Journal of Language and Social Psychology http://jls.sagepub.com/ Beastly: What Makes Animal Metaphors Offensive? Nick Haslam, Steve Loughnan and Pamela Sun Journal of Language and Social Psychology published online 15 June 2011 DOI: 10.1177/0261927X11407168 The online version of this article can be found at: http://jls.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/05/03/0261927X11407168 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com Additional services and information for Journal of Language and Social Psychology can be found at: Email Alerts: http://jls.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://jls.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 JLS407168 SXXX10.1177/0261927X11407168Haslam et al.Journal of Language and Social Psychology JL Beastly: What Makes Animal Metaphors Offensive? Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) 1­–15 © 2011 SAGE Publications DOI: 10.1177/0261927X11407168 http://jls.sagepub.com Nick Haslam1, Steve Loughnan2, and Pamela Sun1 Abstract Animal metaphors convey a wide range of meanings, from insulting slurs to expressions of love. Two studies examined factors contributing to the offensiveness of these metaphors. Study 1 examined 40 common metaphors, finding that their meanings were diverse but centered on depravity, disagreeableness, and stupidity. Their offensiveness was predicted by the revulsion felt toward the animal and by the dehumanizing view of the target that it implied. Study 2 examined contextual factors in metaphor use, finding that the offensiveness of animal metaphors varies with the tone of their expression and the gender and in-group/out-group status of their targets. These variations influence offensiveness by altering the extent to which the target is ascribed animalistic properties. Keywords dehumanization, gender, metaphor, offensiveness Metaphorical language allows people to experience and understand one kind of entity in terms of another. Features of a particular source domain are conceptually mapped and selectively transferred onto a target domain, allowing target categories to be apprehended in novel and often revealing ways. The cognitive processes involved in the understanding of metaphor—associative and inferential, pragmatic and semantic, abstract and embodied—are the focus of an active research literature (e.g., Gibbs, 2006; Ritchie, 2009; Wilson & Carston, 2006). 1 University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, England 2 Corresponding Author: Nick Haslam, Department of Psychology, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia Email: nhaslam@unimelb.edu.au Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 2 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) An especially intriguing class of metaphors involves the use of animals to refer to people. The animal kingdom is an unusually bountiful source domain and provides a rich metaphorical vocabulary. Animal metaphors are ubiquitous (Kövecses, 2002) and often based on a hierarchical structure—the Great Chain of Being—in which humans are located above and animals below (Lakoff & Turner, 1989). Animals are often understood in anthropomorphic ways (Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2007), and people are likened to animals in countless verbal expressions. The diversity, cultural importance, and tantalizing almost-humanness of animals make them ideal vehicles for representing people and may even have made them the first symbols (Berger, 1980). As LéviStrauss (1963) wrote, animals are not just good to eat but also “good to think.” Animal metaphors can be employed in diverse ways. They may be endearing, as in the use of pet names for children or lovers, or hostile when used as slurs and insults, and they may refer to individuals or groups. In many cultures, people use animal terms to abuse persons who violate social rules (Van Oudenhoven et al., 2008) and as ethnic slurs (Rice et al., 2010). Some animal metaphors stand for specific human traits in a kind of zoological shorthand (“mouse” = timidity, “owl” = wisdom), others imply indirect metonymic connections (the French are “frogs” because they stereotypically eat them), and still others imply that the target has an almost literal equivalence to the animal in question (e.g., Nazi images of Jews as “vermin”). Different animal metaphors may have different meanings, and the same metaphor can have radically different implications in different contexts of use. Simian metaphors may be cute when applied to children but extremely offensive when used in interracial contexts. People are often called “animals” not only when they violate social norms in depraved ways but also when they act in ways that are seen as desirably wild. When Victoria Beckham stated that David was an animal in bed, we assume she was not complaining about his body odor or lack of intelligence. Although animal metaphors are widespread, little is known about what accounts for their differences in meaning and offensiveness. Jay (2009) recognizes animal terms as one class of taboo words, and Leach (1964) argued that they are insulting when the animal itself has an ambiguous status or is difficult to classify. Brandes (1984) argues that animal metaphors label socially disapproved attributes as a way to reinforce a society’s moral order, and Goatly (2006) locates their offensiveness in the ideology of human superiority to animals. Linguistic and anthropological studies of animal expressions have been conducted in several languages (e.g., Hsieh, 2006; Rakusan, 2004; Talebinejad & Dastjerdi, 2005), but these have not attempted to lay bare the psychology of those expressions. One lens through which animal metaphors can be viewed is the concept of dehumanization (Haslam, 2006). Dehumanizing animal metaphors have figured prominently in many of the past century’s violent conflicts, such as the Rwandan Hutus’ reference to Tutsis as “cockroaches” (Bell-Fialkoff, 1996). Recent psychological research has shown that many out-groups are subtly denied attributes that distinguish humans from animals (Leyens et al., 2003), implicitly associated with animals in general (Loughnan & Haslam, 2007; Saminaden, Loughnan, & Haslam, 2010; Viki et al., 2006), Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 3 Haslam et al. or associated with apes in particular (Boccato, Capozza, Falvo, & Durante, 2008; Goff, Eberhardt, Williams, & Jackson, 2008). People also go to great lengths to deny and distance themselves from their own animality or “creatureliness” (Goldenberg et al., 2001). Although this research has advanced knowledge of dehumanization, it has not directly examined the use of animal metaphors. Its primary focus on group perception also neglects those occasions when animal metaphors are applied to individuals on the basis of perceived personal qualities rather than group memberships. Most fundamentally, likening people to animals is not invariably dehumanizing: calling one’s baby “piglet” is not the same as angrily calling someone an “ape.” To understand what makes animal metaphors offensive, it is important to understand matters of content and context. That is, what meanings do animal metaphors convey, and what situational factors moderate those meanings? With respect to content, we must map the characteristics that different animal metaphors imply, and which characteristics contribute to offensiveness. There is an almost complete lack of previous psychological research or theory on the aspects of animal metaphors that might predict offensiveness, so our search for candidate predictors was exploratory and largely intuitive rather than based on a solid taxonomy. Nevertheless, several aspects of animal metaphors are plausible predictors of offensiveness. First, more offensive metaphors may be those that involve more disliked animals: likening someone to a reviled or disgusting animal amounts to ascribing its negative qualities to them. Second, more offensive metaphors may be those that are most dehumanizing. Certain animal comparisons may be especially degrading to people’s humanity. These animals need not be the most reviled: the “dog” metaphor is insulting but dogs are not disliked. Third, some animals may signify animality better than others, directly implying that the person is animal-like rather than merely standing for certain human traits. These metaphors may be especially offensive as they imply that the person is other than human. Fourth, metaphors that convey content that accurately describes the particular animal (e.g., calling someone a “cat” implying that they have truly feline traits) may be offensive for the same reason. Fifth, some metaphors may imply a literal equation of the target with a particular animal rather than a mere figurative similarity. Calling someone a “maggot” does not imply that they are literally legless and larval, but calling them an “ape” may imply that they are fundamentally ape-like. Finally, more genetically distant animals may serve as more offensive metaphor because they imply a greater dissimilarity from humans. These six potential predictors of offensiveness may not be conceptually or empirically independent, so multivariate analysis was used to disentangle their associations with offensiveness. We can also hypothesize about contextual determinants of offensiveness. These might involve the metaphor’s target, the metaphor’s user, and the nature of the relationship between them. The offensiveness of a metaphor, in short, may depend not just on its propositional meaning but also on the speaker’s intention in expressing it in a particular social context (i.e., its “illocutionary force”; Holtgraves & Kashima, 2008). With respect to the target, animal metaphors may be considered more offensive when directed at women rather than men on account of benign sexism (Glick & Fiske, 1997), Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 4 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) which portrays women as vulnerable, pure, and in need of protection. In addition, women appear to be more averse to offensive language than men (Jay & Janschewitz, 2008), and awareness of this difference might contribute to the greater judged offensiveness of potentially abusive metaphors directed at them. With respect to the user, the manner in which the metaphor is expressed may moderate its offensiveness. A hostile delivery is likely to make the expression more offensive than a jocular one, even if the same animal metaphor is used. As Norrick (2003) argues in his work on conversational joking, mockery and sarcasm can serve to enhance rapport among friends and colleagues. With respect to the relationship between user and target, animal metaphors may be more offensive when they are expressed across group boundaries. The same animal expression may be more offensive when expressed toward an outgroup member than an in-group member, just as people are more sensitive to criticisms from out-group members than in-group members (Hornsey, Oppes, & Svensson, 2002; Sutton, Elder, & Douglas, 2006). We conducted two studies to examine determinants of the offensiveness of animal metaphors. Study 1 focused on metaphor content, systematically investigating the psychological meanings conveyed by a sample of metaphors and testing six hypotheses about what makes some particularly offensive (animal dislike, dehumanization, animality, metaphor accuracy, literalness, genetic distance). Study 2 focused on the context of metaphor use, testing hypotheses about target gender, user tone (jocular versus hostile), and the user–target relationship (intra- vs. intergroup). Study 2 also examined whether the offensiveness of an animal expression in context was associated with the implied dehumanization of its target, offering an alternative test of a Study 1 hypothesis. Study 1 The first study examined variability in the perceived offensiveness of animal metaphors as a function of their content. Common animal metaphors were rated on items assessing (a) their offensiveness and target gender, (b) what psychological attributes they ascribe to their targets, and (c) other aspects of the metaphor that enabled tests of the six hypotheses. The psychological attributes examined were personality traits and highly evaluative personality attributes (i.e., linked to depravity, stupidity, peculiarity, and worthlessness; Benet-Martinez & Waller, 2002). The latter were included on the expectation that many animal metaphors have insulting content. Method Participants. The sample consisted of 60 Australian undergraduates (33 women, 27 men) whose mean age was 20.7 years (SD = 4.9). Participants were recruited for a study of “Animal metaphors” and paid $10 for taking part. Materials. All participants completed a questionnaire in which they made a series of judgments about the use of several animal metaphors. Seven graduate students were Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 5 Haslam et al. instructed to list animal metaphors in common usage and generated 41 nonredundant metaphors. After removing one metaphor that our ethics committee forbade us to use, we employed a final set of 40 metaphors: animal, ape, bat, beast, cat, chick, chicken, chimp, cow, dog, fish, fox, goat, gorilla, guinea pig, hog, horse, kitten, lamb, leech, monkey, mouse, orangutan, owl, ox, pig, puppy, rabbit, rat, shark, sheep, sloth, slug, snake, toad, vulture, weasel, whale, wolf, and worm. The metaphors included particular species (“gorilla”), broad classes (“fish”), and collective terms (“animal,” “beast”). Each metaphor was judged on 10 items printed on a single sheet of paper, headed by the following statement: “Imagine that someone calls someone else a (ANIMAL TERM).” Offensiveness and target characteristics. “How insulting is this expression?” (1 = Not at all to 5 = Extremely); “Is it used more often to refer to males or females?” (1 = More to males to 5 = More to females). Content of metaphor. “Is this term used mostly to describe the person’s bodily features (e.g., appearance, physique, strength) or their psychological features (e.g., personality, abilities, behavior)?” (1 = More bodily to 5 = More psychological); “Listed below are 14 groups of characteristics. Circle any group that captures what is implied about someone when they are referred to as a (animal term) (you may circle none, one, or more than one).” Ten groups were sets of four trait terms representing poles of the Big Five personality dimensions. Four groups were sets of four terms representing the dimensions of highly evaluative negative characteristics (Benet-Martinez & Waller, 2002). Hypothesis-related items. Accuracy: “Do the characteristics you wrote and circled above accurately describe (animal term plural)?” (1 = Not at all to 5 = Extremely well). Animality: “Are the characteristics you wrote and circled above more typical of animals in general than humans?” (1 = No more typical of animals than humans to 5 = Much more typical of animals than humans). Animal dislike: “How negative are people’s perceptions of (animal term plural)?” (1 = Not at all negative to 5 = Extremely negative). Literalness: “The person using this expression is implying that the other person is literally a (animal term)?” (1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree). Dehumanization: “The person using this expression believes the person they are referring to is less than human” (1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree). Genetic distance: “How genetically similar are (animal term plural) to humans?” (1 = Not at all to 5 = Extremely; reversed). Each participant was randomly assigned 10 animal metaphors to judge, so that each of the 40 metaphors was judged by a random subsample of 15 participants. Procedure. Participants completed the study in laboratory conditions under the supervision of a researcher. All completed the questionnaire in 30 minutes or less. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 6 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) Table 1. Most Common Attribute Sets Endorsed for Each Animal Metaphor (Study 1) Attribute Set Metaphor Agreeableness (high) Agreeableness (low) Extraversion (high) Extraversion (low) Conscientiousness (high) Conscientiousness (low) Neuroticism (high) Neuroticism (low) Openness (high) Openness (low) Depravity Stupidity Worthlessness Peculiarity Kitten, puppy, rabbit Bat, beast, cow, rat, shark, snake, wolf Chick, chimp, fox Guinea pig, kitten, lamb, mouse — Animal, pig, sloth, slug Chicken Owl Monkey Sheep Animal, beast, dog, hog, leech, pig, rat, toad, vulture, whale, worm Ape, cow, fish, gorilla, orangutan Worm — Note. Only metaphors with a peak endorsement frequency ≥5 (33.3%) are included. Metaphors with two equally endorsed peak attribute sets are listed twice. Results All data were aggregated so that the animal metaphor could be the unit of analysis. Thus, the mean rating of each animal on each item was calculated across the 15 randomly assigned people who rated it. For the attribute quality item, the number of participants endorsing each of the 14 attribute sets was counted. The animal metaphors were not seen as directed more toward one gender than the other, M = 2.76, t(39) = 1.74, p = .09, or as having more psychological or more bodily content, M = 3.27, t(39) = 1.96, p = .06. Table 1 lists the metaphors according to the psychological attribute set that was most frequently endorsed for each. Collectively, the metaphors had varied content, with only two attribute sets having no exemplary animal. Figure 1 demonstrates that negative attributes predominate, with the most common elements being depravity, disagreeableness, and stupidity. Correlations between offensiveness ratings and the hypothesis-related ratings are presented in Table 2. The table indicates that more offensive metaphors tend to be addressed to male targets, convey meanings that do not accurately describe the animal in question, involve more disliked animals, are relatively literal, and reflect a dehumanizing perception of the target. When offensiveness was regressed on its five significant correlates to assess which predictors had independent associations with offensiveness—F(5, 34) = 10.53, p < .001, adjusted R2 = .55—only animal dislike (β = .44, p < .01) and dehumanization (β = .48, p < .01) remained as significant predictors. Thus, the offensiveness of animal metaphors appears to be a function of the taboo quality Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 7 Haslam et al. 3.5 3 2.5 2 Positive 1.5 Negative 1 0.5 0 Figure 1. Mean frequency of endorsement of the attribute sets across the 40 animal metaphors, Study 1 Table 2. Correlations Between Offensiveness of Animal Metaphors and Metaphor Attributes, Study 1 (Decimal Omitted) 1. Female target 2. Psychological 3. Accuracy 4. Animality 5. Animal dislike 6. Literalness 7. Dehumanization 8. Genetic distance Offensiveness 1 −33* 04 −35* −15 63** 34* 64** 07 −15 −11 −26 −38* −47** −49** 36* 2 3 10 −13 53** 36* −17 −19 06 −01 −23 33* −31 4 −25 34* 03 −43** 5 6 37* 41** 58** 30 −23 7 −36* **p < .01. * p < .05. of the animal and the dehumanizing depiction of the person. These two phenomena were only moderately related (r = .41, p < .05). Discussion Study 1 indicates that animal metaphors convey diverse meanings. Some of these meanings are positive, indicating that animal metaphors are not invariably insulting. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 8 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) However, the metaphor contents were primarily negative, and they disproportionately involved highly evaluative characteristics. Despite their diversity, the main content of animal metaphors is a view of their targets as depraved, disagreeable, and unintelligent. No support was found for the hypotheses that the offensiveness of animal metaphors reflects how accurately they reflect the specific animal’s attributes, how well they represent general animality, or how genetically distant they are from humans. Thus, it does not appear to be the case that animal metaphors are offensive simply by virtue of likening humans to animals (generally or specifically). Animals are not intrinsically disliked, and the attributes they are seen to embody are often positive. Similarly, animal metaphors do not become offensive simply by taking humans down many phylogenetic notches. Indeed, many of the most offensive metaphors involved our fellow mammals (e.g., apes, pigs, rats). The key finding of Study 1 is that offensiveness is primarily related to two factors. The more offensive metaphors were those that involved more disliked animals and those that were seen as dehumanizing the target. More literal metaphors were also more offensive, but this association reflected the strong link between literalness and dehumanization: more dehumanizing metaphors were those in which the person was equated with an animal rather than just figuratively likened to one. The distinctness of these two primary predictors of offensiveness suggests that two processes are involved. Offensiveness derives both from the transfer of reviled characteristics from taboo animals to metaphor targets and from the positioning of the target as literally less than human, even when the animal in question is not taboo. This possibility is supported by further exploration of the results. Some of the most offensive animal metaphors involved reviled animals such as snakes, rats, and leeches. Targets of these metaphors were not seen as being literally animal-like. The primary content of the metaphors was depravity and disagreeableness, indicating moral disgust. Other highly offensive metaphors were seen as highly literal, equating targets with animals, and having content that primarily involved stupidity. The best examples of these metaphors were “ape” and “dog.” It may be possible to distinguish two kinds of offensive animal metaphors: those that are disgusting and those that are degrading. Despite its exploratory nature, Study 1 therefore suggests some promising lines of further inquiry into the meanings and implications of animal metaphors. Future research would ideally use larger samples, examine metaphors collected in multiple language communities, and ensure that if different participants judge different sets of metaphors they all share one or more common metaphors to anchor their judgments. Study 2 Study 2 shifted focus from the content of animal metaphors to contextual factors that might influence their perceived offensiveness. We presented a vignette in which one of three animal metaphors was used to describe a person and systematically varied Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 9 Haslam et al. contextual factors relating to the metaphor target (gender), user (tone of expression), and user–target relationship (group context). We hypothesized that metaphors would be judged more offensive when expressed to a female target, in a hostile tone, and in an intergroup context. We also assessed the personality attributes that the metaphor expression was judged to imply about the target. We hypothesized that expressions implying that the target was less than human would be judged particularly offensive. Method Participants. Participants were 72 Australian undergraduates (46 women, 26 men) whose mean age was 19.2 years (SD = 2.3). They completed the study as part of a course requirement. Materials. Participants responded to a vignette in which one person uses an animal metaphor to refer to another. The vignette was systematically varied on four factors: tone (jocular vs. hostile), target gender (male vs. female), group context (intra- vs. intergroup), and metaphor (ape, rat, or pig). Three participants were randomly assigned to each of the 24 permutations of these factors. Person A is attending a party at a neighbor’s house. The neighbor has many [Muslim/Anglo-Australian] friends who are present. Person A decides to join in with the people dancing in the living room. While dancing, Person B, [a Muslim/an Anglo-Australian] [female/male] partygoer, steps on Person A’s toes. Person A [angrily/jokingly] says to Person B, “You [pig/rat/ape], have you got two left feet?” After reading the vignette participants rated the animal expression on three items assessing its offensiveness (e.g., “How offensive is the animal expression in this situation?”; “How insulting is the animal expression in this situation?”; “How inappropriate is Person A’s use of the animal expression in this situation?”) on a scale from 1 = Not at all (offensive/insulting/inappropriate) to 7 = Extremely (offensive/ insulting/ inappropriate). These items were summed (α = .88). Participants then rated 24 traits on the extent to which the speaker implied that the target possessed them (“Consider each of the characteristics listed below, and rate the extent to which Person A is implying that Person B has it”), using a scale from 1 = Not at all to 7 = Very much so. These traits had been validated in previous research (Haslam & Bain, 2007; Haslam, Bain, Douge, Lee, & Bastian, 2005) as varying factorially on desirability and perceived uniqueness to humans (UH) relative to animals (i.e., 6 were high UH and desirable, 6 were low UH and desirable, etc.). A dehumanization index was created by subtracting the ratings of the 12 high UH from the 12 low UH traits. Positive index scores indicate that animalistic traits are seen as more characteristic of the target than uniquely human traits. Procedure. Participants completed the study in small groups in a laboratory setting, under the supervision of a researcher and completed the task in 10 minutes or less. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 10 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) Results A four-way ANOVA revealed main effects for all four factors on the offensiveness index. Metaphor was significant, F(2, 48) = 4.99, p < .05, η2 = .09, with post hoc tests indicating that “pig” (M = 15.46, SD = 3.56) was judged more offensive than “ape” (M = 12.33, SD = 5.07) and “rat” (M = 12.67, SD = 5.14), both ps < .05. Hostile tone (M = 15.08, SD = 3.72) was more offensive than jocular tone (M = 11.89, SD = 5.25), F(1, 48) = 12.97, p = .001, η2 = .11. Offensiveness was greater when the target was female (M = 14.56, SD = 5.01) than male (M = 12.42, SD = 4.38), F(1, 48) = 5.81, p < .05, η2 = .05, and when the context was intergroup (M = 15.19, SD = 4.20) rather than intragroup (M = 11.78, SD = 4.79), F(1, 48) = 4.83, p < .001, η2 = .13. There was a significant two-way interaction between tone and group context, F(1, 48) = 4.14, p < .05, η2 = .04, and a significant three-way interaction for metaphor, tone, and group context, F(2, 48) = 3.22, p < .05, η2 = .06. An examination of the twoway interaction indicated that jocular tone only produced lesser offensiveness when an animal metaphor was used in an intragroup context. The three-way interaction reflected a tendency for the “rat” metaphor to have especially reduced offensiveness when expressed in a jocular way to an in-group member. The dehumanization and offensiveness indices were positively correlated (r =.29, p < .05 across individuals; r = .55, p < .01 across vignette versions; see Table 3). Thus, metaphor expressions that implied a denial of uniquely human attributes to the target were judged particularly offensive. Notably, however, only 10 of the 24 vignette versions yielded positive dehumanization index scores, indicating that in most cases the animal metaphor was seen as implying uniquely human traits rather than animalistic traits. Discussion Study 2 demonstrates that the offensiveness of an animal metaphor is not simply intrinsic to the metaphor itself but also depends on the context in which it is expressed. Animal metaphors tend to be more offensive when addressed in a hostile manner to female targets and in intergroup contexts. People may judge animal metaphors to be less socially appropriate when expressed toward people who are stereotyped as more pure and needful of protection (Glick & Fiske, 1997) or more sensitive to offensive language (i.e., women; Jay & Janschewitz, 2008) and when they express or potentially inflame intergroup tensions. Animal metaphors may be seen as invariably hostile and insulting when expressed toward out-group members, a possibility consistent with the interaction between group context and tone. In contrast, when animal metaphors are expressed toward in-group members, they may be seen as jocular by default, unless a hostile tone is used. This possibility is consistent with the higher-order interaction effect, which suggests that certain otherwise offensive animal metaphors may be acceptable jocular terms among in-group members. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 11 Haslam et al. Table 3. Mean (SD) Offensiveness and Dehumanization Indices by Vignette Version, Study 2 Target Animal Pig Rat Ape Tone Gender Group Offensiveness Dehumanization Hostile Female In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group In-group Out-group 15.33 (2.08) 18.00 (3.61) 14.00 (4.36) 15.33 (0.57) 14.67 (3.14) 16.67 (2.73) 11.67 (3.21) 17.00 (1.73) 15.67 (3.21) 16.67 (5.85) 13.67 (4.16) 11.33 (5.51) 5.33 (2.52) 16.33 (2.08) 8.00 (4.36) 14.33 (1.53) 15.33 (2.52) 19.00 (2.00) 11.67 (3.51) 15.00 (2.65) 11.00 (6.08) 9.67 (3.06) 6.00 (3.61) 11.00 (6.25) −2.00 +4.33 −0.33 +0.33 +4.67 −4.33 +1.00 +5.67 −0.67 +6.00 −9.00 −7.00 −3.00 −1.67 −3.33 −6.67 +0.67 +0.33 +0.67 +0.67 −3.33 −8.00 −8.67 −2.00 Male Jocular Female Male Hostile Female Male Jocular Female Male Hostile Female Male Jocular Female Male The offensiveness of animal metaphors appears to be influenced by a variety of contextual factors, but it also seems to be explained in part by the kinds of attributes that they implicitly ascribe to their targets. Variations in the perceived offensiveness of the metaphors across versions of the vignette were associated with the degree to which uniquely human attributes were denied to the target. By implication, animal metaphors may be insulting in part because they are—or are intended to be—dehumanizing. General Discussion The two studies offer complementary views of animal metaphors. Study 1 demonstrates that different metaphors convey different meanings, and these meanings are associated with their offensiveness. Study 2 demonstrates that contextual factors also Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 12 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) influence offensiveness. Evidently, offensiveness is both intrinsic to particular metaphors and determined by contexts of use. One finding that connects both studies is the importance of dehumanization. Both studies show that calling people animals is not invariably dehumanizing. Many animal metaphors were not seen as denying the target’s humanity, especially when they were expressed toward in-group members and in a jocular manner. However, both studies also showed that metaphors were offensive to the degree that they implied a view of the target as less than human. This conclusion is novel in psychological research. Although the use of explicit animal metaphors has been prominent in historical instances of dehumanization, dehumanization research has focused instead on the denial of particular human characteristics to out-groups (e.g., Leyens et al., 2003) or on implicit associations between the groups and animals (e.g., Goff et al., 2008). These subtle and nonconscious forms of dehumanization are important bases for prejudice and intergroup conflict, but sometimes dehumanizing perceptions are manifested blatantly in language. Studying the use of animal metaphors in discourse may be a promising approach to investigating dehumanization that can be pursued outside the lab and in the thick of real conflicts. Our joint content-and-context approach may help illuminate two celebrated events involving animal metaphors. In the 2008 U.S. presidential race then-Governor Palin took offence when then-Senator Obama indirectly likened her to a pig (albeit humanized by lipstick). Obama himself was seemingly likened to a chimpanzee by a New York Post cartoonist. Both events ignited firestorms of controversy. Study 1 indicates that both metaphors are among the most offensive in content but for different reasons. Pigs are taboo creatures, and as metaphors they imply that a person is repulsive and morally depraved. However, the pig metaphor was not judged to be dehumanizing, suggesting that the metaphor’s offensiveness primarily reflects a transfer of dislike from the animal to the person rather than a denial of the person’s humanity. Chimpanzees, in contrast, were not disliked, but they were judged the most dehumanizing metaphor of all. Likening Obama to a chimpanzee was therefore a direct disavowal of his humanness rather than a transfer of disliked qualities. Contextual factors also played their part. Both metaphors were expressed by people presumed to be politically hostile to the respective targets, a sensitizing factor that Study 2 showed to be linked to greater offensiveness. The historical context of depictions of African Americans as ape-like (Goff et al., 2008) also contributed to the offensiveness of the Obama cartoon, and was one reason why it generated more heat than the many simian depictions of George Bush. The gender context of the Palin-pig metaphor also exacerbated its offensiveness: animal metaphors appear to be more offensive in general when expressed toward female targets, and this may be especially so for a metaphor that casts aspersions on a person’s appearance. Our studies only scratch the surface of the psychological study of animal metaphors. Many additional content and contextual factors are likely to influence the offensiveness of animal metaphors and the vocabulary and determinants of metaphor use Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 13 Haslam et al. will vary widely across cultures. Nevertheless, the present work indicates that animal metaphors offer a revealing window into human prejudice and social judgment. Acknowledgement The authors would like to thank the editor and three anonymous reviewers for their generous and helpful feedback on earlier versions of the article. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The research reported in this article was supported by Grant DP0771200 from the Australian Research Council to Nick Haslam and Paul Bain. References Bell-Fialkoff, A. (1996). Ethnic cleansing. New York, NY: St. Martins Press. Benet-Martinez, V., & Waller, N. G. (2002). From adorable to worthless: Implicit and selfreport structure of highly evaluative personality descriptors. European Journal of Personality, 16, 1-41. Berger, J. (1980). Why look at animals? New York, NY: Pantheon Books. Boccato, G., Capozza, D., Falvo, R., & Durante, F. (2008). The missing link: Ingroup, outgroup and the human species. Social Cognition, 26, 224-234. Brandes, S. (1984). Animal metaphors and social control in Tzintzuntzan. Ethnology, 23, 207-215. Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864-886. Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2006). Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation. Mind & Language, 21, 434-458. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1997). Hostile and benevolent sexism: Measuring ambivalent sexist attitudes towards women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 119-135. Goatly, A. (2006). Humans, animals, and metaphors. Society & Animals, 14, 15-37. Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M. J., & Jackson, M. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 292-306. Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Kluck, B., & Cornwell, R. (2001). I am not an animal: Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 427-435. Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 252-264. Haslam, N., & Bain, P. (2007). Humanizing the self: Moderators of the attribution of lesser humanness to others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 57-68. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 14 Journal of Language and Social Psychology XX(X) Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you: Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 937-950. Holtgraves, T. M., & Kashima, Y. (2008). Language, meaning, and social cognition. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 73-94. Hornsey, M. J., Oppes, T., & Svensson, A. (2002). “It’s ok if we say it, but you can’t”: Responses to intergroup and intragroup criticism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 293-307. Hsieh, S. C. (2006). A corpus-based study on animal expressions is Mandarin Chinese and German. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 2206-2222. Jay, T. (2009). The utility and ubiquity of taboo words. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 153-161. Jay, T., & Janschewitz, K. (2008). The pragmatics of swearing. Journal of Politeness Research, 4, 267-288. Kövecses, Z. (2002). Metaphor: A practical introduction. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Lakoff, G., & Turner, M. (1989). More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Leach, E. R. (1964). Anthropological aspects of language: Animal categories and verbal abuse. In E. H. Lenneberg (Ed.), New dimensions in the study of language (pp. 23-64). Cambridge: MIT Press. Lévi-Strauss, C. (1963). Totemism. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Leyens, J.-P., Cortes, B., Demoulin, S., Dovidio, J. F., Fiske, S. T., Gaunt, R., . . . Vaes, J. (2003). Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism: The 2002 Tajfel lecture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 703-717. Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2007). Animals and androids: Implicit associations between social categories and nonhumans. Psychological Science, 18, 116-121. Norrick, N. R. (2003). Issues in conversational joking. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 1333-1359. Rakusan, J. (2004). Cultural diversity in crossing the boundaries between human and animal in language—Germanic and Slavic similes and metaphors. Collegium Antropologicum, 28, 171-181. Rice, D. R., Abrams, D., Badea, C., Bohner, G., Carnaghi, A., Dementi, L. I., . . . Trzebinski, J. (2010). What did you just call me? European and American ratings of the valence of ethnophaulisms. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29, 117-131. Ritchie, L. D. (2009). Relevance and simulation in metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 249-262. Saminaden, A., Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2010). Afterimages of savages: Implicit associations between “primitives”, animals and children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 91-105. Sutton, R. M., Elder, T. J., & Douglas, K. M. (2006). Reactions to internal and external criticism of outgroups: Social convention in the intergroup sensitivity effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 563-575. Talebinejad, M. R., & Dastjerdi, H. V. (2005). A cross-cultural study of animals metaphors: When owls are not wise! Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 133-150. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011 15 Haslam et al. Van Oudenhoven, J. P., de Raad, B., Askevis-Leherpeux, F., Boski, P., Brunborg, G. S., Carmona, C., . . . Woods, S. (2008). Terms of abuse as expression and reinforcement of cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 174-185. Viki, G. T., Winchester, L., Titshall, L., Chisango, T., Pina, A., & Russell, R. (2006). Beyond secondary emotions: The infrahumanization of outgroups using human-related and animalrelated words. Social Cognition, 24, 753-775. Wilson, D., & Carston, R. (2006). Metaphor, relevance and the “emergent property” issue. Mind & Language, 21, 404-433. Bios Nick Haslam is a professor in the Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Australia. His research interests include psychological essentialism, psychiatric classification, and dehumanization. Steve Loughnan is a research associate in the Department of Psychology at the University of Kent. His research focuses on dehumanization processes, biases in self-perception, and the role of metaphor in social perception. Pamela Sun received her bachelor of arts with honors in psychology at the University of Melbourne in 2009, with a thesis on the psychology of animal metaphors. She now works in adolescent mental health. Downloaded from jls.sagepub.com at University of Melbourne Library on June 15, 2011