The Dispositional Approach To Job Attitudes: A Lifetime Longitudinal Test Author(s): Barry M. Staw, Nancy E. Bell, John A. Clausen Source: Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 31, No. 1 (Mar., 1986), pp. 56-77 Published by: Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2392766 Accessed: 07/12/2009 21:26 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=cjohn. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Administrative Science Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org The Dispositional ApproachTo Job Attitudes: A LifetimeLongitudinalTest BarryM. Staw, Nancy E. Bell, and John A. Clausen Universityof California, Berkeley ? 1986 by CornellUniversity. 0001-8392/86/3101-0056/$1.00 Thisresearchwas supportedinpartby grantAG4178 fromthe NationalInstitute of Agingto the Instituteof HumanDevelopment(JohnClausen,principalinvestigator)andby a Universityof California facultyresearchgrantto BarryStaw. Correspondenceregardingthis papershould be sent to BarryM. Staw, Schoolof Business Administration, Universityof California,Berkeley,CA94720. Recent debates between the job enrichment and socialinformation-processing perspectives have led to a trend toward greater situationalism in organizational research. This paper, however, argues for a more dispositional approach in which the role of the person is emphasized. Using a longitudinal sample, measures of affective disposition from as early as adolescence were used to predict job attitudes in later life. Results showed that dispositional measures significantly predicted job attitudes over a time span of nearly fifty years. The implications of these findings are discussed in terms of both theories of job attitudes and organizational development activities that attempt to alter employee job satisfactions Not only have hundredsof studies on job attitudesattempted to outlinethe determinantsof job satisfaction,but theories about how individualsrespondto work have been the center of some of the most active controversiesin organizationalresearch. Thisemphasis is understandable,given the importance of job satisfactionto individualwelfare and its correlationwith other outcomes (e.g., absenteeism and turnover)of concern to organizations. At present, the focus of job-attituderesearchrevolves around the debate between job enrichmentand social-informationprocessing theories. Drawingon Maslow's (1954) theoryof individualneeds, advocates of job enrichmenthave arguedthat most worktasks failto challenge individualsadequatelyor fulfilltheirdesires for growthand stimulation,resultingin widespreadworkdissatisfactionand unproductivebehavior (e.g., Herzberg,1966; Hackmanand Oldham,1980; Lawler, 1982). Thus, by redesigningtask characteristics,it is posited, individualscan become both satisfied and intrinsicallymotivated on the job (Hackmanet al., 1975). So far,the job enrichmentapproachhas received considerableempiricalsupport,althoughthe bulkof the data has come fromsurveys using self-reportmeasures of both task characteristicsand job attitudes, ratherthanfrom studies using objective measures of job characteristics,(e.g., Taber,Beehr, and Walsh, 1985) or well-documentedfield experiments (e.g., Goodman,1979). theory has made two key Social-information-processing attacks on the job-enrichmentapproach.First,need theories were characterizedas ambiguousand unnecessary explanations of workattitudes (Salancikand Pfeffer, 1977). Then,as a replacementfor need fulfillment,social informationprocessing was suggested as a mechanismthat determines whether individualsrespond positivelyor negativelyto task situations (Salancikand Pfeffer, 1978). Ithas been arguedthat individuals'attitudesare not a functionof deep-seated needs but a productof how people sociallyconstructthe worldaround them. Thatis, because tasks are inherentlyambiguous,individualsmay interpretthem inways that are dictated by the context and meaningof theirown actions. Thus, any information that a task is interesting(perhapsprovidedby others or throughthe observationof one's own behaviorin its social context) could be as strong an influenceon job attitudes as the objective propertiesof the task. Validationof the socialtheory has come most directlyfrom information-processing 56/AdministrativeScience Quarterly,31 (1986): 56-77 Job Attitudes laboratorystudies inwhich manipulatedopinionsof others were shown to influencetask satisfaction(O'Reillyand Caldwell, 1979; Whiteand Mitchell,1979; Weiss and Shaw, 1979) and indirectlyfromexperimentson overjustificationthat show the interactionof intrinsicand extrinsicrewardson task attitudes (e.g., Deci, 1975; Lepper,Greene, and Nisbett, 1973; Staw, Calder,and Hess, 1980). The Emerging Situational Approach The debate between job enrichmentand social information processing has contributedto two recent shifts in theories about how people reactto work environments.The first change has been greateremphasis on subjectiveand cognitive factors, with wider acceptance that interpretationof the work situationis at least as importantas objective reality.Although job-enrichmenttheorists have long used perceptualmeasures in assessing task characteristics(e.g., Hackmanand Oldham, 1975; Sims, Szilagyi,and Keller,1976), individualcognitionhas historicallybeen treated more as a source of errorvariance than as an importantcausal factor.Thus,the conflictbetween the social-information-processing andjob-enrichmentpositions has put new life in the search for why a single job can be perceived in such differentways (O'Reilly,Parlette,and Bloom, 1980). The second majorshift injob-attitudetheory has been a move towardgreaterenvironmentaldeterminism.Not only have need-based theories come undersevere criticism,but recent attempts to finda coherent set of individualdifferences that moderatethe effects of job enrichmenthave not been particularlysuccessful (White,1978). Consequently,fromwhat was essentially an interactionalpositionemphasizingthe proper "fit"between individualsand workcontexts (e.g., Hackman and Oldham,1976), we have now moved furtherin the direction of emphasizingsituationalinfluence.Currentlyemphasized are ways to manipulateor change job attitudesexternally,using eitherobjectiveinfluences derivedfromtask design or subjective influences derivedfrom social cues. The best example of such a synthesis is Griffin's(1983) recent field experimentinwhich bothjob enlargementand social labeling were shown to increase the satisfactionof workers in two factorysettings. Restoring the Dispositional Perspective Althoughthe confrontationbetween job-enrichmentand theories has contributedsubsocial-information-processing stantiallyto our knowledge of job attitudes,there has been, we believe, an unfortunatebyproductof this debate. Because of interpretiveand empiricalproblemswith need theory (Wahba and Bridwell,1976) as well as the recent emphasis on situationalinfluence,the field has very nearlyeliminated individual-level variablesfromthe study of job attitudes.The field is no longeras interested in what the individualbringsto the worksetting interms of behavioraltendencies, traits,and personality(now commonlysubsumed underthe rubricof personaldispositions)as in how the organizationcan externally prodthe individualto evoke more positivejob attitudesand behavior. 57/ASQ, March1986 The currenttrendtowardsituationalismin organizationalresearch correspondsto similarleaningsin psychologyduring the 1960s and 1970s. Overthese twenty years, dispositional concepts were criticizedon manygrounds,the most tellingof which was Mischel's (1968)argumentthat personalityscales or traitmeasures account for littlevariancein behavioracross situations. Morerecently,however, a largenumberof counterarguments have surfaced indefense of personalitydeterminants of behavior.Bem and Allen(1974) noted that the behaviorof some but not all individualsis consistent across situations. Block(1977)arguedthat in-depthassessments of personalityby trainedspecialists are much more predictive thanthe paper-and-pencilmeasures of traitsthat are commonlyused. McGowanand Gormly(1976)and Aries, Gold,and Weigel (1983) posited that personalitytraitsare more predictive of multipleinstances of behaviorthan behaviorin a single situation.Buss and Craik(1983) noted that personalityconstructs are best used as predictorswhen they are based on aggregate levels of previousbehavior.Monson, Hesley, and Chernick(1982) showed that personalityis more predictiveof behaviorin ambiguoussituationsthan in settings inwhich role demands are so strong that behavioris externallydetermined regardlessof personaldispositions.And,finally,Funderand Ozer(1983) demonstratedthatthe statisticalmagnitudeof manyof the most famous situationaleffects (e.g., forced compliance,bystanderintervention,and obedience) is no greaterthanthat achieved by the more heavilycriticizeddispositionalresearch.Thus, a consensus has startedto emerge that dispositionalconstructs, when properlymeasuredand relatedto behaviorsthat are conceptuallyrelevant,may be useful explanatoryvariablesafterall. Currentpersonalitytheory has also moved from a defensive posture, attemptingto justifydispositionalresearch,to a more constructivestance. Not only has there been a resurgence of empiricalresearchon dispositionaldeterminantsof behavior (Buss and Craik,1985), but a growingnumberof studies in behaviorgenetics are now providingratherconvincingevidence for the heritabilityof many individualcharacteristics (Buss and Plomin,1984). Studies comparingidenticalvs. fraternaltwins as well as data on the similarityof twins raised aparthave, for example, shown very strong indicationsof a genetic basis for both personalityand mentalabilities(Plomin, DeFries,and McClearn,1980; Bouchard,1984). Thoughbehaviorgeneticists are carefulnot to deny the role of environment or the interactionof heredityand environment,the main effect of individualdispositionson attitudesand behavioris becoming more firmlyestablished. Inthe context of organizationalbehavior,dispositionalresearch is also makingsomething of a comeback.AlthoughMaslow's need theory has been largelydiscreditedand neitherdemographicnorpersonalityvariableshave provedto be strong determinantsof organizationalbehavior,argumentsare starting to be formulatedfor improvementsinthe use of dispositionalconstructs. Weiss and Adler(1984) have noted, for example, that dispositionalvariablesmay have been such weak predictorsof organizationalbehaviorbecause they have been used largelyin an atheoreticalmannerto explainadditionalvariancein situationalstudies. Off-the-shelfmeasures of 58/ASQ, March1986 Job Attitudes personalityas well as easily collected demographicfactors are usuallyadded to surveys and experimentsas ancillaryor moderatingvariables.Seldom is much thought given to the theoreticalcorrespondence between dispositionalvariables and the organizationalbehaviorsthey are intendedto predict. Norare there manystudies designed inwhich situational factors are variedso as to help develop the constructvalidityof dispositionalvariablesor show underwhat conditionsdispositionalfactorswill best explainwork behavior.Thus,as Weiss and Adler(1984) have argued,a byproductof havingputalmost all ourtheoreticalenergies intodemonstratingsituational effects is thatwe may now mistakenlyinferthatdispositional variableshave littleto contributeto organizationalattitudesand behavior. Inthis paperwe outlineevidence for a dispositionalexplanation of job attitudesand attempt to show why it is useful to restore considerationof the individualto researchon job satisfaction.The argumentthatjob satisfactioncan have a dispositionalsource is not necessarilya new idea, but it is one that has been distinctlyout of favorfor at least twenty years. The dispositionalpositioncan be tracedto some of the earliest writingsin organizationalpsychology. Munsterberg(1913: 198), for example, noted that ". . . the feeling of monotony depends much less uponthe particularkindof workthan upon the special dispositionof the individual."Hoppock(1935: 5), in summarizingthe results of the firstextensive study of job satisfaction,also noted that a multitudeof dispositionaland demographicfactors ". . . may be just as importantas the job itself in determiningwhat we tentativelychoose to calljob satisfaction."And,in the 1940s and 1950s there were many survey researchstudies that correlatedmiscellaneous demographicand personalityvariablesto job satisfaction(forreviews, see Herzberget al., 1957; Vroom,1964; Locke, 1976). Thoughfew of these previousattempts to explainjob satisfaction with dispositionalvariablesyielded strong or theoretically meaningfulresults, we would argue, likeWeiss and Adler (1984), that it is time to improvedispositionalexplanations ratherthanabandonthem. Toward a Dispositional Theory of Job Attitudes As a startingpointfor reexaminingdispositionalsources of job satisfaction,it is useful to considerwhether job attitudes have some consistency over time. We would not go as faras Buss and Craik(1985) inarguingthat stabilityis the sine qua non for dispositions,but we would note that examiningtemporal stabilityis a valuablefirststep informulatingdispositional explanations.Evidenceof temporalstabilityprovidesat least a clue, if not evidence, that some dispositionalforces may be operatingon attitudesand behavior.And, more importantly, withoutconsideringtemporalconsistency, dispositional theories must resortto complicatedstage and interactional models to explainjob attitudes. Severalfindingssuggest thatjob attitudesdo have some temporalstability.Schneiderand Dachler(1978),for example, found strong temporalconsistency in satisfactionscores, with the correlationsbetween pre-and post-measures of job attitudes averaging.56 for managersand .58 for nonmanagersin a 59/ASQ, March1986 16-monthlongitudinalstudy. Pulakosand Schmitt(1983) also found that highschool students' pre-employmentexpectations of satisfactionwere a significantpredictorof subsequent job attitudes. Finally,some indirectsupportfor temporalconsistency comes fromthe fact that nationalsurvey data have shown ratherstable levels of job satisfactioneven though there have been manyobjectivechanges in the qualityof worklifeover the past decades (Quinn,Staines, and McCollough, 1974; Quinnand Staines, 1979; Weaver, 1980). Inorderto demonstratedispositionalsources of job satisfaction it is also importantto establish cross-situationalconsistency - to show that behaviorcan be predictedacross disparate contexts as well as withinthem (cf. Mischel, 1968). So far, the strongest evidence for the cross-situationalconsistency of job attitudes comes froma recent study by Staw and Ross (1985). They reanalyzeddata fromthe NationalLongitudinal Survey(originallydesigned to study laboreconomics issues) and found stabilityinjob attitudes over time and situation.Over both three- and five-yeartime intervalsStaw and Ross found significantconsistency injob satisfactionwhen people worked for the same employerand in the same occupation.More importantly,theirreanalysesof the NationalLongitudinalSurvey data showed significantconsistency injob satisfaction when individualschanged both the employerforwhom they worked and theiroccupation. The findingsof temporaland cross-situationalstabilityinjob attitudes are importantyet preliminarysteps in formulatinga dispositionalapproachto job attitudes.They create a basis for what Buss and Craik(1983) have identifiedas an actuarial approachto dispositionalresearch, providinga summaryof past attitudesthat is useful for predictionsover time and situation.Such consistency datado not, however, constitute a dispositionaltheoryof job attitudes, since they have littleto say about why individualsmay show stabilityinjob satisfaction. To formulatea dispositionaltheorywe must begin to understandsome of the mechanisms that underliethese consistency effects. Job Attitudes As a Function of Affective Disposition One clue about dispositionalsources of job satisfactionwas providedlong ago by Fisherand Hanna(1931).They argued that job dissatisfactionmay be caused as much by the ongoing emotionalstate of the individualas by any objective propertiesof the job. Intheirdispositionalexplanationof job attitudes, Fisherand Hanna(1931: vii-viii)proposed an attributionalprocess not too differentfromthe social-informationprocessing ideas now popularin organizationalbehavior: The emotionallymaldevelopedindividualis almost certainto become maladjustedsooner or laterto some or several of the variousmajor apects of his everydaylife. Inasmuchas his feelings and emotions are inherentaspects of himself,he carriesthem with him,so to speak, into every situationwhich he enters. Now, since he does not usually know the reason of his dissatisfaction,does not understandthe whyforand natureof his maladjustment,it is not surprisingthat he very frequentlyattaches or attributesit (hisdissatisfaction)to his work or his workingsituation. Althoughwe would not labeljobdissatisfactionas a pathology, we do agree with Fisherand Hannathat the person's internal 60/ASQ,March1986 Job Attitudes state can serve as an importantstimulusfor the interpretation of job information.People may bringa positiveor negative dispositionto the work setting, process informationabout the job in a way that is consistent with this disposition,and then experience job satisfactionor dissatisfactionas a result.Thus, likesocial-information-processing theory,we would posit that jobs are ambiguousstimulisubject to the cognitive manipulation of meaning. However,we would argue that the formulation of task attitudes can come as much fromthe internalstate of the individualas fromany externalcues. The hypothesis that personaldispositionsaffect job attitudes could draw at least indirectsupportfromthree separate findings. First,there is substantialindividualvariationin the perception of tasks that have identicalformaljob descriptions (O'Reilly,Parlette,and Bloom, 1980). Thus, there is probably enough ambiguityin most job situationsto allow individualsto interpretthe work inways that fit theirown dispositions. Second, there is substantialevidence of a relationshipbetween job and life satisfaction(e.g., Orpen,1978; Near, Rice, and Hunt,1978; Weaver, 1978) as well as a linkbetween job attitudesand mental health(Kornhauser,1965; Gechmanand Wiener, 1975; Kahn,1981). Althoughthese data are usually interpretedas evidence of the effects of job satisfaction,they can also reflecta more generaldispositionalsyndrome (Schmittand Pulakos,1986). A thirdpiece of data indirectly supportingthe dispositionalperspective is the fact that few well-documentedfield experiments have shown long-term changes injob attitudes. Oldhamand Hackman(1980) have arguedthat most organizationalchanges are eithertoo weak to have an effect or are resisted by traditionalpractices inorganizations.What has not been noted, however, is the possibility that attitudechanges may not persist over time because of a tendency for satisfactionto returnto an equilibriumstate determinedby one's affective disposition(cf. Landy,1978). Inourview, a primecandidatefor explainingdispositional sources of job satisfactionis the affective state of the individual. Historicallythere has been substantialinterest in the psychologicalstates of mood and depression (e.g., Beck, 1967; Zuckerman,1977). However,only recentlyhas it been noted that positiveor negative affectivitymay constitute an importanton-goingdispositionof individuals,perhapsintegrating manydisparatepersonalitydimensions (Watsonand Clark, 1984). Recent researchon identicaltwins raisedaparthas also revealedthat affectivity,likemanyother individualdifferences, could have a genetic basis that persists over one's lifetime (Bouchard,1984, 1985). Thus, people's affective dispositions can be thoughtof as generaltendencies toward positive or negative evaluationof life stimuli- tendencies that should influence-theway people perceive work environmentsduring their lives. We do not, of course, expect that one's affective dispositionwill dominateperceptionsto the extent thatwork in a grossly negative setting will be interpretedas a highly pleasantjob but, rather,that affective dispositionswill constitute a significant(andpreviouslyoverlooked)determinantof work attitudes. The present study examines the influenceof affective disposition on job attitudesover long periodsof time. Thisresearch 61/ASQ, March1986 takes advantageof a very unusualdata set, one that extends over much of the lifetimeof a groupof individualswho have been extensively tested at several points intime. We used these data to test whether the affective dispositionof individuals,fromas farbackas juniorand senior highschool years, can predictjob satisfactionin laterlife. METHOD Subjects Datafor this researchwere taken from the Intergenerational Studies (IGS)conducted by the Instituteof HumanDevelopment at the Universityof California,Berkeley.The IGSdataare an aggregationof three separate longitudinalstudies that have investigatedthe lives of selected individualsfor over fifty years. Begun inthe late 1920s and early1930s, the three studies each startedwith differentprincipalinvestigators,samples, and researchgoals, yet there was enough commonalityin measurement to allow IGSstaff subsequently to combine the samples intoa single data pool. The firstof the IGSwas the GuidanceStudy initiatedby Jean WalkerMacfarlanein 1928. Althoughthis projectwas originally plannedas a six-yearstudy of children'sproblembehavior,it was latercontinuedto researchthe interactionof psychological, social, and biologicalfactors in personalitydevelopment. The study's 248 originalparticipantswere drawnfrom a survey of every thirdbirthin Berkeleybetween January1, 1928 and June 30, 1929. Since one of the study's goals was to assess the impactof guidanceof parentsby professionalstaff, the participantswere also dividedintoguidanceand control groups. Extensivepersonalityassessments were taken for the guidance group;fewer data were gatheredforthe control subjects untilearlyadulthood. The BerkeleyGrowthStudy,the second IGS,was started by Nancy Bayleywith 61 infantsbornduring1928 and 1929. The initialsample was located by requestingpermissionof obstetricians to visit parentswith new babies and was laterextended to 74 subjects by including13 additionalbabies bornin the subsequent three years. Althoughthe originalpurposeof the BerkeleyGrowthStudywas to trace the intellectual,motor, and physicaldevelopment of infantsand young children,the projectwas extended over time with the inclusionof a wide rangeof psychologicaland social assessments. The thirdof the IGSwas the OaklandGrowthStudy. Begun in 1931 by HaroldE.Jones, MaryC. Jones, and HerbertR. Stolz, the OaklandGrowthStudywas designed as a study of normal adolescence. The projectoriginallyincluded212 subjects recruitedfromthe fifthand sixth grades of five elementary schools in northeasternOaklandand subsequently enrolledin a single juniorhighschool. The medianbirthmonth for this sample is May 1921, thereby makingthe OaklandGrowth Studysubjects an average of nearlyeight years olderthan subjects in the Guidanceand BerkeleyGrowthStudies. When initialmeasures were taken, members of the OaklandGrowth Studywere 10-12 years in age, butthe most extensive psychologicalassessments of them were made duringjunior and senior highschool years. 62/ASQ, March1986 Job Attitudes Demographically,the Oaklandand BerkeleyGrowthStudies were limitedto Caucasians,whereas the Guidanceand Control samples did includesome (English-speaking)minorities.About 40 percent of the OaklandGrowthStudysubjects came from working-classhomes, whereas about 33 percent of the participants of the Guidanceand BerkeleyGrowthStudies had working-classbackgrounds.Thisdifference is a reflectionof the demographiccharacteristicsof Oaklandand Berkeleyat the time of the studies. At adulthood,approximately90 percent of allthree samples could be characterizedas middle class. Overtime, some selective attritionoccurredin the studies, with working-classmen and lower IQparticipantsof both sexes havingslightlyhigherdrop-outrates (see Eichorn, 1981, for a more thoroughdescriptionof the samples in the IGS). Q-Sort Data and Methodology Because attritionis a majorproblemwith any longitudinal study, let alone one that spans several decades of people's lives, it was necessary for IGSresearchersto set up a procedurefor poolingdata across studies. Althoughallof the IGS investigatedpersonalitydevelopment, the particularmeasures used to assess individualsvariedacross the projects.An importantgoal, therefore,was to aggregate availabledata on a set of common dimensions across the three studies. Block,in collaborationwith Haan(1971),developed a Q-sort methodologyas a way of combiningdiverse data fromvarious assessments intoa set of common personalitycharacteristics. Althoughthe materialsavailablefrom the three studies were heterogenous (consistingof interviews,tests, and case records),there was enough informationon most cases (interms of breadthand depth)for a set of common Q-sortdimensions to be derived.Using the data archivesfor each subject, trained personalityraterswere asked to assess individualsusing a set of descriptivestatements such as "favorsconservativevalues in a varietyof areas," "initiateshumor,""irritable,"and "seeks reassurancefromothers." Thejudges placed allof the items in a forced normaldistributioninwhich lower categories (1-3) represented qualitiesor traitsleast characteristicof the person, middle-rangescores (4-6) represented moderatelycharacteristictraits,and higherscores (7-9) were most characteristic of the person being rated.The procedureresulted in an ipsativemeasure of the salience of variousdimensions of each individual'spersonalityratherthana normativeratingof individualsagainst age or sex peers. Q-sortswere done separatelyfor each of five time periods. Two adolescent-subjectsorts were conducted, one for ages 12-14 (earlyadolescence) and anotherfor ages 15-18 (late adolescence), based on a case assembly of the archivalmaterialavailablefor each study participant.A sort for the firstadult period(aboutage 30 for Guidancesubjects, 36 for Berkeley Growthsubjects, and 38 for participantsin the OaklandGrowth Study)was based on interviews rangingfromfourto six hours for BerkeleyGrowthsubjects to an average of 12 hoursover several days for members of the other studies-.Sorts for the second adultwave (when Guidancesubjects were about age 40 and OaklandGrowthsubjects about 48) were based on tape-recordedinterviewsaveragingfourhours in length. Due 63/ASQ, March1986 to fundinglimitations,BerkeleyGrowthsubjects were not interviewedfor the second adultwave. Sorts for the thirdadult period(aboutage 54 for Guidanceand BerkeleyGrowthsubjects and 62 for OaklandGrowthparticipants)were also based on tape-recordedinterviewslastingabout two hours. At least three judges ratedeach adolescent and at least two ratedeach adultfor the varioustime periods. Combinationsof judges were systematicallyvariedso that no judge ratedthe same subject at more than one time period,nordidjudges have access to informationfromtime periodsother than the one they were rating.The judges were highlyqualified,consisting of clinicalpsychologists and psychiatricsocial workers, allwith substantialclinicaland/orresearchexperience. Thorough traininginthe Q-sortmethod was also provided,and judges' performanceduringthe ratingswas closely monitored. Judges were given feedback if they appearedto be using any of the Q-items ina nondifferentiatingmanner(e.g., by consistently placingan item highin allsubjects' profiles),and several tests were performedto provideassurance against the use of stereotypic ratings.Nondifferentiatingplacement of items and stereotypingdid not appearto be problemsin the Q-sorts,and extra raterswere used if the Q-sortdata were not sufficiently reliableacross the multiplejudges. The mean reliabilitiesof the Q-sortsat the varioustime periodsrangedfrom .72 to .78. Additionalreliabilitydata as well as more extensive explanation of the Q-sortproceduresused in the IGScan be found in Block (with Haan,1971) and in Eichornet al. (1981). Sample Size Althoughthe Q-sortprocedureallowed the IGSresearchersto combine three longitudinalstudies, sample sizes are still modest forthe present investigation.The firstlimitingfactoris that female subjects of the IGS(halfof the three samples) hadvery limitedwork experience and thus hadto be excluded from the present analyses. The second problemis that relatively few IGSparticipantswere assessed for allof the five time periods. Earlyand late adolescent datawere not availablefor subjects who were originallyinthe controlgroupof the Guidance Study,since there were insufficientmaterialsfor 0-sorts to be performedfor these cases. BerkeleyGrowthsubjects were not assessed in the second adultwave. Finally,although extensive efforts were made not to lose participantsover any of the data collectionwaves (whichsometimes involvedinterviewers travelinglong distances to assess those unableto returnto Berkeley)data for manyindividualsare missing for at least one of the time periods. Fora study participantto be includedin every wave of ouranalyses, sufficientdatawould be necessary forfive personalityQ-sorts,rangingover a period of 40 to 50 years, as well as two employmentquestionnaires and an employment interviewadministeredover the last 12 years of the study. Because of these sample limitations,we present analyses using both the largestavailabledata set as well as a smallerset of complete longitudinaldata. Measurement of Affective Disposition Eighty-threeQ-sortitems were used consistently in the IGS across the five waves of data collection. Fromthese 83 personalitydescriptionswe isolatedallthose statements that 64/ASQ, March1986 Job Attitudes appearedto capturesome aspect of affect on a prioritheoretical grounds. Some of these Q-sortstatements pertainedto intrapersonalaspects of affect, such as "cheerful,""feels lack of personalmeaning"(reverse scored), and "irritable"(reverse scored). Otheritems pertainedto interpersonalaspects of affect, such as "behaves in a givingway," "has hostility towardothers" (reversescored), and "feels victimized"(reverse scored). Factoranalyses showed that 17 items pertaining to both intra-and interpersonalaffect formeda single affective factor,in a similarmanneras would be predictedby Watson and Clark(1984). The specific affective items and theirfactorloadingsare shown inTable 1, alongwith the sample size on which each of the factoranalyses was based. Also shown in the table are the squared multiplecorrelationsof the variableswith the factors, eigenvalues, Kaiser'smeasure of samplingadequacy (MSA), and Cronbach'salphameasure of scale reliability.As illustrated in the table, factorloadingswere ratherconsistent across the variouswaves of data collection,averaging.65 across the five time periods.The averagefactor loadingrangedfroma highof .71 forthe late-adolescence periodto a low of .62 for the Adult 1 wave. Althoughit would have been preferableto have had a largersample on which to base the factoranalyses, Kaiser's measure of samplingadequacy indicatedthatthe datawere adequate for the factoringtechnique (Kimand Mueller,1978). Since reliabilities(Cronbach'salpha)rangedfrom .90 to .93 and the squaredmultiplecorrelationsof the items with the factor never fell below .95, we can conclude that the 17-itemaffective measure was a reliableindicatorthat retainedits factor structureover time. Table1 Items and Factor Loadings for The Affective Disposition Scale Item Giving Thin-skinned Sympathetic Punitive Condescending Likeable Warm Hostile Distrustful Botheredby demands Irritable Negative Self-defeating Satisfiedwith self Feels victimized Moody Cheerful Squaredmultiple correlationof variableswith factor Eigenvalue Kaiser'sMSA N Cronbach'salpha Early adolescence Time Period Late Adult 1 adolescence Adult 2 Adult 3 .79 -.39 .78 -.61 -.49 .81 .75 -.81 - .74 -.70 -.67 -.76 -.65 .49 -.74 -.46 .64 .76 -.53 .80 -.66 -.53 .83 .70 -.77 -.76 -.70 -.80 -.75 -.70 .55 -.76 -.69 .75 .73 -.62 .83 -.65 -.35 .68 .70 -.65 -.72 -.49 -.67 -.69 -.52 .36 -.66 -.55 .68 .68 -.52 .72 -.57 -.35 .74 .65 -.79 -.67 -.69 -.54 -.67 -.69 .37 -.71 -.48 .64 .78 -.62 .84 -.78 -.63 .78 .85 -.88 -.83 -.62 -.53 -.80 -.58 .26 -.66 -.07 .75 .96 7.78 .88 101 .92 .97 8.64 .89 94 .93 .95 6.81 .87 130 .91 .95 6.72 .87 104 .90 .97 8.21 .92 128 .92 65/ASQ, March1986 As expected, the temporalstabilityof affective disposition depended on the time periodseparatingdata collectionwaves. Stabilitywas highest (averaging.48) for the temporallyadjacent periods (e.g., Adult1 and 2 waves) and somewhat lower for data collections separated by two time periods(averaging .31) and for measurements dividedby three time periods (averaging.29). These temporalrelationshipsare in linewith previouslongitudinalresearchon individualaffect reviewed by Watson and Clark(1984), as well as with relatedpersonality scales (e.g., the nurturant/hostile dimension)developed in previousresearchusing the IGSsamples (Haan,1981). Measurement of Job Attitudes IGSmeasures of job attitudeswere availablefroma questionnairethat was administeredto study participantsduringthe Adult2 wave of data collectionand fromboth a questionnaire and structuredinterviewthatwere partof the Adult3 wave of assessments. Because the questionnairemeasures of job attitudeswere self-reportsthey were independentof the Q-sortassessment of personality.The interviewmeasures were also largelyindependentof the Q-sortdata, since the staff that conductedthe interviewsdid not have access to any of the Q-sortassessments, nordid ratersforthe Q-sorts have access to any specific interviewresponses. Thus, one strength of this study is an independence of measurement, avoiding problemsof common-methodbias that so frequentlyplague researchon job attitudes (cf. Robertsand Glick,1981). The IGSmeasures of job attitudefor the Adult2 time period consisted of survey questions on bothjob and careersatisfaction. One measure was a single item assessing careersatisfaction with five response alternatives,rangingfrom "it is a career Idislikeand wish Icould leave for another"to "ittruly represents what Ihave wanted to do." A second indicatorof job attitudeswas a 14-itemfacet satisfactionmeasure inwhich subjects were asked to ratevariousaspects of theirwork on a five-pointscale, rangingfrom "likeit very much"to "dislikeit very much."Thejob facets includedin the measure were workinghours,amountof tension and stress, income, degree to which the work involves interests, generalconvenience for the family,amountof leisuretime, opportunityfor advancement, natureof supervision,meeting and being with people, use of skillsand abilities,supervisionof others, respect that others give to the job, freedom to develop ideas, and security of the job. These items were aggregated intoa facet satisfaction measure. Because facet satisfactionand careersatisfaction were strongly intercorrelated (r = .51, N = 75, p<.001) they were also combined intoan overalljob-attitudeindex. Forthe Adult3 time periodthere were five measures of job attitudes availablein the IGSdata.The Michiganfacet-specific measure of job satisfaction(Quinnand Staines, 1979) was administered,in which subjects respondedto 18 positive statements abouttheirjobs, such as "the job securityis good," "the chances for promotionare good," and "Ihave an opportunityto develop my own special abilities."Responses to these items were aggregated intoa facet job-satisfaction measure. Inadditionto this facet satisfactionmeasure the IGS data also includedseveral single-itemscales relatingto job attitudes.The Michiganfacet-freejob-satisfactionitem was 66/ASQ, March1986 Job Attitudes included,which simplyasked, "allinall, how satisfied would you say you are with yourcurrentjob?"Therewas also a career-satisfactionitem with slightlydifferentwordingfrom that administeredduringthe Adult2 wave, and a related questionnaireitem inquiredwhether one would take the job again,with three alternatives,rangingfrom "definitelywould take the same job"to "Idefinitelywould not take the same job." Finally,the structuredinterviewsconducted at the Adult 3 time periodincludeda question about how one felt about going to work,with alternativescoded from "strongpositive responses" to "negativefeelings." When these five measures of job attitudeswere subjected to a factoranalysis,they revealeda single factorwith loadingsrangingfrom .50 to .78. Factorscores were used as an overallmeasure of job attitudes for the Adult3 time period. RESULTS The majordata analyses to be reportedby this study are correlationsbetween affective dispositionandjob attitudes.The firstanalyses displayedare those based on the maximumsamples availablefor each pointintime. The samples differfor each of the waves of data collection,since comparativelyfew of the subjects were assessed at allfive time periodson both affect and job attitudes. Table2 shows the correlatesof Adult2 job attitudes.The smallest sample availableforthe correlationsconsisted of 51 men who were assessed on affect duringlate adolescence and also forAdult2 job attitudes.The largestsample was 76 men who were assessed on both affect and job attitudes during the Adult2 time period.As shown in the table, the results revealedseveral significantrelationshipsas well as a consistent trend.As mightbe expected, the correlationswere progressivelystrongeras the time between measurements decreased. The strongest relationshipresultedfromthe concurrentmeasurement of affect and job attitudes, both assessed at the Adult2 time period.The weakest relationship, though still marginallysignificant,was the predictionof Adult2 job attitudesfromaffect duringearlyadolescence. Table 2 Relationships between Affective Disposition and Adult Job Attitudes Job Attitudes (Adult 2) Affective Disposition Early adolescence Late adolescence Adult 1 Adult 2 p<. 10, p< 05, *p<.01; 67/AS0, March1986 Facet job satisfaction .14 (N = 59) .16 Career satisfaction .19 (N = 58) .270 Overall job attitude (average score) .20 (N = 59) .260 (N = 52) (N = 51) (N = 52) .310 (N= 70) .35000 (N= 76) .230 (N= 69) *35000 (N= 75) (N= 70) .40000 (N = 76) one-tailed tests. .30000 Table3 shows the relationshipbetween affective disposition and job attitudesfromthe Adult3 time period.The sample sizes for the variousanalyses rangedfrom 51 for the adolescent years to 101 for the Adult3 time period.As shown inthe table, the affective dispositionscores from earlyand late adolescence were generallyrelatedto the dependent measures. The facet job satisfaction,generaljob satisfaction,and career satisfactionmeasures were allsignificantlypredictedby affect fromthe adolescent years. The interviewitem measuring workfeelings and the questionnaireitem on whether one would take the job againdid not show significantrelationships with adolescent affect. However,the overalljob attitudemeasure (usingfactorscores) was significantlypredictedby affect from both earlyand late adolescence. Affective dispositionsfromAdult1 were stronglyrelatedto job attitudes at the Adult3 time period.Allof the dependent measures at Adult3 were significantlypredictedby Adult1 affect, and the overalljob attitudemeasure was stronglyrelatedto affect fromthe Adult1 period. Table3 Relationships between Affective Disposition and Adult 3 Job Attitudes Job Attitudes (Adult3) Affective Disposition Early adolescence Late Career satisfaction .20 Facet-free satisfaction Facet satisfaction .20 (N = 51) *35 (N = 51) Take job again .240 .12 (N = 51) (N = 51) .21*35g .09 Overalljob Feelings attitude about work (factorscore) .11 (N = 58) .15 370 (N = 46) 39g adolescence (N = 45) (N = 45) (N = 45) (N = 45) (N = 52) (N = 40) Adult1 *45 (N = 72) .19 (N = 68) .25g (N = 90) *370 (N = 72) .00 (N = 68) .200 (N = 90) 260 (N = 72) .04 (N = 68) .230 (N = 90) 330 (N = 72) .13 (N = 68) .19 (N = 90) .200 (N = 81) .08 (N = 75) .10 (N = 101) 48 (N = 67) .12 (N = 63) .230 (N = 81) Adult2 Adult3 *p<.10, *p<.05, *p<.01; one-tailedtests. Of allthe relationshipsshown inTable3, the correlationsbetween Adult2 affect and Adult3 job attitudeswere the weakest. Onlyone dependent measure was even marginally relatedto Adult2 affect, and the overalljob attitudemeasure did not show a significantcorrelation.The concurrentrelationships between Adult3 affect and Adult3 job attitudes were somewhat stronger.These correlationswere modest in magnitudebut statisticallysignificantdue to the largersample sizes associated with the data. Panel Data One of the interestingfeatures of the results inTable3 is the appearancethat earlymeasures of affect are strongerpredictors of Adult3 job attitudesthan lateraffective measures. However, one cannot be sure of such a trendfromthe array shown in the table. Because differentsubjects were used to test each relationship(withmissing datafor manyof the waves), any comparisonof correlationsinvolvesdifferentindi68/ASQ, March 1986 Job Attitudes vidualssampled. Therefore,the correlationsreportedinTables 2 and 3 should be interpretedas separate tests of the hypothesis thatjob attitudes can be predictedfromaffective disposition ratherthan an answer to the question of whether affect measured from one life stage is a strongerpredictorthanthat fromanotherperiod. Table4 presents data froma constant sample across the five time periods, using the overalljob attitudemeasures as the primarydependent variable.Results were consistent with the data presented inTables 2 and 3. Adult2 job attitudeswere shown to be most stronglypredictedby Adult2 affect, with adolescent affect the weakest predictorof Adult2 attitudes. In contrast,Adult3 job attitudeswere more stronglypredictedby adolescent and Adult1 affect than by affect measured during the Adult2 and 3 waves. Thus,the datawere inconclusivein establishingwhether earlyor late affective measures are the strongest predictorsof subsequent job attitudes; overall,they appearapproximatelyequal in strength. Table4 Relationships between Affective Disposition and Job Attitudes During Adult 2 and Adult 3 Time Periods Using a Constant Sample Job Attitudes Adult 2 Adult 3 overall job overall job attitude attitude Affective Disposition Earlyadolescence Lateadolescence Adult1 Adult2 Adult 3 (N = 52) .270 .260 .310 .380 - (N = 31) .34a .360 .480 .01 .24 *p<. 10, *p<.05, 00p<.01; one-tailedtests. DISCUSSION The data fromTables 2 and 3 show distinctevidence that affective dispositionis a significantpredictorof job satisfaction. The correlationsare not of such magnitudeas to determine more thana minorityof the variance,but they are at least as strong as those routinelyreportedfromcross-sectional studies using a single questionnaire.This is by itself surprising, given that the data reportedhere span almost fiftyyears in time, with nearlyall of the data collections at least ten years apart. The second surprisingfindingfromthe relationshipsreported in Tables 2 and 3 is the lackof a consistent patternof decay. Onlythe antecedents of Adult2 attitudes showed the normally expected patterninwhich earlymeasures of affect would be weaker than subsequent indicatorsin predictingjob satisfaction. We do not have any firmexplanationfor these results except to note that they are not unusualfor research using the IntergenerationalStudies. Clausen(1968),for example, found that adolescent personalityvariableswere strong predictorsof some forms of adultbehavior(e.g., smoking),with at least as 69/ASO, March 1986 much explanatorypower as personalitytraitsmeasured from adultyears. Ininterpretingsuch a patternit is possible to argue that adolescent dispositionsset in motiona set of behaviorsor choices that laterproduceimportantconsequences for the individual.Itis also possible that adolescent assessments are somehow more validindicatorsof individualdispositionthan data taken from latertime periods, perhapsbecause adolescent behaviorsare less disguised or socializedintoacceptable forms thanthose of adults. Finally,because the adolescent and Adult1 personalityassessments were based on a greater amount of personalitydata (inthe case files) than the Adult2 and 3 measurements, they may be more representativeof the affective dispositionof individuals. Sources of Affective Disposition Ourresults showed that it is possible to constructinternally consistent measures of affective disposition,even when these measures are constructed post hoc from priorpsychological assessments. LikeWatson and Clark(1984),we found a range of personaland interpersonaldimensions forminga coherent set of individualcharacteristicsthat retainedits factorstructure over time and that possessed some temporalstability(being statisticallysignificantfor up to 40-year intervals). Evidenceof stabilityin affect does not mean that this disposition is unchangeableor not subject to contextualinfluence. Itis unclearfromourdata how the affect of individualsoriginated, from either genetic or social sources, and how it may be influenced by externalfactors over one's lifetime. Itis also unclear whether the constructof affect remainsa constant entity over time. When assessing personalityin a longitudinalstudy one cannot, as noted by Haan(1981), assume that scores for a personalityvariableare necessarily equivalentfor childhood and adultyears. Since the Q-sortswere based on disparate sets of interviewdata, it is possible that what was interpreted by judges as affect fromthe adultyears is not a simple extension of affect fromadolescence. Therefore,it may be useful to interpretthe present results as showing relationshipsbetween job attitudesand separate (thoughrelated)affective constructs. Of course, even with such a restrictedinference, it is still possible to conclude fromthe IGSdatathat affective measures, fromas farbackas adolescence, are useful predictorsof subsequent work attitudes. While it is possible to speculate about alternativeinterpretations of these results, it is importantto realizethat few organizationalstudies have presented longitudinaldata beyond several years in duration(Bray,Campbell,and Grant,1974 is a rareexception). It is also importantto note that the measurement of affect for each life stage was, inthis study, assessed by a differentraterand that satisfactionwas measured on entirelydifferentinstrumentsfrom any of the affective measures. Too often organizationalresearchstems from measurements taken from single self-reportinstruments,thereby inflating the magnitudeof results, as in the correlationof job and life satisfactionor the common practiceof relatingperceivedtask characteristicsto job attitudes (cf. Robertsand Glick,1981). Thus, the present findingscan be viewed as an extremely conservativetest of the dispositionalapproachto job attitudes. 70/ASO, March1986 Job Attitudes Cognitive Reevaluation of Jobs Intesting the hypothesis that affective dispositionpredictsjob attitudes, it has been assumed that cognitivedistortionmay underliethe consistency between affect and job attitudes. It should be stressed, however, that this is an hypothesizedexplanationof the data, since we have no directinformationon perceptualprocesses. A plausiblealternative,for example, is that individualswith positive dispositions have sought out job environments(orhave been selected intojobs) that are objectively more pleasantthanthe employment situationof those with more negative dispositions.Thisalternativecan be eliminated by controllingfor objective differences injob conditions. Althoughthe only measure of objectivejob qualityavailablefor both the Adult2 and Adult3 waves of IGSdata is the socioeconomic status (SES)of the individuals'job, SES should be at least a roughproxyfor the overalllevel of pay, responsibility, and the approvalof others. Therefore,we computed partial correlationsbetween affect at the variouspoints in individuals' lives with subsequent job attitudes, holdingSES constant. The results of these analyses showed no materialchanges from the data reportedinTables 2 and 3. Forthe Adult3 periodonly, some additionalcontrolmeasures were available.Recently, IGSstaff completed a coding of the substantive complexityof participants'jobs in terms of dealing with data, people, and things. The substantive complexity scales were taken fromthe Dictionaryof OccupationalTitles and probablyprovidesomewhat better measures of objective job qualitythan SES. Therefore,we againcomputed partial correlationsbetween affect at the varioustime periodswith job attitudesduringthe Adult3 period,holdingthe three aspects of substantivecomplexityconstant. The results once again showed no significantdeparturefromthose of Tables2 and 3, suggesting that affect may influenceindividuals'responses to jobs, regardlessof at least some dimensions of task content. Itwas not possible with the IGSdata to sort out allthe possible processes underlyingthe relationshipsbetween affect and job satisfaction,since this would have taken both a largersample and measures thatwere unavailablefor this research. Norwas it possible with the IGSdata to answer questions about whether affective dispositionis a functionof heredityor environment.Affect could resultfrom inheritedbiologicalfactors, and these inheriteddispositionscould inturnbe associated with other mentalor emotionaltraitsunderlyinglaterachievement inoccupationalsettings. Alternatively,affect could be a productof earlysocializationexperiences that conditionor are correlatedwith futurejob opportunitiesor the likelihoodof success. The present data cannot (andwere not intendedto) untangleallthe possible relationshipsbetween job conditions and attitudes northe web of hereditaryand environmental influences. We have instead sought to show that dispositional propertiesof individuals,however constituted or developed overtime, can be significantpredictorsof the satisfactionof individualswith theirwork. Fortunately,we can have some confidence inthis limitedinference.The IGSvariableswere much more independentlymeasured and showed relationships over a much longertime span than is usuallythe case 71/ASO, March1986 in organizationalresearch.Ourconfidence in a dispositional explanationof job attitudes is also strengthened by the fact that adultjob satisfactionwas significantlypredictedby affective dispositions froman age before formalemployment had begun (i.e., earlyand late adolescence). These relationships are difficultto explainby any presumed effects of job experience on individuals'affective disposition. Theoretical Implications Inevaluatingthe theoreticalimplicationsof this study, one should keep in mindthat there are several potentiallevels of dispositionalexplanation.The most fundamentalbut also most primitiveis knowledge that attitudes have some source of consistency over time and context, since this simplyprovidesa clue that dispositionalcharacteristicsare a source of influence. At a second level of explanationare reportedrelationshipsbetween personalityvariablesandjob attitudes that invokeuntested or unspecifiedtheoreticallinkages.Here,the causal mechanisms underlyingsuch relationshipsmust be inferred froma wide rangeof theoreticalpossibilities(e.g., those with high-achievementneeds being less satisfied because they might possibly have higheraspirations).A somewhat higheror thirdlevel of dispositionalexplanationwould be one where there is theoreticalcorrespondence between an individual characteristicandan attitudinalor behavioralconstruct.When, for example, power needs correlatewith promotionor, as in this study, affective dispositionrelates to job satisfaction,the theoreticallinkagebetween variablesis more specified or obvious thanwith the almost arbitraryuse of personalityconstructs in much of the organizationalliterature(cf. Weiss and Adler,1984). Studies using time lags, such as when early needs for power or affective dispositionrelateto subsequent and correspondentdependent variables,are also helpfulin buildinga convincingdispositionalinference, since they show both causal directionand potency. A fourthlevel of explanation, and one not providedby this study, involves knowledge about the exact derivationof dispositionsand aboutthe precise mechanisms by which dispositions can affect attitudes or behavior.Inthis researchwe hypothesizedthat affective dispositions may constitute internalcues that influencethe way people perceive or reactto job environments.However, we had no data on the cognitiveprocesses of the IGSsample, nor couldwe estimate the inheritedvs. social derivationof affective dispositions.These difficultand detailedquestions must therefore await furtherresearchthat logicallyfollows from earlierlevels of dispositionalunderstanding. Althoughwe do not now fullyunderstanddispositionalsources of job attitudes,the generalfindingthatjob attitudes can be predictedby affective dispositionstill has some important implications.Interms of job attitudetheory, it means that we have probablyerredtoo faron the side of situationaldeterminism, throwingaway valuabledispositionalpredictorsin our general disillusionmentwith Maslow's need theory. Italso means that, in orderto understandthe originand change of job attitudes, it may be useful to understandthe backgroundand development of the individualsunderstudy. As Weiss and Adler(1984) so aptlynoted, we need studies focused on dispositionalvariables,perhapsusing situationalvariablesas 72/ASO, March1986 Job Attitudes moderatorsof dispositionalinfluence,ratherthan additional studies using individualcharacteristicsas simple moderators of situationaleffects. Forexample, if we were to explainjob attitudes in dispositionalterms, we would want to identify those contextualfeatures (e.g., workingconditions,supervision, etc.) that eitherallow people with positive affect to enjoy theirwork or preventindividualswith negative affect from unnecessarilyrejectingtheirjobs. This is a very different researchdirectionthanwe have now and one that deserves furtherconsideration. The argumentthat job satisfactionhas dispositionalcauses also suggests some necessary revisionof our models of job attitudes. One possibilityis that objectivefeatures of the job, social informationaboutthe work, and affective dispositionare three separate or independentdeterminantsof job satisfaction. Anotherpossibilityis that these three sources of job attitudes are highlyinteractive.Futureresearchmay find,for example, that the perceptionof objectivejob characteristicsis conditionedby attitudinalcues from both internal(dispositional)and external(social)sources. Alternatively,we may find that internaland externalcues are moderatedby the realitiesof work life, such that neithera positive dispositionnorshared enthusiasm for the job can influencework satisfactionwithout the presence of objectivelypositive task conditions(so as to validateperceived cues fromoneself and others). Additional research is needed to sort out whether such interactional models are better explanationsof job satisfactionthan a simple main-effects model. Sortingout the interactionof dispositionalandjob characteristics is likelyto be a complex researchtask, as alreadyillustratedby some recent studies in occupationalsociology. For example, researchby Kallebergand Loscocco (1983)and Mortimer, Finch,and Maruyama(1985) suggests that not only does job satisfaction increase with aging, but that the facets of the jobthat most directlydetermine satisfactionalso change overtime. Thus,we mighteventuallyneed to formulatecareer and life-stage models of dispositionaland task influences in lieu of simple personalityor situationalpredictions.Itis also difficultto escape fromthe fact that personalitynot only affects the choice of job situation,but that the job situationcan also have some lastingeffects on the individual,thus creating reciprocalcausal loops that are difficultto understandand study (e.g., Kohnand Schooler, 1978). These and related questions should be idealfor a mergingof the psychological and sociologicaltraditionsof organizationalbehavior. Practical Implications Interms of practicalissues, the most obvious implicationof our findingsconcerns the usefulness of organizationaldevelopment activities.The stabilityof job attitudes identifiedby Staw and Ross (1985) and the present findingthat job attitudesare influencedby affective dispositionboth implythat it may be extremely difficultto improvejob attitudesvia externalinterventions.Job redesign efforts may, for example, be prone to failure,since they must contend with strong forces for stability-forces that either make individualsresistantto change or promptindividualsto returnto their baseline states over time. 73/ASQ, March1986 Interpretingour results in a conservativeway, one mightinfer that job redesign is seldom worth the effort. The objective features of tasks are seldom concrete enough to dictate a positive or negative response, and as a consequence, individualsmay generallyinterpretthe workenvironmentinways that are consistent with theirown psychologicalstates. A more liberalinterpretation,and one that is just as defensible, is that organizationalinterventionsneed to be strongerand more pervasive.Situationalchanges, be they job redesign, improvements in physicalsurroundings,or the governance of the organization,may need to be strong andwidespread enough to overwhelm dispositionalforces for stability. A compromise positionon the organizationaldevelopment issue would be the interactionistperspective (Terborg,1981; Schneider, 1983). Interactionistsmightconclude that interventions such as job redesign need to take individualdifferences more fullyintoconsideration.Throughbetter or more extensive measurement of individualdispositions,job assignments could be more preciselytailoredto individualcharacteristics. Alternatively,it mightbe possible to divideindividualson the basis of theirlikelihoodfor change. As Bem and Allen(1974) have noted, some people may simplybe more consistent in theirbehaviorthan others. Thus, situationalchanges such as job redesign may be best suited, ratherthan forthe chronically happyor unhappy,forthose whose affective dispositionvaries over time and across situations. Ourdata on the influenceof affective dispositionmay also have direct implicationsfor personnel selection practices. Ifjob attitudes are ratherstable and governed by individualdispositions, it may be easier to improveorganizationalmorale throughemployee selection thanthroughorganizationaldevelopment activities. Intheirsimplest form,such personnel implicationsmay not necessitate changes in practice,since personalcharacteristicsare probablyalreadybehindmany selection and promotiondecisions in organizations(e.g., Ross and Ferris,1981). The common argumentagainst using personal factors such as likabilityand positiveaffect for evaluation purposes is that they would not be performancerelated. This argumentmay not holdin service occupations, however, since one of the majorproductsof a service relationshipcan be the conveyance of positiveaffect (e.g., Schneider, Parkington, and Buxton, 1980; Smith, Organ,and Near, 1983). Ifwe go beyond the simplest personnel issues, our dispositionalfindingshave additionaland perhapsmore important implications.When we consider, for example, the research findingsthat depressives are more realisticintheirjudgments of riskand causationthan others (e.g., Alloyand Abramson, 1979), we are led to the conclusionthat positive affect could hinderperformancein some managerialand staff roles. As a result,we may need to examine each organizationalrolefor the amountof enthusiasm versus criticalthinkinginvolvedin orderbest to fit jobs with individuals'affective dispositions. Likewise,if we note that workgroups (andorganizations)often requirea varietyof individualsto serve multipleroles, ranging from being a source of social supportto being a devil's advocate, it may be importantto search for the propermix of personalitiesin the organization.These are the kindsof ques74/ASQ0,March1986 Job Attitudes tions that are raisedas one begins to probethe role of individualdispositionin organizationalbehavior.As we pursue the dispositionalperspective further,new theoryas well as revisionsto managerialpracticeare likelyto be suggested. REFERENCES Alloy, Lauren B., and Lynn Y. Abramson 1979 "Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?" Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108: 441-485. Aries, Elizabeth J., Conrad Gold, and Russell H. Weigel 1983 "Disposition and situational influences on dominance behavior in small groups." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44: 779-786. Beck, Aaron T. 1967 Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Hoeber. Berm, Daryl J., and Andrea Allen 1974 "On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior." Psychological Review, 81: 506-520. Block, J. 1977 "Advancing the science of personality: Paradigmatic shift or improving the quality of research?" In David Magnusson and Norman Endler (eds.), Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology: 37-63. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Block, Jack, in collaboration with Norma Haan 1971 Lives through Time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft Books. Bouchard, Thomas J. 1984 "Twins reared together and apart: What they tell us about human diversity." In SidneyW. Fox (ed.), Individualityand Determ'inism, New York: 147179. Plenum. 1985 Personal communication with the authors. Bray, Douglas W., Richard J. Campbell, and Donald L. Grant 1974 Formative Years in Business. New York: Wiley. Buss, Arnold H., and Robert Plomin 1984 Temperament: EarlyDeveloping PersonalityTraits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Buss, David M., and Kenneth H. Craik 1983 "The act-frequency approach to personality." Psychological Review, 90: 105-126. 1985 "Why not measure that trait? Alternative criteria for identifying important dispositions." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48: 934-946. Haan, Norma 1981 "Common dimensions of personality development: Early adolescence to middle life." In Dorothy H. Eichorn, John A. Clausen, Norma Haan, Marjorie P. Honzik, and Paul H. Mussen (eds.), Present and Past in Middle Life: 117-153. New York: Academic Press. Clausen, John A. 1968 "Adolescent antecedents of cigarette smoking: Data from the Oakland Growth Study." Social Science and Medicine, 1: 357-382. Hackman, J. Richard, and Greg R. Oldham 1975 "Development of the Job Diagnostic Survey." Journal of Applied Psychology, 60: 159170. 1976 "Motivation through the design of work: Test of a theory." Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16: 250279. 1980 Work Redesign. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Deci, Edward L. 1975 Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum Press. Eichorn, Dorothy H. 1981 "Samples and procedures." In Dorothy H. Eichorn, John A. Clausen, Norma Haan, Marjorie P. Honzik, and Paul H. Mussen (eds.), Present and Past in Middle Life: 33-54. New York: Academic Press. Eichorn, Dorothy H., and John A. Clausen, Norma Haan, Marjorie P. Honzik, and Paul H. Mussen (eds.) 1981 Present and Past in Middle Life. New York: Academic Press. Fisher, V. E., and Joseph V. Hanna 1931 The Dissatisfied Worker. New York: MacMillan. Funder, David C., and Daniel Ozer 1983 "Behavior as a function of the situation." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44: 107-112. Gechman, Arthur S., and Yoash Wiener 1975 "Job involvement and satisfaction as related to mental health and personal time devoted to work." Journal of Applied Psychology, 60: 521-523. Goodman, Paul S. 1979 Assessing Organization Change: The Rushton Quality of Work Experiment. New York: Wiley-lnterscience. Griffin, Ricky A. 1983 "Objective and social sources of information in task redesign: A field experiment." Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 184-200. 75/ASQ, March 1986 Hackman, J. Richard, Greg R. Oldham, Robert Janson, and Kenneth Purdy 1975 "A new strategy for job enrichment." CaliforniaManagement Review, Summer: 57-71. Herzberg, Frederick 1966 Work and the Nature of Man. Cleveland: World. Herzberg, Frederick, Bernard Mausner, Richard 0. Peterson, and Dora F. Capwell 1957 Job Attitudes: Review of Research and Opinion. Pittsburgh: Psychological Service of Pittsburgh. Hoppock, Robert 1935 Job Satisfaction. New York: Harper. Kahn, Robert L. 1981 Work and Health. New York: Wiley. Kalleberg, Arne L., and Karyn A. Loscocco 1983 "Aging, values, and rewards: Explaining age differences in job satisfaction." American Sociological Review, 48: 7890. Kim, Jae-On, and Charles W. Mueller 1978 FactorAnalysis: Statistical Methods and Practical Issues. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Kohn, Melvin L., and Carmi School er 1978 "The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment." American Journal of Sociology, 84: 24-52. Kornhauser, Arthur W. 1965 Mental Health of the Industrial Worker: A Detroit Study. New York: Wiley. Landy, FrankJ. 1978 "An opponent process theory of job satisfaction." Journal of Applied Psychology, 63: 533547. Lawler, Edward E., IlIl 1982 "Increasing worker involvement to enhance organizational effectiveness." In Paul Goodman (ed.), Change in Organizations: 280-31 5. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Lepper, Mark R., David Greene, and Richard E. Nisbett 1973 "Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the 'overjustification hypothesis'." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28: 129-1 37. Locke, Edwin A. 1976 "The nature and causes of job satisfaction." In Marvin D. Dunnette (ed.), Handbook of Industrialand Organizational Psychology: 1297-1 349. Chicago: Rand McNally. Maslow, Abraham H. 1954 Motivation and Personality. New York: Haper& Row. McGowan, John, and John Gormly 1976 "Validationof personality traits: A multicriteria approach." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34: 791-795. Mischel, Walter 1968 Personality and Assessment. New York: Wiley. Monson, Thomas C., John W. Hesley, and Linda Chernick 1982 "Specifying when personality traits can and cannot predict behavior: An alternative to abandoning the attempt to predict single act criteria." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43: 385-399. Mortimer, Jeylan T., Michael D. Finch, and Geoffrey Maruyama 1985 "Work experience and job satisfaction: Variationby age and gender." Paper presented, the Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Los Angeles. Munsterberg, Hugo 1913 Psychology and IndustrialEfficiency. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Near, Janet R., Robert W. Rice, and Raymond G. Hunt 1978 "Work and extra-work correlates of life and job satisfaction." Academy of Management Journal, 21: 248-264. Oldham, Greg R., and J. Richard Hackman 1980 "Work design in the organizational context." In BarryM. Staw and LarryL. Cummings (eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior, 2: 247-278. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. O'Reilly, Charles A., and David F. Caldwell 1979 "Informational influence asa determinant of perceived task characteristics and job satisfaction." Journal of Applied Psychology, 64: 157-1 65. O'Reilly, Charles A., G. Nicholas Parlette, and Joan R. Bloom 1980 "Perceptual measures of task characteristics: The biasing effects of differing frames of reference and job attitudes." Academy of Management Journal, 23: 118-131. Orpen, Christopher 1978 "Work and nonwork satisfaction: A causal-correlational analysis." Journal of Applied Psychology, 63: 530-532. Plomin, Robert, J. C. DeFries, and Gerald E. McClearn 1980 Behavioral Genetics: A Primer. San Francisco: Freeman. Pulakos, Elaine D., and Neal Schmitt 1983 "A longitudinal study of a valence model approach for the prediction of job satisfaction of new employees." Journal of Applied Psychology, 68: 307312. Quinn, Robert P., and Graham L. Staines 1979 The 1977 Quality of Employment Survey. Ann Arbor, Ml: Institute for Social Research. 76/ASQ, March 1986 Quinn, Robert P., Graham L. Staines, and Margaret R. McCollough 1974 "Job satisfaction: Is there a trend?" Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Labor. Roberts, Karlene H., and William Glick 1981 "The job characteristics approach to task design: A critical review." Journal of Applied Psychology, 66: 193-217. Ross, Jerry, and Kenneth R. Ferris 1981 "Interpersonal attraction and organizational outcomes: A field examination." Administrative Science Quarterly, 26: 61 7-632. Salancik, Gerald R., and Jeffrey Pfeffer 1977 "An examination of need satisfaction models of job attitudes." Administrative Science Quarterly, 22: 427-456. 1978 "A social information processing approach to job attitudes and task design." Administrative Science Quarterly, 23: 224-253. Schmitt, Neal, and Elaine D. Pulakos 1986 "Predicting job satisfaction from life satisfaction: Is there a general satisfaction factor?" International Review of Applied Psychology (in press). Schneider, Benjamin 1983 "Interactional psychology and organizational behavior." In L. L. Cummings and BarryM. Staw (eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior, 5: 1-31. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Schneider, Benjamin, and H. Peter Dach ler 1978 "A note on the stability of the job description index." Journal of Applied Psychology, 63: 650-653. Schneider, Benjamin, John J. Parkington, and Virginia M. Buxton 1980 "Employee and customer perceptions of service in banks." Administrative Science Quarterly, 25: 252-267. Sims, Henry P., Andrew D. Szilagyi, and Robert T. Keller 1976 "The measurement of job characteristics." Academy of Management Journal, 19: 195-212. Job Attitudes Smith, C. Ann, Dennis W. Organ, and Janet Near 1983 "Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature and antecedents." Journal of Applied Psychology, 69: 653-663. Staw, Barry M., Bobby J. Calder, and Randall K. Hess 1980 "Intrinsic motivation and norms about payment." Journal of Personality, 48: 1-14. Staw, Barry M., and Jerry Ross 1985 "Stability in the midst of change: A dispositional approach to job attitudes." Journal of Applied Psychology, 70: 469-480. Taber,Tom D., Terry A. Beehr, and Jeffrey T. Walsh 1985 "Relationships between job evaluation ratings and selfratings of job characteristics." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35: 27-45. Terborg, James R. 1981 "Interactional psychology and research on human behavior in organizations." Academy of Management Review, 6: 569576. Vroom, Victor H. 1964 Work and Motivation. New York: Wiley. Wahba, Mahmoud A., and Lawrence G. Bridwell 1976 "Maslow reconsidered: A review of research on the need hierarchy theory." Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 15: 212-240. Watson, David, and Lee Anna Clark 1984 "Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states." Psychological Bulletin, 96: 465-490. Weaver, Charles N. 1978 "Job satisfaction as a component of happiness among males and females." Personnel Psychology, 31: 831-840. 1980 "Job satisfaction in the United States in the 1970's." Journal of Applied Psychology, 65: 364-367. Weiss, Howard M., and Seymour Adler 1984 "Personality and organizational behavior." In BarryM. Staw and L. L. Cummings (eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior, 6: 1-50. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 77/ASQ, March 1986 Weiss, Howard M., and James B. Shaw 1979 "Social influences on judgments about tasks." Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 24: 126-140. White, J. Kenneth 1978 "Individualdifferences and the job quality-worker response relationship: Review, integration, and comments." Academy of Management Review, 3: 267280. White, Sam E., and Terence R. M itchel l 1979 "Job enrichment versus social cues: A comparison and competitive test." Journal of Applied Psychology, 64: 1-9. Zuckerman, Marvin 1977 "Development of a situationspecific trait-state test for the prediction and measurement of affective responses." Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45: 513-523.