. (1996). Great pretenders. Boulder, CO: Westview. Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664-670. Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions. New York: Basic Books. 200 Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice / May 2004 Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., Johnston, L., & Marshall, W. L. (1995). Cognitive distortions in sex offenders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 479-507. Wright, R. T., & Decker, S. H. (1993). Burglars on the job: Streetlife and residential break-ins. Boston: Northeastern. Zamble, E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1997). The criminal recidivism process. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Shadd Maruna is a lecturer at the Institute of Criminology at the University of Cambridge (UK). Previously, he was an assistant professor at the School of Criminal Justice at the Unviersity at Albany, State University of New York. His research focuses on ex-offender reintegration and desistance from crime. His book Making Good: How Ex-convicts Reform and Rebuild their Lives(American Psychological Association Books, 2001) was awarded the American Society of Criminology’s Michael J Hindelang Award for 2001. He has recently co-edited a new book on the topic, After Crime and Punishment: Pathways to Offender Reintegration (Willan Publishing, 2004).