Nonsocial Play in Preschoolers: Necessarily Evil? Kameth H. Rubin University of Waterloo RUBIN, KENNETH H Nonsocial Flay in Preschoolers Necessarily Evil? CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1982, 53, 651-657 It has been suggested that children who play on their own, without interacting with peers, may be at nsk for social, cognitive, and social-cognitive problems Recently, however, the children's play hterature has revealed that some forms of nonsocial activity are constructive and adaptive In this study the social, cognitive, and social-cogmtive correlates of nonsocial play were examined 122 4-year-olds were observed for 20 mm dunng free play They were also administered a role-takmg test and tests of social and impersonal problemsolvmg skills Sociometiic popvdanty and social competence (as rated by teachers) were also assessed Analyses indicated that nonsocial-functional (sensonmotor) and dramatic activities generally correlated negatively with the measures of competence Parallel-constructive activities generally correlated positively with the various measures of competence In recent years, it has been suggested that children who play on their own, vwthout frequently interacting with peers, may be at nsk for later social and social-cognitive problems These suggesfaons stem from both theoretical and empirical sources Theoretically, Piaget (1926, 1932), in his early work, noted that the reciprocal nature of peer relations and the inevitable social conflicts concemmg diflFenng viewpoints in early childhood promoted the development of moral judgmental, discursive, and perspective-taking skills (see Rubm & Everett [m press] for a recent review of this hterature) From non-Piagetian perspectives, peers have been thought to serve as social leammg models and as social remforcers m the development of social and cognitive skiBs (Allen 1976, Combs & Slaby 1978, Hartup 1979) Given these theoretical perspecbves, developmentalists have recently predicted negative outcomes for children who have had madequate peer mteractive experiences Indirect empincal support for these theoretical positions emanates from the hterature concerning sociometnc measures of peer acceptance Poor sociometnc ratings m early and middle childhood have been shovwi to predict later social, educational, and mental health problems (Cowen, Pederson, Babigian, Izzo, & Trost 1973, Ro£F, Sells, & Golden 1972, Ullman 1957) However, direct support for the theoretical positions has been nuxed, at best At present, there is a notable lack of data to suggest that low rates of peer mterachon m early childhood either predict or concurrently relate with indices of soaal and social-cogmtive competence (Asher, Markell, & Hymel 1981) Moreover, low rates of peer interaction m early childhood have been found to correlate negatively with peer populanty m some studies ( e g , Goldman, Corsmi, & deUnoste 1980) but not m others ( e g , Gottman 1977) One possible explanation for the general lack of data supportive of the position that nonsocial activity correlates negatively with mdices of social competence emanates from the observational hterature concemmg children's play Rubm and his colleagues (Rubm, Maiom, & Homung 1976, Rubin, Watson, & Jambor 1978) have indicated that between 402 and 60% of the nonsocial activity of presdiool- and kmdergarten-aged children is constructive or educational in nature Given these data, it may be that the earlier reported correlations between nonsocial behavior and measures of populanty and social competence were somewhat obscured by mclusion of particular adaptive cognitive forms of play within the nonsocial categoncal system From this perspective it is conceivable that some forms of nonsocial activity are concur- This research was supported by a grant from the Ontano Mental Health Foimdation and the Ontano Ministiy of Commumty and^ Social Services An abbreviated version of this article was presented at the Bienmal Meetme of the Society for Research m Child Development, Apnl 1981, Boston I would Uce to thadc Judy Midde for her help in the collection and codmg of^the data Special thanks are also given to the teachers and children m the Regional Mumcipahty of Waterloo who graciously made their time available to me Requests for repnnts should be sent to Kenneth H Rubm, Department of Psychology, Umversity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontano, N2L 3G1 \PluU Devdepmtnt, 1982, S3, 651-657 ® 1982 by the Soaety for Reseaicfa in Child Development, Inc All nghta reserved 0009-392O/82/53O3-0O22$OI 00) 652 Child Development rently predicbve of developmental lag whereas others are equally predictive of posibve adaptation Such a suggestion would be in keeping with the Asher et al (1981) posibon that the simple use of rate of nonsocial acbvity to identify children at nsk would be less mentonous than the use of a quahtative assessment of nonsocial behavior for identification purposes The purpose of this study was simply to identify those forms of nonsocial play m 4-yearolds which correlate either positively or negabvely with assessments of competence m the social, social-cognitive, and cognitive domains In particular, it was hypothesized that the incidence of nonsocial-funcbonal (or sensonmotor) play, that is, repetibve muscle movements with or without objects (Smilansky 1968), because of its relatively low cognibve play status for 4-year-olds (Rubin, Fem, & Vandenberg, in press), would correlate negabvely with markers of soaal, social-cogmbve, and cognitive development Similarly, since most dramabc or fantasy play of 4-year-olds is earned out in small groups (Rubin et al, m press), it was predicted that the incidence of the sociaDy less mature form of dramabc activity (i e , nonsocial-dramatic play) would correlate negatively with assessments of social, social-cognibve, and cognibve development Construcbve play ( e g , artwork and puzzle and block construcbon) is often earned out most proficiently when alone Moreover, this adaptive behavior is the most frequent form of play generally observed m preschool settmgs (Rubin et al, m press) Given this {)erspecbve, it was predicted that the incidence of nonsocial forms of constructive pky would correlate positively with the latter measures of competence Fmally, it was predicted that the madence of unoccupied and onlooker behavior, two of Farten's (1932) least mature forms of social parbcipation, would correlate negabvely with assessments of social, social-cognibve, and cognibve development Method Subjects Fifty-three male and 69 female 4-year-olds (M = 58 11 months, SD = 4 37 months) who attended preschools or day-care centers m a southwestern Ontario community served as subjects in this study Familial socioeconomic status ranged from lower-middle to upper-middle class Instruments and Procedures Behavtoral observations—Each child was observed for a total of 30 min dunng free play following procedures similar to those described m Rubm et al (1976, 1978) Basically, each child was observed for six 10-sec bme intervals each day over a 30-day penod Behaviors were coded on a checklist which included the cogmtive play categories of Smilansky (1968), tiiat IS, functional-sensonmotor, construcbve, and dramabc play and games with rules The cognitive play categones were nested withm the social participabon categones earber descnbed m Parten (1932), that is, sobtary, parallel, and group activities Thus, for example, if a given child was observed to construct a puzzle m close proxirmty but not with another child, the activity was coded as parallel constructive Other observabonal categones included unoccupied behavior, onlooker behavior, reading or being read to, rough-and-tumble play, exploration, acbve conversabons with teachers or peers, and transitional (moving from one acbvity to another) activities After coding the child's play behavior, the observer noted the names of the focal child's play or conversational partners and who it was that lnibated the group activity FoUowmg Furman, Rahe, and Hartup (1979), the affecbve quahty of each social interchange was coded as positive, neutral, or negative Four observers recorded the children's play behaviors Each observer was tramed by using videotapes and observmg children's play in a laboratory preschool not employed m the present study Rehabihty was assessed by pairing each observer with every other observer for a total of 30 mm of observational coding each The number of coding agreements/ (number of agreements -f- disagreements) exceeded 85% m each case Soctometrtc populartty—The sociometnc rating scale recently developed by Asher, Singleton, Tinsley, and Hymel (1979) was employed in this study Each child was mdividually presented with color Polaroid photographs of each of his or her classmates The cmldren were asked to assign each picture to one of three boxes on which there was drawn either a happy face ("children you hke a lot"), a neutral face ("children you kmda hke"), or a sad face ("children you don't hke") As m Asher et al (1979), cluldren thus received a number of positive, negative, and neutral rabngs Posibve ratmgs were accorded a score of 3, neutral ratings a score of 2, and negative ratings a score of 1 Each child received one total rabng score Since the numbers of children m each of the eight classrooms vaned, the rabng score re- Kenneth H. Rubin ceived by each child was divided by the number of children m each class who were given the sociometnc test Social-competence —Two teachers m each of the classrooms were given the Behar and Stnngfield (1974) Preschool Behavior Questionnaire (PBQ) to fill out High scores on the PBQ are suggestive of social maladjustment The teacher ratings m each class were averaged for each child Role taktng and social problem solving — Cognitive role-takmg skill was measured by admmistenng the DeVnes (1970) "hide the penny" game to each child The expenmenter and child took turns hiding a coin m one of his or her hands Each child was scored according to the recursive thought cntena described by DeVnes The maximum score was 10 Social problem-solving skill was assessed by admmistermg an elaborated version of Spivack and Shure's (1974) Preschool Interpersonal Problem-Solving (PIPS) test to each child The Social Problem-Solvmg Task (SPST) was designed to assess both quantitative and qualitative features of social problem solving In general, each child was presented with a senes of eight pictured problem situations m which one story character wants to play with a toy or use some matenal that another child has in his or her possession The child was asked what the central character in the story could do or say so that she or he could gam access to the toy or matenal The characters m the story vaned with regard to either age (same vs different age characters), sex (same vs different sex characters), and race (same vs cross-sex characters) For example, with regard to the three agerelated picture stones, in one case two 4-yearold, same-sex, same-race characters were portrayed In a second picture, sex and race were held constant, and trie subject was told that a 4-year-old wanted to play with an object in the possession of a 6-year-ola In the third case, the 4-year-old wanted a toy being played with by a 2-year-old Similar covanations occurred for the sex and race of the story characters Detailed examples of the stones may be obtamed from the author After presentation of each picture and the assoaated story* the child was asked to tell the expenmenter everything that the central character could do or say so that she or he could obtain the desired object As m the PIPS (Spi- 653 vack & Shure 1974) the number of relevant alternatives was computed Impersonal problem solving —^An elaboration of Smith and Dutton's (1979) lure retneval problem was administered to each child An extended descnpbon is found m Cheyne and Rubin (Note 1) Each child was seated at a low table on which was placed a set of sticks and blocks Each set consisted of three 24-cm, three 15-cm, and three 6-cm sbcks and five blocks with four holes The expenmenter pointed out that the sbcks were of different lengths and demonstrated the mserbon of a stick into one of the holes m a block The child was allowed to play with these materials for 8 mm Durmg this penod, the expenmenter kept a conbnuous record of the child's play constructions and his or her verbahzabons Constructions were coded as simple, moderate, or complex (Cheyne & Rubin, Note 1), and the number of each type of construction was noted From these observations a measure of configural complexity was computed by subbactmg the number of simple constructions from the number of moderate plus complex construcbons (Cheyne & Rubin, Note 1) The number of fantasy ( e g , rocket ships, people) construcbons was also recorded Following this penod, all children were immediately presented with six sticks (two of each length), a block, and the problem of retnevmg a marble enclosed m a transparent box placed at a distance from the child The solubon mvolved the joining of the two longest sticks by means of the block, releasing a latch on the box, and raking m the marble Although hints in the manner of Sylva (1977) were given at approximately 1-mm mtervals (to a maximum of five hints), the major dependent vanable for impersonal problem solving was ttme to solution Dunng the problem-solving penod, the number of posibve selfstatements ("I can do it'") and the number of denials ("I can't do it", "I don't wanna do it") were also recorded Receptive vocabulary —The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test was administered to each child Mental age (MA) was computed Results and DiBcussion A correlation matnx was computed for the vanables descnbed in the Methods secbon Pnnciple correlations of interest herem mcluded those with each of the nonsocial behavior cat- 1 Full descnpbons of the instruments and scormg procedures are available from the author on request 654 Child Developinimt egones, that is, so&iary-functional, -constmcbve, and -dramatic play, poraZfei-functional, -constructiye, and -dramatic play, and unoccupied and onlooker activity Those correlations which reached (p < 05 or better, one-tailed) or approached significance are reported m table 1 Perhaps the least mature form of play produced by 4-year-olds in preschool settmgs is that which is earned out alone and mvolyes sensorunotor, repetitive motor acbons (Rubm et al, m press) Sohtary-functional play was found to correlate negatively with MA, r(121) = 19, p < 02 Wrth the efiFects of MA partialed out, the frequency of solitary-functional play was negatively correlated with (a) the number of social overtures received from other children, (b) the proportion of positive interactions to the total number of social interactions, (c) the number of peer cwiyersations, (d) the sociometnc rating, and (e) the mdex of construction complexity computed during the "play phase" of the impersonal problem-solvmg paradigm It should be noted that complexity of object construction during play is a significant predictor of problem-solving proficiency (Cheyne & Rubin, Note 1) Taken together, these correlational data suggest that the production of a large amount of sohtary-functional play by 4year-olds may be taken as an "at-nsk" indicator The incidence of sohtary-constractive play was negatively correlated with (a) the number of social overtures received and (b) the number of peer conversations held during free play Given the lack of significant correlations with either the peer sociometnc ratings or with the teacher ratings of social competence, this category would appear to be somewhat benign and certainly less negatively tinged than sohtaryfunctional activity When fantasy or dramatic play occurs in 4-year-olds it is usually carried out with or near other children That is, the "norm" for this age group IS the production of social rather than nonsocial dramatic play (Rubm et al, in press). As such, solitary-dramatic play at 4 years of age may reflect a lag m those social, cognitive, and social-cognitive skills usually associated with pretense activity, that is, perspective-takmg and problem-solvmg skills Given this speculation, it IS noteworthy that the frequency of sohtarydramatic play was negatively related with chronological age (CA) even within this restricted age-range sample Sohtary-dramatic play was also negatively correlated with (a) the prc^rtion of positive group interactions to the total number of social interactions, (b) performance on the DeVnes (1970) perspective-taking task, (c) soaometnc status, and (d) the measure of TABLE 1 PARTIAI. CORRELATIONS BETWEEN NONSOCIAL PLAY CATEGORIES AND OTHER MEASURES, CONTROLLING FOH MA {N = 123) Sol-F Reading Conversations Rough and tumble Transitional Social initiations received (N) Proportion positive interactions Proportion neutral interactions Proportion negative lnteracUons CA DeVnes Soaometnc Relevant categones (SPST) Complexity Fantasy constructions Denials Time to solution Teacher rating total So!-D Sol-C - 11 - 48* 04 13 - 05 35* 11 02 - 30* - 23** P-F PC 02 08 01 21** 09 02 P-D On Un - 26** - 02 - 10 - 07 10 - 30* 19*** 06 03 - 14 - 06 - 10 - 09 11 11 - 16*** 11 09 - 18*** 13 - 07 13 12 11 - 05 - 09 - 18*** - 06 - 22** - 03 - 09 06 - 07 04 21** - 18*** - 07 - 22** - 03 - 16*** - 08 03 03 - 06 - 05 - 13 - 06 - 15*** - 15*** - 10 21*** - 23** - 05 06 04 12 01 - 01 05 - 16*** 09 - 14 05 - - 01 16*** _ 06 16*** - 11 16*** 09 01 03 03 14 - 07 15*** 01 - - 02 - 25** - 21** 10 17*** - 12 03 i9»*» 03 11 04 - 06 - 05 01 15*** 03 08 15*** - 03 - 02 10 20*** - 09 06 13 07 06 12 19*** 15*** 18*** 18*** 12 - 03 20*** 10 02 - 12 26** - 01 13 08 - 32* NOTE —Sol — sohtaiy, P — i>anllel. On •• unoccujMed, On — onlooker, F — ftinctumil, C — comtnictive, O — dramatic • # < 001 (fflie-Ualed) **p< 01 ***p< 05 Kenneth H. Robin construcbon complexity made dunng the "play phase" of the impersonal problem-solvmg task The latter measure has been viewed as a significant precursor to the solubon of the impersonal problem presented herein (see Cheyne & Rubm. Note 1) Sohtary-dramatic play was posibvely correlated with teacher ratmgs of social maladjustment A positive correlabonal trend was found with the amount of time it took to complete the impersonal problem-solvmg task (p < 06) Given the data above, it would appear as if the mcidence of sohtary-dramatic play m 4-year-olds is a concurrent mdex of lack of competence in the social, social-cognibve, and cognibve domams For play enthusiasts, the message may be that not all pretense acbvity augurs well for 4-year-olds Given the correlational nature of these data, however, firm causal statements concenung sobtary pretense cannot be made Parallel-funcbonal acbvity, or repebbve motor acbvity in close proximity to others, was found to correlate posibvely with (a) the frequency of transitional (or moving from one activity to the next) behaviors, (fc) the proportion of negabve mteracbons to the total number of social mteracbons, and (c) the measure of play construction complexity The picture pamted by such acbvity is diat it involves sensonmotor, usually large-scale, frenebc motor behavior that bnngs children mto conflicts, as evinced by the posibve correlabon with negative mteractions The posibve correlabon with transitional activity suggests the possibility that the two behaviors may be taken as a classroom index of lmpulsivity Given the negabve correlation between parallel-functional play and the construcbon of complex play figures/objects (which appears to require planmng, reflection, and self-regulabon), the nobon of parallel-funcbonal acbvity as a behavioral index of lmpulsivity IS not farfetched Further research would do well to explore this postulated hnk directly Interesbngly, neither teacher nor peer ratings of social competence or popularity were negabvely linked to such potenbally disturbmg behavior m the classroom It might be worthwhile to note that the correlabon between parallel-funcbonal acbvity and the number of posibve sodometnc rabngs from same-sex peers was, m fact, moderately significant m the negabve direcbon, r(121) = — 15, p < 05 As such, the production of parallel-funcbonal play m 4-year-olds may very caubously be taken as an mdicator of problemabc social development Parallel-constnicbve behavior appears to 655 resemble those acbvities that elementary school teachers promote m their classrooms Moreover, it IS the most frequently occumng activity observed m prescbool setbngs (Rubm et al, m press) Typically, the children sit m close proximity, often around the same table, and carry out art, LEGO-block, puzzle, or other creative/ construcbve acbvibes As one might expect from this descnpbon, teacher ratmgs erf social maladjustment were significantly and negabvely correlated with the frequency of parallel-constructive play (with the effects of MA parballed out) The incidence of this acbvity was also linked significantly and posibvely with MA With the effects of MA parbaled out, the frequency of parallel-constnicbve play was negabvely correlated with the mcidence of roughand-tumble play and with the amount of time it took to solve the impersonal problem Posibve correlabons were evmced for (a) sociometnc ratmgs, (b) the number of relevant altemabves produced on the social problem-solvmg task, and (c) the measure of play construcbon complexity Taken together, diese correlabonal data suggest that children who play near but not with others and who engage m construcbve activities are good problem solvers in both the social and nonsocial domains Such children are also viewed as popular among their peers and as socially skilled by their teachers Parallel-dramatic acbvity occurs when children take on roles in close proximity to but not in coordinabon with their peers. Suoi uncoordinated pretense may result from the child's mabihty to take into account the various roles and social rules ostensibly mvolved m sociodramatic activity (Garvey 1977, Rubin et al, m press) Given this perspective, it was not surprising to find a significant negabve correlabon between the producbon of parallel-dramabc play and GA Although the themes of pretense activity were not coded m this study, it would have been mteresbng to note whether fantasy behavior conducted near but not with others is more likely to take on hostile/aggressive than domesbc or less rambunctious themes Performance of such acbvity m proximity to but not with others could serve as a safeguard m ascertaining that one's peers understand that the hostihty displayed is not meant hterally or that it IS "just pretend " Clues about the themes of parallel-dramabc play are drawn from the posibve correlations with teacher ratmgs of social maladjustment as well as with tiie mcidence of rough-and-tumble play Moreover, the emission 656 C3iild Devdopment of parallel-dramatic play was negatively correlated with positive social exchange and yet not correlated with negative interactions Interestingly, the frequency of such dramatic activity was correlated with the production of fantasy objects ( e g , guns) and people durmg the stick-block play session of the impersonal problem-solvmg task The data thus suggest that parallel-dramatic play, although not viewed as desirable by teacliers, may be an appropnate medium for hostile displays m the preschool Unoccupied behavior was positively related with teacher ratings of social maladjustment and negatively correlated with the number of peer conversations Onlooker activity was negatively correlated with MA, r(121) = - 18, p < 03 With the effects of MA partialed out, onlooker behavior was negatively correlated with (a) CA, (b) the number of peer conversations, (c) rough-and-tumble play, (d) complexity of play constructions, and (e) teacher ratmgs of social maladjustment In short, the more often that children spent their tune harmlessly observing others or looking at objects around the room, the greater the hkelfliood that they were younger mentally and chronologically than their counterparts who displayed less such behavior Onlookers were viewed also as relatively soaally competent by then- teachers As such, onlooker behavior may be viewed as a somewhat benign "Charhe Brown''-type activity The correlational analyses computed for males and females separately did not vary significantly from those data presented above Interestmgly, the DeVnes measure of role taking correlated negatively with sohtary-function^ play for girls, r = — 28, p < 02, but not for boys Alternatively, this social-cognitive measure correlated negatively with onlooker behavior for boys, r = — 32, p < 03, but not for girls These negative correlations with behavioral mdices of nonsocial play do make sense theoretically However, the sex differences elude convement explanation at this time No other sex differences were found m the separate correlational analyses In summary, the observational data reported herem support the recent contention that qualitative rather than quantitative dimensions of nonsocial activity be explored when children are targeted as "at nsk" for developmental problems (Asher et al 1981) Some nonsocial activities ( e g , sohtary-functional, sohtary-dramatic, and parallel-functional play) do, mdeed, correlate negatively with mdices of social, soaal- cognitive, and cogmtive skdl Other forms of nonsocial activity ( e g , soLtary-constructive, onlooker behaviors) are somewhat benign Parallel-constructive play IS highly predictive of competence To some degree, these latter data nicely mirror those of Jennings (1975), who reported that preschool children who show greater onentation to objects rather than people spend much of their time constructing products m their play, purportedly as a result of such activity, they perform well on tests of abibty to organize and classify physical matenals The paraDelconstnictive play correlations reported herein support Jennmgs's findings Moreover, the present study also suggests that parallel-constructive players perform well, not only on impersonal problems but also on social problems Furthermore, such nonsocial activity was associated with peer populanty and teacher ratings of social competence These data clearly indicate that not all nonsocial activity is associated with negative developmental prognosis Grven the lack of an overall negative correlational picture for nonsocial but constructive forms of play, it would thus behoove researchers and practitioners not to denigrate all forms of nonsocial activity Psychologists and educators who plan preschool intervention programs to amehorate social play "deficits" or to prevent the supposed negative outcomes of nonsocial activity in early childhood would do well to focus on those behaviors found to be concurrently correlated with assessments of social, cognitive, and social-cognitive lag Reference Note 1 Cheyne, J A, & Rubin, K H Playful precursors of problem-solving m preschoolers Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research m Child Development, Boston, April 1981 References Allen, V Chddren as teachers New York Academic Press, 1976 Asher, S R , Markell, R A , & Hymel, S Identifying children at nsk m peer relations- a cntique of the rate-of-mteraction approach to assessment Child Vevdopmera, 1981, 52(4), 123&-1245 Asher, S R , Singleton, L C , Tuisley, B R , & Hymel, S A reliable sociometnc measure for preschool children Developmental Psychology, 1979, 15, 443-444 Behar, L , & Strmgfield, S A behavior rating scale Kenneth H. Rubin for the preschool child Development^ Psy-" chology, 1974, 10, 601-^10 Combs, M L , & Slaby, D A Social skills traimng with children In B Lahey & A Kazdm (Eds ), Advances in chnictd chdd psychology Vol 1. New York Plenum, 1978 Cowen, E L , Pederson, A , Babigian, H , Izzo, L D , & Trost, M A Long-term follow-up of early detected vulnerable children Journal of Constdtmg and Clinical Psychology, 1973, 41, 438-446 DeVnes, R The development of role-taking as reflected by the behavior of bnght, average, and retarded children m a social guessing game ChUd Devdopmera, 1970, 4 1 , 759-770 Furman, W , Rahe, D F , & Hartup, W W Rehabihtation of socially withdrawn preschool children through mixed-age and same-age sociallzabon Child Development, 1979, 50, 915922 Garvey, G Play Gambndge, Mass Harvard University Press, 1977 Goldman, J A , Corsmi, D A , & deUnoste, R Imphcations of positive and negabve sociometnc status for assessmg the social competence of young children Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 1980, 1, 209-220 Gottman, J M Toward a definihon of social isolation in children Chdd Development, 1977, 48, 513-517 Hartup, W W Peer relabons and the growth of social competence In M W Kent & J E Rolf (Eds ), Primary prevention of psychopathology Vol 3. Social competence tn children Hanover, N H University Press of New En^and, 1979 Jenmngs, K D People versus object orientation, social behavior, and mtellectual abihties m children Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 511-519 Parten, M B Social participation among preschool 657 children Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1932, 27, 243-269 Piaget, J The language and thought of the chdd London Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1926 Piaget, J The moral judgment of the child Glencoe. III Free Press, 1932 Roff, M , Sells, S B , & Golden, M M Socud adjustment and personality development m chddren Minneapolis Umversity of Minnesota Press, 1972 Rubm, K H , & Everett, B Social perspecbvetaking m young children In S Moore & C Cooper (Eds ), The young chdd Vol 3 Washington, D G National Association for the Education of Young Children Pubhcabons, m press Rubm, K H , Fem, G G , & Vandenberg, B Play In E M Hethenngton (Ed ), Handbook of chdd psychology Vol 3 Social develojnnent New York Wiley, m press Rubm, K H , Maiom, T L , & Homung, M Free play behaviors m middle- and lower-class preschoolers Parten and Piaget revisited Chdd Development, 1976, 47, 414-119 Rubm, K H , Watson, K , & Jambor, T Free-play behaviors in preschool and kindergarten children Chdd Development, 1978, 49, 534-536 Smilansky, S The effects of sociodramatic play on dtsadvantaged preschool chddren New York Wiley, 1968 Smith, P K, & Dutton, S Play and traimng m direct and innovative problem solving Chdd Development, 1979, 50, 830-836 ack, G, & Shure, M B Social adjustment m young chddren Jossey-Bass, 1974 Sylva, K Play and learmng In B Tizard & D Harvey (Ekls ), The btdogy of piay London SIMP/Hememann, 1977 Ullman, G H Teachers, peers, and tests as predictors of adjustment Journal of Educational Psychology, 1957, 48, 257-267