Psychology, Second Edition Copyright © 1986, 1989 by Worth Publishers, Inc. All rights reserved Manufactured in the United States of America Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 88-50722 ISBN: 0-87901-400-8 4 5 - 93 92 91 90 89 Editors: Anne Vinnicombe and Deborah Posner Production: Barbara Anne Seixas Design: Malcolm Grear Designers Art director: George Touloumes Layout design: Patricia Lawson and David Lopez Illustrators: Warren Budd and Demetrios Zangos Picture editors: Elaine Bernstein and David Hinchman Cover design: Demetrios Zangos Composition: York Graphic Services, Inc. Printing and binding: R. R. Donnelley & Sons Company Illustration credits begin on page IC-1, and constitute an extension of the copyright page. Worth Publishers, Inc. 33 Irving Place New York, New York 10003 Preface My goals in writing this book can be reduced to one overriding aim: to merge rigorous science with a broad human perspective in a book that engages both the mind and the heart. I wanted to set forth clearly the principles and processes of psychology and at the same time to remain sensitive to students' interests and to their futures as individuals. My aim was a book that helps students to gain insight into the phenomena of their everyday lives, to feel a sense of wonder about seemingly ordinary human processes, and to see how psychology addresses deep intellec­ tual issues that cross disciplines. I also wanted to produce a book that conveys to its readers the inquisitive, compassionate, and sometimes playful spirit in which psychology can be approached. Believing with Thoreau that "Anything living is easily and naturally expressed in pop­ ular language," I sought to communicate scholarship with crisp narra­ tive and vivid story telling. To achieve these goals, I established, and have steadfastly tried to follow, eight principles: 1. To exemplify the process of inquiry. The student is repeatedly shown not just the outcome of research, but how the research process works. Throughout, the book tries to excite the read­ er's curiosity. It invites readers to imagine themselves as par­ ticipants in classic experiments. Several chapters introduce re­ search stories as mysteries that are progressively unraveled as one clue after another is put into place. 2. To teach critical thinking. By presenting research as intellectual detective work, I have tried to exemplify an inquiring, analyti­ cal mind set. The reader will discover how an empirical ap­ proach can help evaluate competing ideas and claims for highly publicized phenomena ranging from subliminal persua­ sion, ESP, and mother-infant bonding to astrology, basketball streak shooting, and hypnotic age regression. And whether they are studying memory, cognition, or statistics, students learn principles of critical reasoning. 3. To put facts in the service of concepts. My intention has been not to fill students' intellectual file drawers with facts but to reveal psychology's major concepts. In each chapter I have placed the greater emphasis on the concepts that students should carry with them long after they have forgotten the details of what they have read. 4. To be as up-to-date as possible. Few things dampen students' interest as quickly as the sense that they are reading stale news. While doing justice to the classic contributions of prior years, I therefore sought to present the most important recent xii Preface developments in the discipline. Accordingly, 73 percent of the references in this edition are from the 1980s, and 47 percent of these were published between 1986 and 1989. 5. To integrate principles and applications. Throughout—by means of anecdotes, case histories, and the posing of hypothetical situations—I have tried to relate the findings of basic research to their applications and implications. Where psychology can illuminate pressing human issues—be they racism and sexism, health and happiness, or violence and war—I have not hesi­ tated to shine its light. 6. To enhance comprehension by providing continuity. Many chap­ ters have a significant issue or theme that links the subtopics, forming a thread through the chapter. The "Learning" chap­ ter, for example, conveys the idea that bold thinkers (Pavlov, Skinner, Bandura) can serve as intellectual pioneers. The "Thinking and Language" chapter raises the issue of human rationality and irrationality. Other threads, such as the naturenurture issue, weave throughout the whole book. 7. To reinforce learning at every step. Everyday examples and rhe­ torical questions encourage students to process the material actively. Concepts are frequently applied and reinforced in later chapters. Pedagogical aids in the margins augment learn­ ing without diluting or interrupting the text narrative. Each chapter concludes with a narrative summary, a glossary of de­ fined key terms, and suggested readings that are attuned to students' interests and abilities. 8. To provide organizational flexibility. I have chosen an organiza­ tion in which developmental psychology is covered early be­ cause students usually find the material of particular interest and because it introduces themes and concepts that are used later in the text. Nevertheless, many instructors will have their own preferred sequence. Thus the chapters were written to anticipate other approaches to organizing the introductory course. Statistics, for example, is covered in an appendix, thus facilitating its being covered at any time. S E C O N D EDITION FEATURES This new edition retains the first edition's basic organization, format, and voice. It is, however, thoroughly updated (more than 40 percent of the references are new to this edition), painstakingly revised and polished paragraph by paragraph, and there are dozens of fresh examples. Without watering down the content, new learning aids summarize or diagram difficult concepts. Research related to biopsychology, gender, and industrial/organizational psychology is inte­ grated throughout the text, with a cross-reference guide to these topics at the ends of the chapters on biological foundations, gender, and motivation. There are numerous changes within chapters: To increase the appeal of biological psychology, Chapter 2—formerly the longest and most arduous chapter—has been shortened by moving material on evolution and behavior genetics to later chap­ ters. Without reducing overall coverage of biological foundations, this reduces redundant coverage and enables students to get more quickly to inherently interesting neuroscience research. Chapter 3 treats infancy and childhood within a single section, lending more coherence to its coverage of physical, cognitive, and so­ cial development. Chapter 4 similarly unifies the coverage of adult­ hood and aging. Now, for example, we explore changes in memory, intelligence, and other traits across all of the adult years. Chapter 5, Gender, is reorganized, and now emphasizes more strongly the social construction of gender. Among the major changes in other chapters you will find new material on the biology of memory, on the biological and cognitive underpinnings of depression, on teen sexual activity and pregnancy, on the psychological effects of AIDS, on well-being across the life span, on drugs and behavior, and on managerial motivation. Coverage of obesity has been moved to the chapter on health, pain to the chapter on sensation, and the ethics of animal research are now discussed in the introductory chapter. The discussion of TV, pornography, and ag­ gression has been carefully revised and updated. SUPPLEMENTS Psychology is accompanied by a comprehensive and widely acclaimed teaching and learning package. For students, there is a successful study guide, Discovering Psychology, prepared by Richard Straub (University of Michigan, Dearborn). Using the SQ3R (study, question, read, recite, review) method, each chapter contains overviews, guided study and review questions, and progress tests. In this new edition, all answers to test questions are explained and page-referenced— enabling students to know why each possible answer is right or wrong. Discovering Psychology is available as a paperback and Diskcovering Psychology is a microcomputer version for use on IBM PC, Macintosh, or Apple II. The masterfully improved computer software prepared by Thomas Ludwig (Hope College) brings some of psychology's concepts and methods to life. PsychSim II: Computer Simulations in Psychology contains eleven revised programs and five new ones for use on the IBM PC or true compatibles, Macintosh, and the Apple II family. Some simulations engage the student as experimenter, by conditioning a rat or electrically probing the hypothalamus. Others engage the student as subject, as when responding to tests of memory or visual illusions. Still others provide a dynamic tutorial/demonstration of, for example, hemispheric processing or cognitive development principles. Student worksheets are provided. The Instructor's Resources, created by Martin Bolt (Calvin College), have been acclaimed by users everywhere. The resources include ideas for organizing the course, chapter objectives, lecture/discussion topics, classroom exercises, student projects, film suggestions, and ready-touse handouts for student participation. The new resources package is 30 percent bigger, now includes approximately 140 transparencies, and offers many new demonstration handouts. The Test Bank, by John Brink (Calvin College), builds upon the first edition Test Bank, which was written and edited by Brink with the able assistance of Martin Bolt, Nancy Campbell-Goymer (BirminghamSouthern College), James Eison (Southeast Missouri State University), and Anne Nowlin (Roane State Community College). The new edition includes definitional/factual questions, more conceptual questions than previous versions, and adds a new section of essay questions. All questions are keyed to learning objectives and are page-referenced to xiv Preface the textbook. The Test Bank questions are available on Computest, a user-friendly computerized test-generation system for IBM PC, Macin­ tosh, and the Apple II family of microcomputers. IN APPRECIATION Aided by nearly 150 consultants and reviewers over the last six years, this has become a far better, more accurate book than one author alone (this author, at least) could have written. It gives me pleasure, there­ fore, to thank, and to exonerate from blame, the esteemed colleagues who contributed criticisms, corrections, and creative ideas. This new edition benefited from careful reviews at several stages. Nearly a thousand students at seven colleges and universities critiqued the first edition. Fourteen sensitive and knowledgeable teachers of­ fered their page-by-page critique of the first edition after using it in class. This resulted in hundreds of small and large improvements, for which I am indebted to: Lisa J. Bishop, Indiana State University Laurie Braidwood, Indiana State University William Buskist, Auburn University Thomas H. Carr, Michigan State University-East Lansing Richard N. Ek, Corning Community College Roberta A. Eveslage, ]ohnson County Community College Larry Gregory, New Mexico State University Michael McCall, Monroe Community College-Rochester James A. Polyson, University of Richmond Donis Price, Mesa Community College Walter Swap, Tufts University Linda L. Walsh, University of Northern Iowa Rita Wicks-Nelson, West Virginia Institute of Technology Mary Lou Zanich, Indiana University of Pennsylvania Additional reviewers critiqued individual first edition chapters and/or successive second edition drafts. For their generous help and countless good ideas, I thank: David Barkmeier, Bunker Hill Community College John B. Best, Eastern Illinois University Martin Bolt, Calvin College Richard Bowen, Loyola University of Chicago Kenneth S. Bowers, University of Waterloo David W. Brokaw, Azusa Pacific University Freda Rebelsky Camp, Boston University Linda Camras, DcPaul University Bernardo J. Carducci, Indiana University Southeast-New Albany Dennis Clare, College of San Mateo Timothy DeVoogd, Cornell University Alice A. Eagly, Purdue University Mary Frances Farkas, Lansing Community College Larry Gregory, New Mexico State University Richard A. Griggs, University of Florida-Gainesville Joseph H. Grosslight, Florida State University-Tallahassee Diane F. Halpern, California State University-San Bernardino Janet Shibley Hyde, University of Wisconsin-Madison Mary Jasnoski, George Washington University Carl Merle Johnson, Central Michigan University John Kounios, Tufts University Robert M. Levy, Indiana State University T. C. Lewandowski, Delaware County College A. W. Logue, State University of New York-Stony Brook Nancy Maloney, Vancouver William Siegfried, John Simpson, Donald H. McBurney, Shelly E. Taylor, University University of North Carolina-Charlotte Community College-hangar a Campus Matthew Margres, Saginaw Valley State University University of Washington-Seattle University of California-Los Angeles of Pittsburgh Donna Wood McCarty, Clayton Ross A. Thompson, State College Mark McDaniel, Nebraska-Lincoln Purdue University Elizabeth C. McDonel, University of Alabama-Tuscaloosa Douglas Mook, University of Virginia James A. Polyson, University of Janet J. Turnage, University of University of Central Florida Ko Vandonselaar, University of Saskatchewan Nancy J. Vye, Vanderbilt University Mary Roth Walsh, University of Richmond Oakley Ray, Vanderbilt University Lowell Duane M. Rumbaugh, Florida Rita Wicks-Nelson, West Virginia Institute of Technology Georgia State University Neil Salkind, University of Kansas Nancy L. Segal, Wilse B. Webb, University of University of Minnesota In preparing the first edition, consultants helped me reflect the most current thinking in their specialties, and expert reviewers critiqued the various chapter drafts. Because the result of their guid­ ance is carried forward into this new edition, I remain indebted to: T. John Akamatsu, Kent State University Harry H. Avis, Sierra College Richard B. Day, University McMaster Edward L. Deci, University of Bernard J. Baars, The Wright Rochester Institute John K. Bare, Carleton College Timothy DeVoogd, Jonathan Baron, University of Pennsylvania Andrew Baum, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences Cornell University David Foulkes, Georgia Mental Health Institute and Emory University Larry H. Fujinaka, Leeward Community College Kathleen Stassen Berger, Bronx Robert J. Gatchel, Community College, City University of New York Allen E. Bergin, Brigham Young University Texas Southwestern Medical CenterDallas Mary Gauvain, Oregon State University George D. Bishop, University of Alan G. Glaros, University of University of Texas-San Antonio Missouri-Kansas City Douglas W. Bloomquist, Judith P. Goggin, Framingham State College Texas-El Paso Kenneth S. Bowers, Marvin R. Goldfried, University of State University of New York-Stony Brook Waterloo Robert M. Boynton, University of California-San Diego Ross Buck, University of University of Connecticut Timothy P. Carmody, San Francisco Veterans Administration Medical Center Stanley Coren, University of British Columbia Donald Cronkite, Hope College Peter W. Culicover, The Ohio State University William T. Greenough, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Joseph H. Grosslight, Florida State University-Tallahassee James V. Hinrichs, University of Iowa Douglas Hintzman, University of Oregon Nils Hovik, Lehigh County Community College I. M. Hulicka, State University College-Buffalo xvi Preface Janet Shibley Hyde, University of Wisconsin-Madison Carroll E. Izard, University of Delaware John Jung, California State University-Long Beach John F. Kihlstrom, University of Arizona Kathleen Kowal, University of Alexander J. Rosen, University of Illinois at Chicago Circle Kay F. Schaffer, University of Toledo Alexander W. Siegel, University of Houston Ronald K. Siegel, School of Medicine at the University of California-Los Angeles North Carolina-Wilmington Donald P. Spence, Richard E. Mayer, Medicine at Rutgers The State University of New Jersey University of California-Santa Barbara Donald H. McBurney, University Richard A. Steffy, Vanderbilt Waterloo Leonard Stern, Eastern Washington University University Robert J. Sternberg, of Pittsburgh Timothy P. McNamara, University Donald Meichenbaum, School of of Waterloo University of Yale University Donald H. Mershon, North Carolina State University Carol A. Nowak, Center for the Study of Aging, State University of New York-Buffalo Anne Nowlin, Roane State Community College Jacob L. Orlofsky, University of Missouri-St. Louis Willis F. Overton, Temple George C. Stone, Richard D. Walk, Florida Joseph J. Palladino, Merold Westphal, University of Ovide F. Pomerleau, School of Medicine at the University of MichiganAnn Arbor Dennis R. Proffitt, University of Virginia Judith Rodin, Yale University George Washington University George Weaver, Florida State University University Daniel J. Ozer, Boston University Southern Indiana Herbert L. Petri, Towson State University Robert Plutchik, Albert Einstein College of Medicine University of California-San Francisco Elliot Tanis, Hope College Don Tucker, University of Oregon Rhoda K. Unger, Montclair State College Wilse B. Webb, University of Fordham University David A. Wilder, Rutgers The State University of New Jersey Joan Wilterdink, University of Wisconsin-Madison Jeffrey J. Wine, Stanford University Joseph Wolpe, The Medical College of Pennsylvania, Eastern Pennsylvania Psychiatric Institute Gordon Wood, Michigan State University Fourteen individuals read the whole first edition manuscript and provided me not only with a critique of each chapter but also with their sense of the style and balance of the whole book. For their advice and warm encouragement, I am grateful to: John B. Best, Eastern Illinois University Martin Bolt, Calvin College Cynthia J. Brandau, Belleville Area College Sharon S. Brehm, University of Kansas Steven L. Buck, University of Washington James Eison, Southeast Missouri State University Robert M. Levy, Indiana State University G. William Lucker, University of Texas-El Paso Angela P. McGlynn, Mercer County Community College Carol Myers, Holland, Michigan Bobby J. Poe, Belleville Area College Catherine A. Riordan, University of Missouri-Rolla Richard Straub, University of Michigan-Dearborn Robert B. Wallace, Hartford University of Preface Through both editions, I have benefited from the meticulous cri­ tique, probing questions, and constant encouragement of Charles Brewer (Furman University). At Worth Publishers—a company whose entire staff is devoted to the highest quality in everything they do—a host of people played key supportive roles. Alison Meersschaert commissioned this book, envi­ sioned its goals and a process to fulfill them, and nurtured the book nearly to the end of the draft first edition. I am also indebted to Manag­ ing Editor Anne Vinnicombe, leader of a dedicated editorial team, for her prodigious effort in bringing both editions to fulfillment and metic­ ulously scrutinizing the accuracy, logical flow, and clarity of every page. For this edition, developmental editor Barbara Brooks's careful analysis and thoughtful suggestions improved every chapter. Debbie Posner demonstrated exceptional commitment and competence in co­ ordinating the transformation of manuscript into book, as did Chris­ tine Brune, who supervised countless editorial details. Thanks also go to Worth's production team, led by George Touloumes, for once again crafting a final product that exceeds my expectations. At Hope College, the supporting team members for this edition included Julia Zuwerink, Wendy Braje, and Richard Burtt, who re­ searched, checked, and proofed countless items; Kathy Adamski, who typed hundreds of dictated letters without ever losing her good cheer; Phyllis and Richard Vandervelde, who processed thousands of pages of various chapter drafts with their customary excellence; and my psy­ chology colleagues, Les Beach, Jane Dickie, Charles Green, Thomas Ludwig, James Motiff, Patricia Roehling, John Shaughnessy, and Phil­ lip Van Eyl, whose knowledge and personal libraries I have tapped on hundreds of occasions. The influence of my writing coach, poet-essay­ ist Jack Ridl, continues to be evident in the voice you will be hearing in the pages that follow. XVI Contents in Brief PART 1 PART Foundations of Psychology CHAPTER CHAPTER 2 2 CHAPTER 24 CHAPTER PART 3 CHAPTER 6 CHAPTER 5 88 16 CHAPTER 3 CHAPTER 474 18 Health Experiencing the World 506 136 6 Sensation PART 138 7 Social Behavior CHAPTER CHAPTER 19 168 Social Influence 8 CHAPTER States of C o n s c i o u s n e s s PART 192 4 254 610 G-1 REFERENCES R-0 ILLUSTRATION CREDITS NAME NI-0 INDEX SUBJECT INDEX IC-1 SI—1 11 Thinking and L a n g u a g e CHAPTER 574 Statistical Reasoning in Everyday Life GLOSSARY 228 10 Memory CHAPTER 20 Social Relations 226 9 Learning CHAPTER 546 APPENDIX Learning and Thinking CHAPTER 544 7 Perception CHAPTER 442 17 Therapy CHAPTER PART 408 Psychological Disorders 116 406 15 Personality 4 Gender 380 Personality, Disorder, and Weil-Being 54 56 Adolescence and Adulthood CHAPTER 14 CHAPTER The Developing Child 348 Emotion 2 Development over the Life Span 346 13 Motivation Biological R o o t s of B e h a v i o r PART 5 Motivation and Emotion 1 Introducing P s y c h o l o g y CHAPTER xxviii 282 12 Intelligence 314 xix Contents PART 2 Development over the Life Span CHAPTER 3 The Developing Child DEVELOPMENTAL ISSUES 57 Genes or Experience? 58 Stages? 58 Continuity or Stability or Change? 59 PRENATAL DEVELOPMENT NEWBORN 59 From Life Comes Life 59 AND THE The Competent Newborn 61 INFANCY AND CHILDHOOD Physical Development 62 Development 65 Box 62 Cognitive Social Development 70 Father Care 73 REFLECTIONS ON ISSUE: AND GENES Temperament 80 A DEVELOPMENTAL EXPERIENCE Studies of Twins 81 80 xxii Contents Adoption Studies 83 How Much Credit (or Blame) Do Parents Deserve? 84 Summing Up 85 Terms and Concepts to Remember 86 For Further Reading 87 CHAPTER 4 Adolescence and Adulthood ADOLESCENCE Physical Development 90 Development 91 ADULTHOOD AND Development 102 ISSUES Cognitive Social Development 95 AGING Physical Development 98 REFLECTIONS 88 89 98 Cognitive Social Development 104 ON DEVELOPMENTAL 111 Continuity and Stages 111 Change 112 Stability and Summing Up 114 Terms and Concepts to Remember 115 For Further Reading 115 CHAPTER 5 Gender 116 BIOLOGICAL GENDER: IS INFLUENCES ON BIOLOGY DESTINY? 118 Hormones 118 Sociobiology: Doing What Comes Naturally? 119 THE SOCIAL GENDER 121 CONSTRUCTION Theories of Gender-Typing 121 Roles 124 GENDER OF Gender Nature-Nurture Interaction 128 DIFFERENCES 128 The Politics of Studying Gender Differences 129 How Do Males and Females Differ? 129 Summing Up 134 Terms and Concepts to Remember 134 For Further Reading 135 The Developing Child In mid-1978, the newest astonishment in medicine, covering all the front pages, was the birth of an English baby nine months after conception in a dish. The older surprise, which should still be fazing us all, is that a soli­ tary sperm and a single egg can fuse and become a human being under any circumstance, and that, however implanted, [this multiplied] cell af­ fixed to the uterine wall will grow and differentiate into eight pounds of baby; this has been going on under our eyes for so long a time that we've gotten used to it; hence the outcries of amazement at this really minor technical modification of the general procedure—nothing much, really, beyond relocating the beginning of the process from the fallopian tube to a plastic container. Lewis Thomas, The Medusa and Snail, 1979 The developing child is no less a wonder after birth than in the womb. As we journey through life from womb to tomb, when and how do we change? As psychologists Clyde Kluckhohn and Henry Murray (1956) noted, each person develops in certain respects like all other persons, like some other persons, and like no other persons. Usually, our atten­ tion is drawn to the ways in which we are unique. But to developmen­ tal psychologists our commonalities are as important as our unique­ nesses. Virtually all of us—Michelangelo, Queen Elizabeth, Martin Luther King, Jr., you, and I—began walking around age 1, talking by age 2, and as children we engaged in social play in preparation for life's serious work. We all smile and cry, love and hate, and occasionally ponder the fact that someday we will die. As preparation for consider­ ing both the similarities and differences in our physical, cognitive, and social development, let us first confront three overriding developmen­ tal issues. DEVELOPMENTAL I S S U E S Three major issues pervade developmental psychology: 1. How much is our development influenced by our genetic in­ heritance and how much by our experience? 2. Is development a gradual, continuous process, or does it pro­ ceed through a sequence of separate stages? In male or female, y o u n g or old, d e v e l o p ­ ment Is a process of physical, mental, a n d 3. Do our individual traits persist or do we become different per­ sons as we age? social g r o w t h that continues t h r o u g h o u t life. 57 58 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Let's briefly consider each of these issues and then return to them later in this and the next chapter. GENES OR EXPERIENCE? Our genes are the biochemical units of heredity that make each of us a distinctive human being. The genes we share are what make us people rather than dogs or tulips. But might our individual genetic makeups explain why one person is outgoing, another shy, or why one person is slow-witted and another smart? Questions like these raise an issue of profound importance: Are we influenced more by our genes or by our life experience? This nature-nurture (or genes-experience) debate has long been one of psychology's chief concerns. Is behavior, like eye color, pretty much fixed, or is it changeable? Our answer affects how we view cer­ tain social policies. Suppose that you presume people are the way they are "by nature." You probably will not have much faith in programs that try to rehabilitate prisoners or compensate for educational disad­ vantage. And you will probably agree with developmental psycholo­ gists who emphasize the influence of our genes. As a flower unfolds in accord with its genetic blueprint, so our genes design an orderly se­ quence of biological growth processes called maturation. Maturation decrees many of our commonalities: standing before walking, using nouns before adjectives. Although extreme deprivation or abuse will retard development, the genetic growth tendencies are inherent. If you take the nurture side of the debate, you probably will agree with the developmentalists who emphasize external influences. As a potter shapes a lump of clay, our experiences are presumed to shape us. This view was argued by the seventeenth-century philosopher John Locke, who proposed that at birth the child in some ways is an empty page on which experience writes its story. Although few today wholeheartedly support Locke's proposition, research provides many examples of nurture's effects. In reality, nearly everyone agrees that our behaviors are a product of the interaction of (1) our genes, (2) our past experience, and (3) the present situation to which we are responding. Moreover, these factors sometimes interact. For example, if an attractive, athletic teenage boy has been treated as a leader and is now sought out by girls, shall we say his positive self-image is due to his genes or his environment? It's both, because his environment is reacting to his genetic endowment. Asking which factor is more important is like asking whether the area of a football field is due more to its length or to its width. CONTINUITY OR More than 98 percent of our genes are identical to those of c h i m p a n z e e s . Not surprisingly, the physiological systems and even the brain organizations of humans a n d c h i m p a n z e e s are quite similar. " M a t u r a t i o n (read the genetic p r o g r a m , largely) sets the course of development, which is modified by experience, espe­ cially if that experience is deviant from w h a t is normal for the s p e c i e s . " Sandra Scarr (1982) STAGES? Everyone agrees that adults are vastly different from infants. But do they differ as a giant redwood differs from its seedling—a difference created by gradual, cumulative growth? Or do they differ as a butterfly differs from a caterpillar—a difference of distinct stages? Generally speaking, researchers who emphasize experience and learning tend to see development as a slow, continuous shaping proc­ ess. Those who emphasize biological maturation tend to see develop­ ment as a sequence of genetically predetermined stages or steps. They believe that, depending on an individual's heredity and experiences, progress through the various stages may be quick or slow, but every­ one passes through the same stages in the same order. Nature or nurture? Stability or c h a n g e ? For e x a m p l e , h o w m u c h is this o u t g o i n g b a b y ' s temperament due to her heredity a n d h o w m u c h to her u p b r i n g i n g ? A n d h o w likely is it that she will be an o u t g o ­ ing adult? T h e s e questions define t w o fundamental issues of developmental psy­ chology. C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child STABILITY O R C H A N C E ? For most of this century, psychologists have taken the position that once a person's personality is formed, it hardens and usually remains set for life. Researchers who have followed lives through time are now in the midst of a spirited debate over the extent to which our past reaches into our future. Is development characterized more by stability over time or by change? Are the effects of early experience enduring or temporary? Will the cranky infant grow up to be an irritable adult, or is such a child just as likely to become a placid, patient person? Do the differences among classmates in, say, aggressiveness, aptitude, or strivings for achievement persist throughout the life span? In short, to what degree do we grow to be merely older versions of our early selves and to what degree do we become new persons? Most developmentalists today believe that for certain traits, such as basic temperament, there is an underlying continuity, especially in the years following early childhood. Yet, as we age we also change— physically, cognitively, socially. Thus we have today's life-span view: Human development is a lifelong process. PRENATAL D E V E L O P M E N T AND THE NEWBORN FROM L I F E C O M E S L I F E Nothing is more natural than a species reproducing itself. Yet nothing is more wondrous. Consider human reproduction: The process starts when a mature ovum (egg) is released by a woman's ovary and the some 300 million sperm deposited during intercourse begin their race upstream toward it. When a girl is born, she carries all the eggs she will ever have, although only a few will ever mature and be released. A boy, in contrast, begins producing sperm at puberty. The manufactur­ ing process continues 24 hours a day for the rest of his life, although the rate of production—over 1000 sperm a second—does slow down with age. New life is created when egg and sperm unite, and the twentythree chromosomes carried in the egg are paired with the twenty-three chromosomes brought to it by the sperm. These forty-six chromosomes contain the master plan for your body (Figure 3 - 1 ) . Each chromosome is composed of long threads of a molecule called DNA (deoxyribonu­ cleic acid). DNA in turn is made up of thousands of segments, called genes, that are capable of synthesizing specific proteins (the biochemi­ cal building blocks of life). Your sex is determined by the twenty-third pair of chromosomes, the sex chromosomes. The member of the pair that came from your mother was, invariably, an X chromosome. From your father, you had a fifty-fifty chance of receiving an X chromosome, making you a female, or a Y chromosome, making you a male. Biology has become so ad­ vanced that scientists have pinpointed the single tiny gene on the Y chromosome that seems responsible for throwing the master switch leading to the production of testosterone, and thus to maleness (Roberts, 1988). Like space voyagers approaching a huge planet, the sperm ap­ proach a cell 85,000 times bigger than themselves. The relatively few sperm that make it to the egg release digestive enzymes that eat away (0 Figure 3 - 1 T h e g e n e s : their location a n d composition. C o n t a i n e d i n the n u ­ cleus of each of the trillions of cells (a) in your body are c h r o m o s o m e s (b). Each c h r o m o s o m e is c o m p o s e d in part of the molecule D N A (c). G e n e s , which are s e g ­ ments of D N A , form templates for the production of proteins. By directing the manufacture of proteins, the g e n e s deter­ mine our individual biological development. 59 60 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n the egg's protective coating, allowing one sperm to penetrate (Figure 3 - 2 ) . As it does so, an electrical charge shoots across the ovum's sur­ face, blocking out other sperm during the minute or so that it takes the egg to form a barrier. Meanwhile, fingerlike projections sprout around the successful sperm and pull it inward. The egg nucleus and the sperm nucleus move toward each other and, before half a day has elapsed, they fuse. The two have become one. But even at that moment, when one lucky sperm has won the 1 in 300 million lottery, an individual's destiny is not assured. Fewer than half of fertilized eggs, called zygotes, survive beyond the first week (Grobstein, 1979), and only a fourth survive to birth (Diamond, 1986). If human life begins at conception, then most people die without being born. But for you and me good fortune prevailed. Beginning as one cell, each of us became two cells, then four—each cell just like the first. Then, within the first week, when this cell division had produced a zygote of approximately 100 cells, the cells began to differentiate—to specialize in structure and function. Within 2 weeks, the increasingly diverse cells became attached to the mother's uterine wall, beginning approximately 37 weeks of the closest human relationship (see Figure 3-3). During the ensuing 6 weeks, the developing human is called an embryo. In this embryonic period the organs begin to form and may begin to function: The heart begins to beat and the liver begins to make red blood cells. By the ninth week, the embryo has become unmistak­ ably human and is called a fetus. By the end of the sixth month, inter­ nal organs such as the stomach have become sufficiently formed and functional that they allow a prematurely born fetus a chance of sur­ vival. Nutrients and oxygen in the mother's blood pass through the pla­ centa into the blood of the fetus. If the mother is severely malnourished during the last third of the pregnancy, when the demand for nutrients reaches a peak, the baby may be born prematurely—or even be stillborn. (a) Figure 3 - 3 Prenatal development. (b) Figure 3 - 2 D e v e l o p m e n t begins w h e n a sperm unites with an e g g . T h e resulting z y g o t e is a single cell that, if all g o e s well, will become a 100-trillion-cell h u m a n being. Prenatal stages Zygote: Embryo: Fetus: conception to 2 weeks. 2 weeks through 8 weeks. 9 weeks to birth. (d) (c) the body is n o w b i g g e r than the head, fetus to the wall of the uterus and (a) T h e e m b r y o g r o w s and develops rap­ and the arms and legs have g r o w n n o ­ t h r o u g h which the fetus is nourished, is idly. At 40 d a y s , the spine is visible and ticeably, (c) By the end of the second clearly visible in this photo.) (d) As the the arms and legs are b e g i n n i n g to g r o w . month, w h e n the fetal period begins, fetus enters the fourth m o n t h , it w e i g h s (b) Five days later the e m b r y o ' s propor­ facial features, hands, and feet have a b o u t 3 ounces. tions have b e g u n to c h a n g e . T h e rest of f orm ed. (The placenta, which attaches the C H A P T E R Along with nourishment, harmful substances, called teratogens, can pass through or harm the placenta—with potentially tragic effects. If the mother is a heroin addict, her baby is born a heroin addict. If she is a heavy smoker, her newborn is likely to be underweight, sometimes dangerously so (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1983a). If she drinks much alcohol, her baby is at greater risk for birth defects and mental retardation (Raymond, 1987). If she carries the AIDS virus, her baby will often be infected as well (Minkoff, 1987). 3 T h e Developing Child Tera literally means " m o n s t e r " ; teratogens are " m o n s t e r - p r o d u c i n g " agents, such as chemicals and viruses, that may harm the fetus. THE C O M P E T E N T N E W B O R N Newborns come equipped with reflexes that are ideally suited for sur­ vival. The infants will withdraw a limb to escape pain; if a cloth is put over their faces, interfering with their breathing, they will turn their heads from side to side and swipe at it. New parents are often awed by the coordinated sequence of reflexes by which babies obtain food. The rooting reflex is one example: When their cheeks are touched, babies will open their mouths and vigorously "root" for a nipple. Finding one, they will automatically close on it and begin sucking—which itself requires a coordinated sequence of tonguing, swallowing, and breath­ ing. Failing to find satisfaction, the hungry baby may cry—a behavior that parents are predisposed to find highly unpleasant to hear and very rewarding to relieve. The pioneering American psychologist William James (who once said, "The first lecture on psychology I ever heard was the first I ever gave") presumed that the newborn experiences a "blooming, buzzing confusion." Until the 1960s few people disagreed. It was said that, apart from a blur of meaningless light and dark shades, newborns could not see. Then, just as the development of new technology led to a surge of progress in the neurosciences, so too did new investigative techniques enhance the study of infants. Scientists discovered that babies can tell you a lot—if you know how to ask. To ask, you must capitalize on what the baby can do—gaze, suck, turn the head. So, equipped with eye-tracking machines, pacifiers wired to electronic gear, and other such devices, researchers set out to answer parents' age-old question: What can my baby see, hear, smell, and think? "It is a rare privilege to watch the birth, g r o w t h , and first feeble struggles of a liv­ ing human m i n d . " Annie Sullivan, in Helen Keller's The Story of My Life, 1903 They discovered that a baby's sensory equipment is "wired" to facilitate social responsiveness. Newborns turn their heads in the di­ rection of human voices, but not in response to artificial sounds. They gaze more at a drawing of a human face than at a bull's-eye pattern; yet they gaze more at a bull's-eye pattern—which has contrasts much like that of the human eye—than at a solid disk (Fantz, 1961). They focus best on objects about 9 inches away, which, wonder of wonders, just happens to be the typical distance between a nursing infant's eyes and the mother's. Newborns, it seems, arrive perfectly designed to see their mothers' eyes first of all. Babies' perceptual abilities are continuously developing during the first months of life. Within days of birth, babies can distinguish their mothers' facial expression, odor, and voice. A week-old nursing baby, placed between a gauze pad from its mother's bra and one from an­ other nursing mother, will generally turn toward the smell of its own mother's pad (MacFarlane, 1978). At 3 weeks of age, an infant who is allowed to suck on a pacifier that sometimes turns on recordings of its mother's voice and sometimes that of female stranger, will suck more vigorously when it hears its mother's voice (Mills & Melhuish, 1974). So not only can infants see what they need to see, and smell and hear well, but they are already using their sensory equipment to learn. Studies like the one under w a y in this M I T lab are exploring infants' abilities to perceive, think, a n d remember. This infant is being tested on pattern perception. Experiments have demonstrated that even very y o u n g infants are capable of sophis­ ticated visual discrimination. T h e y often s h o w a marked preference for one picture over another, a n d will also look m u c h longer at an unfamiliar pattern than at one which they have seen before. S u c h tests have c h a n g e d psychologists' ideas of w h a t the world looks like to a baby. 62 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Two teams of investigators have reported the astonishing and con­ troversial finding that, in the second week—or even the first hour of life—infants tend to imitate facial expressions (Field & others, 1982; Meltzoff & Moore, 1983, 1985). If such findings can be substantiated by additional research, it will be a further tribute to the newborn's competence. Consider: How do newborn babies relate their own facial movements to those of an adult? And how can they coordinate the movements involved? The findings are controversial because they contradict what most people have pre­ sumed to be the newborn's very limited sensory and motor abilities. Researcher Tiffany Field (1987) notes that "Our knowledge of in­ fancy was in its infancy 20 years ago, but what we have learned since then has dramatically changed the way we perceive and treat infants." More and more, psychologists see the baby as "a very sophisticated perceiver of the world, with very sensitive social and emotional quali­ ties and impressive intellectual abilities." The "helpless infant" of the 1950s has become the "amazing newborn" of the 1980s. Imitation? W h e n A n d r e w Meltzoff s h o w s his t o n g u e , an 1 8 - d a y - o l d boy responds similarly. " A s our experimental techniques have become more a n d more sophisti­ INFANCY AND C H I L D H O O D During infancy, a baby grows from newborn to toddler, and during childhood from toddler to teenager. Beginning in this chapter with infancy and childhood and continuing in the next chapter with adoles­ cence through old age, we will see how people of all ages are continu­ ally developing—physically, cognitively, and socially. PHYSICAL c a t e d , " reports Meltzoff ( 1 9 8 7 ) , " t h e in­ fants themselves have appeared more a n d more c l e v e r . " DEVELOPMENT Brain D e v e l o p m e n t While you resided in your mother's womb, your body was forming nerve cells at the rate of about one-quarter million per minute. On the day you were born you had essentially all the brain cells you were ever going to have. However, the human nervous system is immature at birth; the neural networks that enable us to walk, talk, and remember are only beginning to form (see Figure 3 - 4 ) . Along with the development of neural networks we see increasing myelinization of neurons. (As we saw in Chapter 2, myelin is a fatty cell that encases the axon, enabling messages to speed many times faster.) The nerve fibers that monitor and coordinate bladder control, for example, are not fully myelinated until the second year. To expect a toddler to do without a diaper before then is to court disaster. The storage of permanent memory also requires neural develop­ ment. Our earliest memories go back only to between our third and fourth birthdays (Kihlstrom & Harachkiewicz, 1982). For parents, this "infantile amnesia," as Freud called it, can be disconcerting. After all the hours we spend with our babies—after all the frolicking on the rug, all the diapering, feeding, and rocking to sleep—what would they con­ sciously remember of us if we died? Nothing! But if nothing is consciously recalled, something has still been gained. From birth (and even before), infants can learn. They can learn to turn their heads away to make an adult "peekaboo" at them, to pull a string to make a mobile turn, to turn their heads to the left or right to receive a sugar solution when their forehead is stroked (Blass, 1987; Bower, 1977; Lancioni, 1980). Such learning tends not to persist unless At birth 6 months Figure 3 - 4 1 month 15 months 3 months 2 years In h u m a n s , the brain is immature at birth. T h e s e drawings of sec­ tions of brain tissue from the cerebral cor­ tex illustrate the increasing complexity of the neural networks in the maturing h u m a n brain. C H A P T E R reactivated. Nevertheless, early learning may prepare our brains for those later experiences that we do remember. For example, children who become deaf at age 2, after being exposed to speech, are later more easily language trained than those deaf from birth (Lenneberg, 1967). It has been suggested that the first 2 years are therefore critical for learning language. Animals such as guinea pigs, whose brains are mature at birth, more readily form permanent memories from infancy than do animals with immature brains, such as rats (Campbell & Coulter, 1976). Be­ cause human brains are also immature at birth, these findings cast doubt on the idea that people subconsciously remember their prenatal life or the trauma of their birth. But, given occasional reminders, 3-month-old infants who learn that moving their leg propels a mobile will indeed remember the association for at least a month (RoveeCollier, 1988)—so some infant memory does exist. Does experience, as well as biological maturation, help develop the brain's neural connections? Although "forgotten," early learning may help prepare our brains for thought and language, and for later experi­ ences. Surely our early learning must somehow be recorded "in there." If early experiences affect us by leaving their "marks" in the brain, then it should be possible to detect evidence of this. The modern tools of neuroscience allow us a closer look. Working at the University of California, Berkeley, Mark Rosenzweig caged some rats in solitary con­ finement, while others were caged in a communal playground (Figure 3-5). Rats living in the deprived environment usually developed a lighter and thinner cortex with smaller nerve cell bodies, as well as fewer glial cells (the "glue cells" that support and nourish the brain's neurons). Rosenzweig (1984a; Renner & Rosenzweig, 1987) reported being so surprised by these effects of experience on brain tissue that he repeated the experiment several times before publishing his findings— findings that have led to improvements in the environments provided for laboratory and farm animals and for institutionalized children. Other recent studies extended these findings. Several research teams have found that infant rats and premature babies benefit from the stimulation of being touched or massaged (Field & others, 1986; Meaney & others, 1988). "Handled" infants of both species gain weight more rapidly and develop faster neurologically. In adulthood, handled rat pups also secrete less of a stress hormone that during aging causes neuron death in the hippocampus, a brain center impor­ tant for memory. William Greenough and his University of Illinois co­ workers (1987) further discovered that repeated experiences sculpt a rat's neural tissue—at the very spot in the brain where the experience is processed. This sculpting seems to work by preserving activated neural connections while allowing unused connections to degenerate. 3 T h e Developing Child Impoverished environment Enriched environment Figure 3 - 5 Experience affects the brain's development. In experiments pio­ neered by M a r k R o s e n z w e i g and D a v i d K r e c h , rats were reared either alone in an More and more, researchers are becoming convinced that the brain's neural connections are dynamic; throughout life our neural tis­ sue is changing. Our genes dictate our overall brain architecture, but experience directs the details. If a monkey is trained to push a lever with a finger several thousand times a day, the brain tissue that con­ trols the finger changes to reflect the experience. The wiring of Michael Jordan's brain reflects the thousands of hours he has spent shooting baskets. Experience, it seems, helps nurture nature. environment without playthings or with Motor Development As the infant's muscles and neural networks mature, ever more complicated skills emerge. Although the age at M. C. D i a m o n d . C o p y r i g h t © 1 9 7 2 S c i e n ­ others in an environment enriched with playthings that were c h a n g e d daily. In fourteen out of sixteen repetitions of this basic experiment, the rats placed in the enriched environment developed signifi­ cantly more cerebral cortex relative to the rest of the brain's tissue than those in the impoverished environment. (From " B r a i n c h a n g e s in response to e x p e r i e n c e " by M. R. R o s e n z w e i g , E. L Bennett, and tific A m e r i c a n , Inc. All rights reserved.) 63 which infants sit, stand, and walk varies from child to child, the se­ quence in which babies pass these developmental milestones (Figure 3 - 6 ) is universal. Can experience retard or speed up the maturation of physical skills? If babies were bound to a cradleboard for much of their first year—the traditional practice among the Hopi Indians—would they walk later than do unbound infants? If allowed to spend an hour a day in a walker chair after age 4 months, would they walk earlier? Amaz­ ingly, in view of what we now know about the effects experience has on the brain, the answer to both questions seems to be no (Dennis, 1940; Ridenour, 1982). Biological maturation—including the rapid development of the cerebellum at the rear of the brain—creates a readiness to learn walk­ ing at about 1 year of age. Experience before that time has no more than a small effect, although restriction later may retard development (Super, 1981). This is true for other physical skills, including bowel and bladder control. Until the necessary muscular and neural maturation has occurred, no amount of pleading, harassment, or punishment can lead to successful toilet training. After a spurt during the first 2 years, growth slows to a steady 2 to 3 inches per year through childhood. With all of the neurons and most of their interconnections in place, brain development after age 2 simi­ larly proceeds at a slower pace. The sensory and motor cortex areas continue to mature relatively rapidly, enabling fine motor skills to de­ velop further (R. Wilson, 1978). The association areas of the cortex— those associated with thinking, memory, and language—are the last brain areas to develop. C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child COGNITIVE D E V E L O P M E N T Brain scans and brain wave analyses reveal that the development of different brain areas during infancy and childhood corresponds closely with cognitive development (Chugani & Phelps, 1986; Thatcher & oth­ ers, 1987). Brain and mind develop together. Cognition refers to all the mental activities associated with thinking, knowing, and remember­ ing. Few questions have intrigued developmental psychologists more than these: When can children begin to remember? See things from another's point of view? Reason logically? Think symbolically? Simply put, how does a child's mind grow? Such were the questions posed by developmental psychologist Jean Piaget (pronounced Pea-ah-ZHAY). "Who knows the thoughts of a child?" wondered poet Nora Perry. As much as anyone of his generation, Piaget knew. His interest in children's cognitive processes began in 1920, when he was working in Paris to develop questions for children's intelligence tests. In the course of administering tests to find out at what age children could answer certain questions correctly, Piaget became intrigued by children's wrong answers. Where others saw childish mistakes, Piaget saw intelligence at work. He observed that the errors made by children of a given age were often strikingly similar. The more than 50 years Piaget spent in such informal activities with children convinced him that the child's mind is not a miniature model of the adult''s: Young children actively construct their understandings of the world in radically different ways than adults do, a fact that we overlook when attempting to teach children by using our adult logic. Piaget further believed that the child's mind develops through a series of stages, in an upward march from the sensorimotor simplicity of the newborn to the abstract reasoning power of the adult. An 8-year-old child therefore comprehends things that a 3-year-old cannot. An 8-year-old might grasp the analogy "getting an idea is like having a light turn on in your head," but trying to teach the same analogy to a 3-year-old would be fruitless. Jean Piaget ( 1 9 3 0 , p. 2 3 7 ) : " I f we e x a m ­ ine the intellectual d e v e l o p m e n t of the individual or of the whole of humanity, we shall find that the h u m a n spirit g o e s t h r o u g h a certain n u m b e r of stages, each different from the o t h e r . " " F o r everything there is a season, and a time for every matter under h e a v e n . " How the Mind of a Child Grows The driving force behind this intellectual progression is the unceasing struggle to make sense out of one's world. To this end, the maturing brain builds concepts, which Piaget called schemas. Schemas are ways of looking at the world that organize our past experiences and provide a framework for under­ standing our future experiences. We start life with simple schemas— those involving sense-driven reflexes such as sucking and grasping. By adulthood we have built a seemingly limitless number of schemas that range from knowing how to tie a knot to knowing what it means to be in love. Piaget proposed two concepts to explain how we use and adjust our schemas. First, we interpret our experience in terms of our current understandings; in Piaget's terms, we incorporate, or assimilate, new experiences into our existing schemas. Assimilation is interpreting new experiences in light of one's sche­ mas, as when a toddler calls all four-legged animals "doggies." But we also adjust, or accommodate, our schemas to fit the particulars of new experiences. The child learns fairly quickly that the original "doggie" schema is too broad, and accommodates by refining the category. When new experiences just will not fit our old schemas, our schemas may change to accommodate the experiences. When prejudiced people perceive a minority person through their preconceived ideas, they are assimilating. When experience forces them to modify their former schemas, they are accommodating. Ecclesiastes 3:1 Look carefully at the "devil's tuning fork" below. N o w look a w a y — n o , better first study it some m o r e — a n d then look away and draw it. Not so easy, is it? Because this tuning fork is an impossible object, you have no s c h e m a into which you can as­ similate what y o u see. 65 66 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Highly realistic art is easy to assimilate; it requires for interpretation only those schemas already available from observing the Science itself is a process of assimilation and accommodation. Sci­ entists interpret nature using their preconceived theories—for exam­ ple, that newborns are passive, incompetent creatures—to assimilate what would otherwise be a bewildering body of disconnected observa­ tions. Then, as new observations collide with these theories, the theo­ ries must be changed or replaced to accommodate the findings. Thus the new concept of the newborn as competent and active replaces the old schema. That, Piaget believed, is how children (and adults) construct reality using both assimilation and accommodation. What we know is not reality exactly as it is, but our constructions of it. world. Highly abstract art is difficult to assimilate, which m a y explain the frustra­ tion it sometimes causes. W h e n art c o m ­ bines realism with abstraction, it allows observers to impose m e a n i n g by stimulat­ ing them to stretch their schemas. Piaget's Theory of Cognitive Stages Piaget went on to describe cognitive development as occurring in four major stages (Table 3 - 1 ) . P I A G E T ' S S T A C E 5 O F COGNITIVE D E V E L O P M E N T Approximate age Description of stage Developmental milestones Birth—2 years Sensorimotor Infant experiences the world through senses and actions (looking, touching, mouthing). Object permanence (page 67); stranger anxiety (page 68). 2 - 6 years 7 - 1 2 years Teen years Preoperational Child represents things with words and images, but cannot reason with logic. Ability to pretend (page 67); egocentrism (page 68). Concrete operational Child thinks logically about concrete events; can grasp concrete analogies and perform arithmetical operations. Conservation (page 69); mathematical transformations (page 69). Formal operational Teenager develops abstract reasoning. Scientific reasoning (page 92); potential for mature moral reasoning (pages 9 2 - 9 5 ) . C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child T h e sensorimotor stage of development b e g i n s in infancy a n d continues until a b o u t a g e 2 . Very y o u n g children explore The developing child, he believed, moves from one age-related plateau to the next. Each plateau has distinctive characteristics that permit spe­ cific kinds of thinking. The differences between these kinds of thinking are qualitative: They involve changes in the way the child thinks. During the first stage, which occurs between birth and approxi­ mately age 2, infants are limited to sensorimotor intelligence: Their under­ standing of the world is restricted to their interactions with objects through their senses and motor activity—through looking, touching, sucking, grasping, and the like. In the preschool years of 2 to 6 most children demonstrate preoperational intelligence: They can think about objects without physically interacting with them, which means they can begin to think about objects in a simple symbolic way. This new type of thinking is reflected in the preschooler's ability to pretend, to think about past events and anticipate future ones, and to begin to use language. Children in the preoperational stage are not able, however, to think in a truly logical fashion. They may figure out that five plus three is eight and not instantly realize that three plus five is also eight. Beginning at about age 7, children demonstrate concrete operational intelligence: They can perform the mental operations that produce logi­ cal thought, but they are able to think logically only about concrete things. It is not until about age 12, when children enter the stage of formal operational intelligence, that they are able to begin to think hypothetically and abstractly. To appreciate how the mind of a child grows, let's look more closely at each of these stages. Sensorimotor Stage During the sensorimotor stage, infants under­ stand their world in terms of their senses and the effects of their ac­ tions; they are aware only of what they can see, smell, suck, taste, and grasp, and at first they seem to be unaware that things continue to exist apart from their perceptions. In one of his tests, Piaget would show an infant an appealing toy and then flop his beret over it to see whether the infant searched for the toy. Before the age of 8 months, they did not. They lacked object permanence—the awareness that objects continue to exist when not perceived. The infant lives in the present. What is out of sight is out of mind. By 8 months, infants begin to develop what psychologists now believe is a memory for things no longer seen. Hide the toy and the the world t h r o u g h their senses, e n j o y i n g the smell, feel, and taste of almost a n y ­ t h i n g they c a n g e t their hands o n . As the child m o v e s into the preoperational stage, at the a g e of a b o u t 18 months, pretend­ ing becomes possible. This child's imagi­ nation is still expressed t h r o u g h simple c a r e - g i v i n g ; in another year the pretend activities will b e c o m e m u c h more elabo­ rate. " C h i l d r e n think not of what is past, nor w h a t is to c o m e , but enjoy the present time, which few of us d o . " L a Bruyere, 1 6 4 5 - 1 6 9 6 Les caraderes: De I'homme 67 68 PART 2 D e v e l o p m e n t over the Life S p a n O b j e c t permanence. Y o u n g e r children lack the sense that things continue to exist w h e n not in sight; but for this 8 / 2 - m o n t h 1 infant will momentarily look for it. Within another month or two, the infant will look for it even after being restrained for several seconds. This flowering of recall occurs simultaneously with the emergence of a fear of strangers, called stranger anxiety. Watch how infants of different ages react when handed over to a stranger and you will notice that, beginning at 8 or 9 months, they often will cry and reach for their familiar caregivers. Is it a mere coincidence that object permanence and stranger anxiety develop together? Probably not. After about 8 months of age, the child has schemas for familiar faces; when a new face cannot be assimilated into these remembered schemas, the infant becomes distressed (Kagan, 1984). This link between cognitive development and social behavior illustrates the interplay of brain maturation, cogni­ tive development, and social development. Preoperational Stage Seen through the eyes of Piaget, preschool chil­ dren are still far from being short grownups. Although aware of them­ selves, of time, and of the permanence of objects, they are, he said, egocentric: They cannot perceive things from another's point of view. The preschooler who blocks your view of the television while trying to see it herself and the one who asks a question while you are on the phone both assume that you see and hear what they see and hear. When relating to a young child, it may help to remember that such behaviors reflect a cognitive limitation: The egocentric preschooler has difficulty taking another's viewpoint. Preschoolers also find it easier to follow positive instructions ("Hold the puppy gently") than negative ones ("Don't squeeze the puppy"). One characteristic of parents who abuse their children is that they generally have no understanding of these limits. They perceive their children as junior adults who are in control of their behavior (Larrance & Twentyman, 1983). Thus children who stand in the way, spill food, disobey negative instructions, or cry may be perceived as willfully malicious. Just before age 3 children do, however, become more capable of thinking symbolically. Judy DeLoache (1987) discovered this when she showed a group of ZV^-year-dlds a model of a room and hid a model toy in it (say a miniature stuffed dog behind a miniature couch). The chil­ dren could easily remember where to find the miniature toy, but could not readily locate the actual stuffed dog behind the couch in the real room. When 3-year-olds were given a look at the model room, how­ ever, they would usually go right to the actual stuffed animal in the old child, out of sight is not out of mind. C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child real room, showing that they could think of the model as a symbol for the room. Piaget believed that during this preschool period and up to about age 7, children are in what he called the preoperational stage—unable to perform mental operations. For a 5-year-old, the quantity of milk that is "too much" in a tall, narrow glass may become an acceptable amount if poured into a short, wide glass. This is because the child focuses only on the height dimension, and is incapable of reversing the operation by mentally pouring it back. The child lacks the concept of conservation—the principle that the quantity of a substance remains the same despite changes in its shape. Children's conversations con­ firm this inability to reverse information (Phillips, 1969, p. 61): "Do you have a brother?" "Yes." "What's his name?" "Jim." "Does Jim have a brother?" "No." Concrete Operational Stage With older children, Piaget would roll one of two identical balls of clay into a rope shape and ask whether there was more clay in the rope or the ball. Children who are in the preoperational stage almost always say that the rope has more clay because they assume "longer is more." They cannot mentally reverse the clay-rolling process to see that the amount of clay is the same in both shapes. But children who are in the concrete operational stage realize that a given quantity remains the same no matter how its shape changes. Piaget contended that during the stage of concrete operations (roughly ages 7 to 12) children acquire the mental operations needed to comprehend mathematical transformations and conservation. When my daughter Laura was age 6, I was astonished at her inability to reverse arithmetic operations—until considering Piaget. Asked, "What is eight plus four?" she required 5 seconds to compute "twelve," and another 5 seconds to then compute twelve minus four. By age 8, she could reverse the process and answer the second question instantly. Although the operations usually must involve concrete images of physical actions or objects, not abstract ideas, preteen children exhibit logic. Eleven-year-olds can mentally pour the milk back and forth be­ tween different-shaped glasses, so they realize that change in shape does not mean change in quantity. They also enjoy jokes that allow them to utilize their recently acquired concepts, such as conservation: Mr. Jones went into a restaurant and ordered a whole pizza for his dinner. When the waiter asked if he wanted it cut into 6 or 8 pieces, Mr. Jones said, "Oh, you'd better make it 6, I could never eat 8 pieces!" (McGhee, 1976) If Piaget was correct that children construct their understandings through assimilation and accommodation, and that in early childhood their thinking is radically different from adult thinking, what are the implications for preschool and elementary school teachers? Might teachers capitalize on what comes naturally to children? Believing that children actively construct their own understandings, Piaget con­ tended that teachers should strive to "create the possibilities for a child to invent and discover." Build on what children already know, allow them to touch and see, to witness concrete demonstrations, to think for themselves. Exploit their natural ways of thinking and learning. Be­ cause the young child is incapable of adult logic, teachers must under- T h e preoperational child cannot perform the mental operations essential to under­ standing conservation. A glass of milk seems to be " m o r e " after being poured into a tall, narrow g l a s s . 69 70 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Piaget's writings have influenced m a n y educators to provide children with situa­ tions a n d e n c o u r a g e m e n t that will prompt t h e m to do their o w n exploring a n d dis­ stand how children think, and therefore realize that what is simple and obvious to them—that subtraction is the reverse of addition—may be incomprehensible to a 6-year-old. Reflections on Piaget's Theory Piaget's stage theory is controversial. Do children's cognitive abilities really go through distinct stages? Does object permanence in fact appear rather abruptly, much as a tulip blos­ soms in spring? Today's researchers contend that we have underesti­ mated the competence of young children. Given very simple tasks, preschoolers are not purely egocentric; they will adjust their explana­ tions to make them clearer to a listener who is blindfolded, and will show a toy or picture with the front side facing the viewer (Gelman, 1979; Siegel & Hodkin, 1982). If questioned in a way that makes sense to them, 5- and 6-year-olds will exhibit some understanding of conser­ vation (Donaldson, 1979). It seems, then, that the abilities to take an­ other's perspective and to perform mental operations are not utterly absent in the preoperational stage—and then suddenly appear. Rather, these abilities begin earlier than Piaget believed and develop more gradually. What remains of Piaget's ideas about the mind of the child? Plenty. For Piaget identified and named important cognitive phenomena and helped stimulate interest in studying how the mind develops. That we today are adapting his ideas to accommodate new findings would not surprise him. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT As we have seen, babies are social creatures from birth. Almost from the start, parent and baby communicate through eye contact, touch, smiles, and voice. The end result is social behavior that promotes in­ fants' survival and their emerging sense of self, so that they, too, even­ tually may bear and nurture a new generation. In all cultures, infants develop an intense bond with those who care for them. Beginning with newborns' attraction to humans in gen­ eral, infants soon come to prefer familiar faces and voices and then to coo and gurgle when given their mothers' or fathers' attention. By 8 covering. For y o u n g e r children, especially, direct observation has proved an effective learning tool. C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child months, when they are dealing with the linked milestones of object permanence and stranger anxiety, they will crawl wherever mother or father goes and will become distressed when separated from them. At 12 months many infants cling tightly to a parent when frightened or anticipating separation and, when reunited, shower the parent with smiles and hugs. No social behavior is more striking than this intense infant love, called attachment, a powerful survival impulse that keeps infants close to their caregivers. Among the early social responses— love, fear, aggression—the first-and greatest is this bond of love. Origins of Attachment How does this parent-infant bond develop? A number of elements work together to create this relationship. Body Contact For many years developmental psychologists rea­ soned that infants became attached to those who satisfied their need for nourishment. It makes perfect sense. But an accidental finding re­ vealed this explanation of attachment to be incomplete. For his 1950s studies of the development of learning abilities, University of Wiscon­ sin psychologist Harry Harlow needed to breed monkeys. To equalize the infant monkeys' early experiences and to prevent the spread of disease, he separated the monkeys from their mothers shortly after birth and raised them in sanitary, individual cages, which included a cheesecloth baby blanket (Harlow & others, 1971). Surprisingly, the infants became intensely attached to their blankets: When the blankets were taken to be laundered, the monkeys were greatly distressed. They acted as if they had been separated from their mothers. Harlow soon recognized that this attachment to the blanket contra­ dicted the idea that attachment is derived from the association with nourishment. But could he demonstrate this more convincingly? Doing so would require some way to pit the drawing power of a food source against the contact comfort of the blanket. Harlow's creative solution consisted of two artificial mothers—one a bare wire cylinder with a wooden head, the other a similar cylinder wrapped with foam rubber and covered with terrycloth. Either could be associated with feeding through an attached bottle. Even when reared with a nourishing wire mother and a nonnourishing cloth mother, the monkeys overwhelmingly preferred the cloth mother (Figure 3 - 7 ) . Like human infants clinging to their mothers, they would cling to the cloth mother when anxious and would use her as a base of security from which to venture out into the environment, as if attached to the mother by an invisible elastic band that stretches so far and then pulls the infant back. Further studies with Margaret Harlow and others revealed that other qualities— rocking, warmth, and feeding—could boost the magnetism of the com­ fortable cloth mother. In human infants, too, attachment usually grows from body con­ tact with parents who are soft and warm and who rock, pat, and feed. This may help explain the recent finding that infants whose parents were instructed to carry them for at least 3 hours a day cried less than other infants (Hunziker & Barr, 1986). The net result of Harlow's re­ search should also reassure the fathers of breast-fed infants: Attach­ ment does not depend on feeding alone. Familiarity Another key to attachment is familiarity (Rheingold, 1985). Infants prefer faces and objects with which they are familiar. In certain animals, attachments based on familiarity form during a critical period—a restricted time period during which certain events must take Figure 3 - 7 W h e n Harry Harlow reared m o n k e y s with t w o artificial m o t h e r s — o n e a bare wire cylinder with a w o o d e n head a n d an attached f e e d i n g bottle a n d the other a cylinder covered with foam rubber a n d w r a p p e d with terrycloth but without a f e e d i n g bottle—they preferred the c o m ­ fortable cloth mother to the nourishing wire mother. It is interesting that m o n k e y s also prefer the texture of terrycloth to the u n m o n k e y - l i k e smoothness of satins a n d silks, a n d that h u m a n infants are similarly more soothed by a textured than a s m o o t h blanket ( M a c c o b y , 1 9 8 0 ) . 72 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n place if proper development is to occur—shortly after birth. The first moving object that a gosling, duckling, or chick sees during the hours shortly after hatching is normally its mother, and thereafter the young fowl follows her, and her alone. Konrad Lorenz (1937) explored this rigid attachment process, called imprinting. He wondered what duck­ lings would do if he were the first moving creature they observed. What they did was follow him around. Further tests revealed that baby birds would imprint to a variety of moving objects—an animal of an­ other species, a box on wheels, a bouncing ball—and that, once formed, this attachment was often difficult to reverse (Colombo, 1982). The attachment of infants to their parents is reciprocated. Most mammals lick and groom their newborns during the first hours after birth and, if prevented from doing so, may later reject their offspring. Might there be a critical period during which contact triggers such bonding in humans? Proponents of bonding maintain that physical contact during the first hours after birth boosts parent-infant attachment (Kennell & Klaus, 1982). But developmental psychologists Michael Lamb (1982), Susan Goldberg (1983), and Barbara Myers (1984a, 1984b) scrutinized the research on mother-infant bonding and each came away uncon­ vinced. Either the studies were seriously flawed, they reported, or the effects of physical "bonding" are both minimal and temporary. Hu­ mans don't have a precise critical period for becoming attached. Critics of the bonding notion welcome the trend toward involving parents in the childbirth process. The danger, they suggest, comes in making parents who have not experienced early contact—including mothers who have had cesareans or parents who have adopted chil­ dren—feel inadequate. These parents may be led by proponents of bonding to fear that they and their child have missed out on something terribly important. And that, say the critics, is simply not true. Likewise, there is little evidence that breast-feeding is psychologi­ cally more advantageous to an infant than bottle-feeding. Bottle-fed infants usually enjoy nearly the same cuddling, eye contact, and sen­ sory experience as nursing infants. To be sure, breast-feeding is com­ mendable as an intimate and pleasurable way of providing ideal nutri­ tion—as ideal as cow's milk is for calves. But to be bottle-fed is not to be psychologically handicapped (Fergusson & others, 1987). Responsive Parenting How are children's attachments linked with parental behavior? Placed in a strange situation (usually a laboratory playroom), some children show secure attachment: In the mother's pres­ ence they play comfortably, happily exploring their new environment; when she leaves, they are distressed; when she returns, they seek contact with her. Other infants show insecure attachment: They are less likely to explore their surroundings and may even cling to their mother; when the mother leaves, they cry loudly; and when she re­ turns, they may be indifferent or even hostile toward her (Ainsworth, 1973; Ainsworth & others, 1978). What accounts for these differences? The innate differences among infants is one likely answer. Some babies may be more disposed to forming a secure attachment just as, from birth, some babies are more easily held, cuddled, and comforted. But there is more to infant differences than biology. Mary Ainsworth (1979) explored another possible influence on attachment: the mother's behavior. She observed mother-infant pairs at home during the first 6 months and then later observed the 1-year-old infants in a strange situation without their mother. Sensitive, responsive mothers— mothers who continually noticed what their babies were doing and When imprinting studies go awry . . . A l t h o u g h breast-feeding provides babies with superior nutrition a n d can increase a mother's sense of intimacy with her in­ fant, fathers, too, love to feed their b a ­ bies, a n d there is little evidence that breast-feeding significantly affects the in­ fant's psychological development. T h e in­ timacy evident in this photo will benefit both infant a n d father. CHAPTER responded appropriately—tended to have infants who became se­ curely attached. Insensitive, unresponsive mothers—mothers who at­ tended to their babies when they felt like doing so but ignored them at other times—tended to have infants who became insecurely attached. The Harlows' monkey studies, in which the artificial mothers were the ultimate in unresponsiveness, produced even more striking conse­ quences. When put in strange situations without their artificial moth­ ers, the deprived infants were more than distressed—they were terri­ fied. Although informative, such studies would probably not be con­ ducted in today's climate of concern for the welfare of animals. FATHER T h e Developing Child 73 M o n k e y s raised by artificial mothers were terror-stricken when placed in strange sit­ uations without their surrogate mothers. CARE Perhaps you are wondering why t h e focus of so much research has been on mothers and not on fathers, t o o . The c o m m o n assumption, long evident in court child-custody decisions, has been that fathers are less interested and less c o m p e t e n t in child c a r e than are m o t h ­ ers. In both subtle and not-so-subtle ways, psychologists have a c c e p t e d this assumption. Infants w h o lack m o t h e r c a r e a r e said to suffer "maternal depri­ vation"; those lacking father c a r e are said merely to experience "father a b ­ sence." It is true t h a t across the world m o t h ­ ers tend to a s s u m e m o r e responsibility for infant care, and t h a t a breast-feed­ Michael Lamb (1979): "Mothers and fa­ thers can be equally effective as parents. They just have different styles." 3 departure as by their mothers'. M o r e ­ over, infants whose fathers have shared in their c a r e , for example, by changing diapers, are m o r e secure when left with a stranger. Looking for differences, research psychologists have also uncovered sev­ eral distinctive ways in which fathers and mothers interact with their infants. Fathers tend to smile less at their babies (males smile less at everyone), to spend m o r e of their interaction in play rather than caretaking (especially with sons), and to play with m o r e physical excite­ ment (Parke, 1 9 8 1 ) . However, when fathers are the pri­ mary caregivers, they interact with ing m o t h e r and nursing infant have wonderfully coordinated biological sys­ their babies m o r e as mothers typically do. This suggests t h a t father-mother tems t h a t predispose their responsive­ ness t o o n e a n o t h e r ( M a c c o b y , 1 9 8 0 ) . Nevertheless, many modern fathers a r e differences are not biologically fixed, but have social roots as well. Animal research confirms this. W h e n the Har­ becoming m o r e involved in infant care, and researchers are becoming m o r e lows c a g e d mothers and fathers with their infant monkeys, the fathers w e r e interested in fathers. protective and affectionate t o w a r d their infants and m o r e likely than O n e of t h e leading father-watchers, Ross Parke ( 1 9 8 1 ) , reported that fa­ thers can be just as interested in, sensi­ mothers to e n g a g e in physical play. Within two-parent families, both tive to, and affectionate t o w a r d their infants as mothers are. Although m o t h ­ parents have yet a n o t h e r gift to offer: their support of o n e another. Mothers ers typically do m o s t of t h e infant c a r e , fathers are as capable (at least when researchers are watching). It also seems and fathers w h o support o n e another and w h o sense this mutual support and a g r e e m e n t in child-rearing also tend to that although most infants prefer their mothers when anxious, when left alone be m o r e responsive to their infants and to feel m o r e c o m p e t e n t as parents they are as distressed by their fathers' (Dickie, 1 9 8 7 ) . 74 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Effects of Attachment What have researchers learned about the ef­ fects of attachment on later development? Secure Attachment Breeds Social Competence It a trusting, secure at­ tachment has lasting benefits, then the quality of an infant's attach­ ments should predict the child's social competence in the years that follow. Even adult romantic love styles exhibit childlike secure or inse­ cure attachment (Hazen & Shaver, 1987). But do our early attachment patterns actually predict our later social behavior? At the University of Minnesota, Alan Sroufe and his co-workers (Sroufe, 1978; Sroufe & others, 1983) confirmed that infants' attach­ ments do predict their later social competence. Sroufe reported that infants who are securely attached at 12 to 18 months of age—those who use their mother as a base for comfortably exploring the world and as a haven when distressed—function more confidently as 2- to 3V2-year-olds. Given challenging tasks, they are more enthusiastic and persistent. When with other children, they are more outgoing and re­ sponsive. Developmental theorist Erik Erikson, whose ideas we will consider further in Chapter 4, would say that such children approach life with a sense of basic trust—a sense that the world is predictable and reliable— rather than mistrust. Erikson theorized that infants whose needs are well met by sensitive caregivers form a lifelong attitude of trust rather than fear. Basic trust is the first of Erikson's proposed stages in children's social development (see Table 3 - 2 ) . Approximate age Description of stage Infancy (1st year) Trust vs. mistrust 'If needs are met, infant develops a sense of basic trust. Toddler (2nd year) Autonomy vs. shame and doubt Toddler strives to learn independence and self-confidence. Preschooler ( 3 - 5 years) Initiative vs. guilt Preschooler learns to initiate tasks and grapples with self-control. Elementary School (6 years to puberty) Competence vs. inferiority Child learns either to feel effective or inadequate. Other psychologists believe the child's social competence reflects not the early parenting but the continued responsive parenting that these children are still receiving when retested (Lamb, 1987). The genes shared by competent parents and their competent children may also predispose their similar behavior (Goldsmith & Alansky, 1987; Plomin & others, 1985). " O u t of the conflict between trust a n d mistrust, the infant develops hope, which is the earliest form of w h a t gradually be­ comes faith in a d u l t s . " Erik Erikson ( 1 9 8 3 ) C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child The attachments of early childhood in time relax. Whether raised entirely at home or also in a day care center, whether living in America, Guatemala, or the Kalahari Desert, anxiety over being separated from parents peaks at around 13 months and then gradually declines (Kagan, 1976; see Figure 3 - 8 ) . With time, children become familiar with a wider and wider range of situations, and with the advent of language they communicate with strangers more freely. Does this mean that, as we develop, our need for and love of oth­ ers fades away? Hardly. In other ways our capacity for love grows, and our pleasure in touching and holding those we love never ceases. The powerful parental love nonetheless gradually relaxes, allowing chil­ dren to move out into the world. One might even say that much of the life cycle story—from fetus to birth and infancy, to adolescence, to marriage and parenthood, to old age and death—boils down to two realities: attachment and separation. Deprivation of Attachment If social competence is affected by a se­ cure attachment, what are the outcomes of parental deprivation or pro­ longed separation? One way to study the benefits of positive early nurturing experiences is to observe the development of children de­ nied such experiences. In all of psychology, no research literature is more saddening. Children reared in institutions without the stimula­ tion and attention of a regular caregiver, or locked away at home under conditions of extreme neglect, are frequently pathetic creatures— withdrawn, frightened, speechless. Adopted into a loving home, they usually progress rapidly, especially in their cognitive development. They are particularly likely to do well if they were in the company of other children while they were deprived of adult contact. Neverthe­ less, they often bear scars from their early neglect (Rutter, 1979). Most abusive parents report being battered or neglected as chil­ dren (Kempe & Kempe, 1978). Although most abused children do not later become abusive parents, 30 percent do abuse their children—a rate six times higher than the national rate of child abuse (Kaufman & Zigler, 1987). Moreover, young children who have been terrorized through sexual abuse or wartime atrocities (beatings, witnessing tor­ ture, and living in constant fear) also suffer scars; nightmares, depres­ sion, and a troubled adolescence are frequent outcomes (Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Goleman, 1987). These findings were underscored by the Harlows. They reared monkeys not only with artificial mothers but also alone in barren cages or, worse, in total isolation from even the sight and sound of other monkeys. If socially deprived in these ways for 6 months or longer (corresponding to the first 2 years or more of human life), the monkeys were socially devastated. They either cowered in fright or lashed out in aggression when placed with other monkeys their age. Upon reaching sexual maturity, most were incapable of mating. Females who were artificially impregnated often were neglectful, sadistically cruel, or even murderous toward their firstborn offspring. The unloved had become the unloving. The new generation of unloved animals would nevertheless per­ sistently approach and cling to their abusive mothers. In fact, so pow­ erful was the infant monkey's drive for attachment that even artificial "monster mothers"—mothers constructed to occasionally blast com­ pressed air, poke spikes through their terrycloth bodies, or fling their infant off—could only temporarily break their infant's attachment. When the monster mother calmed down, the pitiful infant would re- Figure 3 - 8 Infants' anxiety over separa­ tion from parents. In an experiment, g r o u p s of infants w h o had a n d had not experienced day care were left alone by their mothers in an unfamiliar room. In both g r o u p s , the percentage w h o cried w h e n the mother left peaked at about 13 months. (From K a g a n , 1976.) M o n k e y s raised in total isolation from the sight or s o u n d of other m o n k e y s for more than 6 months were terrified of other m o n k e y s and either lashed out a g g r e s ­ sively or c o w e r e d in fright. 75 76 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n turn, clinging again, as if all were forgiven. Indeed, the distress of abuse seemed even to intensify the infant's clinging, a phenomenon sometimes observed among abused human children. Disruption of Attachment What happens to an infant when attach­ ment is disrupted? Does being uprooted, even from neglectful or abu­ sive parents, predispose the child to later emotional difficulties? These questions bear heavily on custody decisions in cases of child neglect or abuse. Separated from their families, both monkey and human infants become agitated and, before long, withdrawn and even despairing (Bowlby, 1973; Mineka & Suomi, 1978). Fearing that such extreme stress might cause lasting damage (and when in doubt acting to protect parents' rights), the courts have generally been very reluctant to re­ move children from their homes. However, it seems that infants gener­ ally recover from the distress of separation if placed in a more positive and stable environment. In studies of adopted children, Leon Yarrow and his co-workers (1973) found that when children over 6 months of age were removed from their foster mothers they initially had difficul­ ties eating, sleeping, and relating to their new mothers. But by 10 years of age there was little discernible difference in the adjustment of chil­ dren who had been placed before the age of 6 months (with little ac­ companying distress) and those who had been adopted between the ages of 6 and 16 months (with much more distress). Although adop­ tions at later ages might more often be permanently disruptive, it seems that most year-old infants can form new attachments without permanent emotional scars. Foster care with a series of foster families, or repeated removal from a mother and then reunion with her, can be very disruptive, however. " W e can hazard a tentative conclusion: T h e child's later adjustment will be pri­ marily determined by the quality of the relationship with the new caretakers, not Does Day Care Disrupt Attachment? In 1950, when only 14 percent of American mothers worked outside the home, society's Perfect Mom was at the door with cookies, milk, and a sympathetic ear when her children arrived home from school. Today, with half of mothers of American children under age 5. employed, society's Supermom pur­ sues a successful career while sharing parental duties with Dad. Where are these children of mothers who work full-time? Almost 50 percent are cared for in someone else's home. The rest are nearly evenly di­ vided between day care centers and being cared for in their own homes. Of this last group, 20 percent are cared for by sitters, 44 percent by fathers, and 36 percent by another relative (Bureau of the Census, 1987). During the 1950s and '60s, when Perfect Mom was the social norm, the research questions were, "Is day care bad for children? Does it disrupt children's attachments to their parents?" For the high-quality day care programs most commonly studied, the answers were no (Belsky, 1984). In Mother Care/Other Care, developmental psychologist Sandra Scarr (1986) explains that children are "biologically sturdy indi­ viduals . . . who can thrive in a wide variety of life situations." Today, the questions have therefore shifted to the effect that different forms of day care may have on different types and ages of children. We now know enough about the consequences of child neglect to distinguish good from poor day care. Scarr and Richard Weinberg (1986) explain: "Good care means three or four infants and toddlers per care giver and six to eight preschoolers. . . . Good care also means a cheerful, stimulating, and safe physical environment. . . . " The ideal, then, is a verbally stimulating environment in which any child can frequently be seen talking with an adult caregiver (Phillips, McCartney, & Scarr, 1987). by the experience of separation." Eleanor M a c c o b y (1980) C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child High-quality day care provides a safe, stimulating environment a n d a caregiver for every six to eight toddlers. Although parents of children 2 years and older may gain some relief from knowing that day care can mean high-quality child care, the scientific jury is still debating infant day care. Many developmentalists, including Scarr, believe that high-quality infant care does not hinder secure attachments. Fellow developmental psychologists Jay Belsky (1988) and Edward Zigler (1986) caution that, in Belsky's words, "Chil­ dren growing up in families using more than 20 hours per week of non-parental care in their first year of life are at heightened risk of seeming insecure as 1-year-olds and of being disobedient and aggres­ sive at older ages." One striking outcome of research on mother care and other care is how little quality time children receive from parents, employed or not. One national survey revealed that employed mothers average only 11 minutes and fathers 8 minutes per weekday in child-centered activities such as reading, conversing, and playing with their children; homemaker mothers devote not much more—only about 30 minutes per day—to such activities (Timmer & others, 1985-1986). Moreover, there is little disagreement that the half million preschool children actually left alone for part of the time their parents are at work deserve better! So do those children who merely exist for 9 hours a day in minimally equipped, understaffed centers with untrained and poorly paid care­ givers. What all children need is a consistent, warm relationship with people whom they can learn to trust. Self-Concept If attachment is the number one social achievement of infancy, for childhood it is the construction of a positive sense of self. By the end of childhood, at about age 12, most children have devel­ oped a clear self-concept—a sense of their own personal worth and social identity. When and how does this sense of self develop, and how can parents foster a child's self-esteem? "Is my baby aware of herself—does she know that she is a person distinct from others?" The baby cannot talk, so we cannot ask her. Perhaps, however, the infant's behavior could provide clues to the be­ ginnings of her self-awareness. But what sorts of behavior? In 1877, biologist Charles Darwin offered one idea: Self-awareness begins when a child recognizes herself in a mirror. By this indicator, self-recognition 77 78 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n emerges gradually over about a year, starting in roughly the sixth month, when the child reaches toward the mirror to touch her image as if it were another child (Damon & Hart, 1982). How can we know when the infant recognizes that the girl in the mirror is indeed herself and not just an agreeable playmate? In a simple variation of the mirror procedure, researchers surreptitiously dabbed rouge on their subjects' noses before placing them in front of the mir­ ror. Beginning at 15 to 18 months, children, upon seeing the red spot, will touch their noses (Gallup & Suarez, 1986). Apparently, 18-montholds have a schema of how their faces should look; it is as if they wonder, "What is that spot doing on my face?" Beginning with this simple self-awareness, the child's self-concept gradually becomes stronger. By school age, children begin to describe themselves in terms of their gender, their group memberships, and their psychological traits. They come to see themselves as good and skillful in some ways but not others. They form a concept of which traits, ideally, they would like to have, and by age 8 or 10 their selfimage has become quite stable. Children's views of themselves affects their actions. Children who have formed a positive self-concept tend to be more confident, inde­ pendent, optimistic, assertive, and sociable (Maccoby, 1980). All this raises a profoundly important question: How can parents encourage a positive self-concept? Child-Rearing Practices Some parents spank, some reason; some parents are strict, some are lax; some parents seem indifferent to their children, some liberally hug and kiss them. Whether such differences in parenting affect children's behavior has been the subject of much research. The most heavily researched aspect of parenting has been how, and to what extent, parents seek to control their children. Several investigators have identified three specific styles of child management: (1) permissive, (2) authoritarian, and (3) authoritative. Permissive parents tend to submit to their children's desires, make few demands, and use little punishment. Authoritarian parents impose rules and expect obedience: "Don't interrupt." "Don't leave your room a mess." "Don't stay out late or you'll be grounded." "Why? Because I said s o . " Authoritative parents exert control by establishing rules and consistently enforcing them, but also by explaining the reasons for their rules and, especially with older children, encouraging open dis­ cussion when making the rules. Studies by Stanley Coopersmith (1967), Diana Baumrind (1983), and John Buri and others (1988) reveal that the children with the highest self-esteem and most self-reliance tend to have warm, concerned, authoritative parents. What might account for this finding? As later chapters will explain, many experiments indicate that people become more motivated and self-confident if they experience control over their lives; those who experience little control tend to see themselves as somewhat helpless and incompetent. Moreover, children who sense enough control to be able to attribute their behaviors to their own choices ("I obey because I am good") internalize their behaviors more than do children who com­ ply solely because they are coerced ("I obey or I get in bad trouble"). Of the three parenting styles studied, it seems that authoritative parenting provides children with the greatest sense of control over their own lives for two reasons. First, authoritative parents openly dis­ cuss family rules, by explaining them to younger children and reason­ ing about them with older children. When such rules seem not so Mirror images are fascinating to infants from the a g e of about 6 months, but the recognition that the child in the mirror is " m e " does not happen until a b o u t 1 8 months. C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child Studies s u g g e s t that consistency in enforc­ ing rules, c o m b i n e d with calm discussion a n d explanation, helps children achieve self-control. much imposed as negotiated, older children feel more self-control (Baumrind, 1983; Lewis, 1981). Second, when parents enforce rules with consistent, predictable consequences, the child controls the out­ come. Recall that infants become more attached to parents who sensi­ tively and predictably respond to their behaviors. Such infants experi­ ence control. It is when the consequences become extreme—perhaps a threat­ ened spanking for noncompliance—that the child of authoritarian par­ ents is left with no feeling of choice. Similarly, children may lose their sense of control when their parents nag or explode unpredictably. Given permissive parents, children learn early that their own coercive behavior—whining, yelling, tantrums—brings desired results (Patter­ son, 1986). Thus Eleanor Maccoby (1980, p. 389) concluded that "skill­ ful parents must operate within a very delicate balance of forces. They need to obtain compliance to reasonable demands—for the child's, the parents', and the family's sake—without . . . destroying their children's sense of [choice]." Before jumping to any conclusions about the consequences of dif­ ferent parenting styles, we must heed a caution. The evidence is corre­ lational. It tells us that certain child-rearing practices (say, being firm but open) are associated with certain childhood outcomes (say, social competence). But as we have seen before, correlation does not neces­ sarily reveal cause and effect. There may be other possible explanations (see Figure 3 - 9 ) . Perhaps socially mature, agreeable children elicit greater trust and more reasonable treatment from their parents than do less competent and less cooperative children. Or perhaps some other unnoted characteristic of authoritative parents produces their children's competence. For example, such parents are less likely to be enduring the stresses of poverty or recent divorce (Hetherington, 1979), and they are more likely to be well educated—factors that might also be linked with children's competence. Or, as was suggested ear­ lier, maybe competent parents and their competent children share genes that predispose social competence. Thus, knowing that parents' behavior is related to their children's behavior does not prove cause and effect. 80 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n When considering "expert" child-rearing advice—of which there seems to be no shortage—we should also remember that it inevitably reflects the advice-giver's values. Even if we knew exactly how to en­ courage the development of any given trait in children, we could not advise parents without assuming that some traits are to be preferred over others. But which? Should the chief end of childhood be unques­ tioning obedience? Then an authoritarian style could be recom­ mended. Are sociability and self-reliance a higher end? Then firm but open authoritative parenting is advisable. Different experts have differ­ ent values, which, along with the uncertainties of cause and effect, helps explain their disagreements. Parents struggle with conflicting advice and with the other stresses of child-rearing. Indeed, the tens of thousands of dollars it costs to raise a child buys 20 years of not only joy and love but also worry and irritation. Yet for most parents, a child is a personal investment in the human future. To paraphrase psychiatrist Carl Jung, we reach back­ ward into our parents and forward into our children, and through their children into a future that we will never see, but about which we must nonetheless care. R E F L E C T I O N S ON A D E V E L O P M E N T A L I S S U E : G E N E S AND E X P E R I E N C E Our journey through developmental psychology is set in the context of three pervasive issues: whether development is (1) steered more by genes or experience, and whether it is characterized (2) by continuous growth or distinct stages, and (3) by stability or by change. Let's step back for a moment and take stock of current thinking on the naturenurture issue. Everyone agrees: Each of us is influenced by genes and experience working together. The real question is: How important is each? For physical attributes such as hair color, the genetic factor predominates. For psychological attributes, the answer is less obvious. In Chapter 12, Intelligence, we will examine the thorny debate over genetic and envi­ ronmental determinants of intelligence. Here, let us consider the pro­ vocative findings of several recent investigations into the inheritance of personal and social traits. TEMPERAMENT Infants and young children cannot take personality tests, so investiga­ tors observe their actual behavior and infer their temperament. Tem­ perament is a catchall term for the rudiments of personality and gener­ ally refers to the child's emotional excitability—whether the child is reactive (responds readily to stimuli), intense, and fidgety, or easygo­ ing, quiet, and placid. From the first weeks of life, "easy" babies are cheerful, relaxed, and predictable in feeding and sleeping. "Difficult" babies are more irritable, intense, and unpredictable (Thomas & Chess, 1986). More­ over, the most emotionally reactive newborns tend also to be the most reactive 9-month-olds (Wilson & Matheny, 1986), and the most emo­ tionally intense preschoolers tend to be relatively intense as young adults (Larsen & Diener, 1987). Physiological tests reveal that these temperamental qualities are linked to a reactive sympathetic nervous system (Kagan & others, 1988). Infant monkeys also vary in tempera- Faced with a mild stress, some children are characteristically more anxious, just as some m o n k e y s are naturally more fearful. C H A P T E R ment; from birth, some are timid and fearful, others more relaxed (Suomi, 1983). Are such temperamental differences hereditary? Several lines of evidence indicate they are indeed. Animal breeders selectively mate dogs, horses, and other animals to be either highly reactive or easygo­ ing. In one selective breeding study, Finnish psychologist Kirsti Lagerspetz (1979) took normal albino mice and bred the most aggres­ sive ones with one another and the least aggressive ones with one another. After repeating this for twenty-six generations, she had one set of fierce mice and one of placid mice. When researcher Stephen Suomi placed genetically predisposed "uptight" versus "easygoing" monkeys with foster mothers who were themselves uptight or easygoing, heredity tended to override rearing. The naturally uptight infant monkeys later reacted more anxiously to the stress of separation from their mother, even if they had been raised by easygoing, nurturant foster mothers (Asher, 1987). To judge from twin studies (see below), genes help determine human temperament, too. Moreover, newborns of different racial groups exhibit differing temperaments. Babies of Caucasian and Afri­ can descent tend to be more reactive and irritable than Chinese and Native American babies (who share a common Asian descent). For example, if restrained, undressed, or covered with a cloth, Caucasian babies will typically respond more intensely (Freedman, 1979). 3 T h e Developing Child M o s t N a v a j o babies calmly accept the cradleboard; C a u c a s i a n babies protest v i g ­ orously. Findings like these s u g g e s t that the rudiments of personality are to some extent genetically influenced. STUDIES O F T W I N S Selective breeding experiments seek to vary heredity but not environ­ ment, thereby revealing an effect of heredity. Could we also do the reverse—vary environment but not heredity—to seek the effect of en­ vironment? Happily for our purposes, in three or four human births out of every thousand, nature has given us ready-made subjects for this experiment. Identical twins develop from a single fertilized egg that splits into two genetically identical replicas, each of which be­ comes a person (Figure 3 - 1 0 ) . Fraternal twins, who develop from sep­ arate eggs, are genetically no more similar than ordinary brothers and sisters. Curiously, twinning rates vary by race. Caucasians have twins roughly twice as often as Asians, and half as often as blacks ( D i a m o n d , 1986). Figure 3 - 1 0 Identical twins develop from a single fertilized e g g ; fraternal twins from two. Do identical twins, being genetic replicas of one another, develop more similar personalities than fraternal twins? To find out, Birgitta Floderus-Myrhed and her colleagues (1980) administered tests of extraversion (outgoingness) and neuroticism (psychological instability) to nearly 13,000 pairs of Swedish identical and fraternal twins, as did Richard Rose and his colleagues (1988) with 7000 pairs of Finnish 81 82 PART 2 Development over the Life Span twins. Their findings: On both of these personality dimensions identi­ cal twins were much more similar than fraternal twins, suggesting that there is substantial genetic influence on both traits. Other dimensions of personality also reflect genetic influences. John Loehlin and Robert Nichols (1976) gave a battery of question­ naires to 850 identical and fraternal twin pairs who were identified while competing for National Merit scholarships. Once again, identical twins were substantially more similar, and in a variety of ways—in abilities, personality, and even interests. However, most of the identi­ cal twins also reported being treated more alike than did fraternal twins, raising the possibility that their experience rather than their genes accounts for their similarity. Not so, said Loehlin and Nichols, because identical twins whose parents treated them alike were not psy­ chologically more alike than identical twins who were treated less simi­ larly. T h e M i n n e s o t a Twin Study Better than studying identical twins who recall being reared differently would be to study identical twins who were reared in different environments. To the extent that such twins differ from one another (more than do identical twins reared together), one could only credit their differing environments. Thus in 1979 when University of Minnesota psychologist Thomas Bouchard read a newspaper account of the reuniting of 39-year-old twins who had been separated from infancy, he seized the opportunity and flew them to Minneapolis for extensive tests. Bouchard was looking for dif­ ferences; what "the Jim twins," Jim Lewis and Jim Springer, presented were amazing similarities (Holden, 1980a, 1980b). Both had married women named Linda, divorced, and married women named Betty. One had a son James Alan, the other a son James Allan. Both had dogs named Toy, chain-smoked Salems, served as sheriff's deputies, drove Chevrolets, chewed their fingernails to the nub, enjoyed stock car rac­ ing, had basement workshops, and had built circular white benches around trees in their yards. They also had similar medical histories: Both gained 10 pounds at about the same time and then lost it; both suffered what they mistakenly believed were heart attacks, and both began having late-afternoon headaches at age 18. Equally striking similarities were presented by identical twins Oskar Stohr and Jack Yufe, one of whom was raised by his grand- Identical twins: 6 years a g o these t w o girls shared the same fertilized e g g . " T h e Jim t w i n s . " Despite h a v i n g been separated at birth, these identical twins have remarkable similarities. Here each is s h o w n in his basement carpentry w o r k ­ s h o p , only one of the " c o i n c i d e n c e s " psy chologist T h o m a s Bouchard discovered. C H A P T E R mother in Germany as a Catholic and a Nazi, while the other was raised by his father in the Caribbean as a Jew. Nevertheless, they share traits and habits galore: They like spicy foods and sweet liqueurs, have a habit of falling asleep in front of the television, flush the toilet before using it, store rubber bands on their wrists, and dip buttered toast in their coffee. Stohr is domineering toward women and yells at his wife, as did Yufe before he was separated. Aided by publicity in magazine and newspaper stories, Bouchard, David Lykken, Auke Tellegen, and their colleagues have located and studied some four dozen pairs of identical twins reared apart. They continue to be impressed by the similarities not only of tastes and physical attributes but also of assessments of personality, abilities, and even fears (Tellegen & others, 1988). The more bizarre similarities— such as flushing the toilet before using it—exist not because we have genes for specific behaviors, contends Bouchard; rather, presented with a similar range of options, similarly disposed people tend to make similar choices. Even in the domains where heredity strongly influ­ ences personality (such as "social potency"—having an assertive, takecharge nature), it does so not through a single gene but through a complex combination of genes. Criticism of Twin Studies The cute stories do not impress Bouchard's critics. They contend that if any two strangers of the same sex and age were to spend hours comparing their behaviors and life histories, they would probably discover a string of coincidental similar­ ities. Even the more impressive data from the personality assessments (Bouchard, 1984) are clouded by the fact that many of the separated twins were actually together for several months before adoption, or had been reunited for some years before being tested. Moreover, adop­ tion agencies tend to place separated twins in similar homes. When people come from a narrow range of environments, the hereditary fac­ tor will play a bigger role. Nevertheless, the Swedish, National Merit, and Minnesota twin studies illustrate why scientific opinion is shifting toward a greater appreciation of genetic influences, and why further research is needed. 3 T h e Developing Child " I n s o m e domains it looks as t h o u g h our identical twins reared apart are . . . just as similar as identical twins reared together. N o w that's an a m a z i n g finding a n d I can assure y o u none of us w o u l d have e x ­ pected that degree of similarity." T h o m a s Bouchard ( 1 9 8 1 ) C o i n c i d e n c e s are not unique to twins. Patricia Kern of C o l o r a d o w a s born M a r c h 1 3 , 1941 and n a m e d Patricia A n n C a m p b e l l . Patricia DiBiasi of O r e g o n also w a s born M a r c h 1 3 , 1941 and n a m e d Patricia A n n C a m p b e l l . Both had fathers n a m e d Robert, w o r k e d as bookkeepers, and have children ages 21 and 19. Both studied c o s m e t o l o g y , enjoy oil painting as a h o b b y , a n d married military m e n , within 11 days of each other. T h e y are not g e ­ netically related. (From an AP report, M a y 2 , 1983.) ADOPTION S T U D I E S Adoption studies offer additional clues. For any given trait we can ask whether adopted children are more like their adoptive parents, who contributed a home environment, or their biological parents, who con­ tributed their genes. Sandra Scarr (Scarr, 1982; Scarr & Weinberg, 1983), John Loehlin and his colleagues (1982, 1985, 1987), and Robert Plomin and John DeFries (1985) therefore studied hundreds of adop­ tive families in Minnesota, Texas, and Colorado, respectively. The stunning finding of these studies is that people who grow up together do not much resemble one another in personality, whether they are biologically related or not (Rowe, 1987). Moreover, in many studies of families without twins, the personalities of parents have been astonish­ ingly unrelated to the personalities of their children. Sandra Scarr and her colleagues (1981) summarized the findings vividly: " T w o children in the same family [are on a v e r a g e ] as different from one another as are pairs of children selected randomly from the p o p u l a t i o n . " Robert Plomin and Denise Daniels ( 1 9 8 7 ) It would have to be concluded that upper-middle class brothers who at­ tended the same school and whose parents took them to the same plays, sporting events, music lessons, and therapists and used similar childrearing practices on them would be found to be only slightly more similar to each other in personality measures than to working-class or farm boys, whose lives would be totally different. 83 84 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n What we have here is developmental psychology's newest and one of its biggest puzzles: Why are children in the same family so different? Why do the shared genes and the shared family environment (the family's social class, the parents' personalities, the neighborhood) have so little discernible effect on children's personalities? Is it because even though siblings share genes, each sibling has a very different combina­ tion of genes? Is it because the different siblings of a family experience different environments (differing peer influences, birth orders, and so forth)? Might sibling differences be triggered by brothers and sisters comparing themselves with one another, perhaps unconsciously dis­ tancing themselves in an effort to create their own identities? And why are identical twins so much alike in personality when biological parent-child and sibling pairs are so little alike? Is it because identical twins share not only the same individual genes but also the same combinations of genes? Is it because they tend not only to be treated alike by parents and friends but, being the same age, also to experience the same cultural influences simultaneously? All these pos­ sibilities have been suggested. (Notice how in hindsight we begin to transform into tomorrow's common sense a finding that has stunned today's psychologists.) Adoption studies show that, although the personalities of adopted children do not much resemble those of their adoptive parents, adop­ tion has many positive effects. First, in their values and social atti­ tudes, adopted children are demonstrably influenced by their home environments. Second, in the adoptive homes studied, child neglect and abuse were virtually unheard of. So it is not surprising that nearly all the adoptive children thrived. They scored higher than their biologi­ cal parents on intelligence tests, and many became happier and more stable people than they surely would have in a neglectful environment. Children need not resemble their adoptive parents to have benefited greatly from adoption. HOW MUCH C R E D I T (OR BLAME) DO PARENTS DESERVE? Parents typically feel enormous pride in their children's successes, and guilt or shame over their failures. They beam when folks offer congrat­ ulations for the child who wins an award. They wonder where they went wrong with the child who repeatedly is called into the principal's office. Society reinforces such feelings: Believing that parents shape their children as a potter molds clay, people readily praise parents for their children's virtues and blame them for their children's vices. This chapter has provided some confirmation of the power of par­ enting. The extremes provide the sharpest examples—the abused who become abusive, the loved but firmly handled children who become self-confident and socially competent. Given our readiness to praise or blame, and to feel pride or shame, we do well also to remember a simple principle: Within the normal range of environments, children's genetically predisposed tendencies will assert themselves. Children are not so easily molded as clay. Moreover, as our next chapter illus­ trates, lives also are formed by environmental influences beyond par­ ents' control—by peer influences, by chance events, by all sorts of life experiences. It may be scary to realize how risky is the business of having and raising children. In procreation a woman and a man shuffle their gene decks and deal a life-forming hand to their child-to-be, who thereafter Studies of adopted families have provided n e w clues to hereditary a n d e n v i r o n m e n ­ tal influences on development. H o w simi­ lar w o u l d y o u expect these children to be to their adoptive parents a n d siblings? To their biological parents a n d siblings? C H A P T E R 3 T h e Developing Child is subject to countless influences beyond their control. Remembering that lives are formed by influences both under and beyond parents' control, we had best be restrained in crediting parents for their children's achievements and slower still to blame them for their children's problems. To say that genes and experience are both important is true, but an oversimplification. More precisely, their effects are intertwined. Imag­ ine two babies, one genetically predisposed to be attractive, sociable, and easygoing, the other less so. Assume further that the first baby attracts more affectionate and stimulating care than the second, and so develops into a warmer and more outgoing person. Moreover, as the two children grow older, the more naturally outgoing one seeks out activities and friends that encourage further social confidence. What has caused their resulting personality difference? One cannot truthfully say that their personalities are formed of x percent genes and y percent experience, for the gene-experience effect is combined. In fact, genes direct experience (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). As in our imagi­ nary example, one's genetically influenced traits may evoke significant responses in others. Moreover, as we grow older we select environ­ ments well suited to our natures. In such ways, our genes influence the experiences that shape us. Developmental psychologists examine how we develop physically, cognitively, and socially by studying the human life span from conception to death. quickly learn to discriminate the smell and sound of their mothers; and they may even be capable of imitating sim­ ple gestures. DEVELOPMENTAL I S S U E S INFANCY AND C H I L D H O O D Three issues pervade developmental psychology. First, to what extent is each of our traits influenced by our genes and to what extent by our experiences? Second, is devel­ opment a continuous process, or do we develop through distinct stages? Third, are our lives characterized more by stability of traits or by change? Physical Development Within the brain, nerve cells form before birth and, sculpted by experience, their intercon­ nections continue to multiply after birth. Infants' more complex physical skills—sitting, standing, walking— develop in a predictable sequence whose actual timing is a function of individual maturation rate. Childhood—from toddlerhood to the teen years—is a period of slow, steady physical development. PRENATAL D E V E L O P M E N T AND THE NEWBORN From Life Comes Life The life cycle begins as one sperm, out of the some 300 million ejaculated, unites with an egg to form a zygote. Two weeks later, the developing embryo attaches to the uterine wall, and after 2 months is a recog­ nizably human fetus. Along with nutrients, teratogens ingested hy the mother can reach the developing child and possibly place it at risk. The Competent Newborn With the aid of new methods for studying babies, researchers have discovered that newborns are surprisingly competent. They are born with sensory equipment and reflexes that facilitate their inter­ acting with adults and securing nourishment; they Cognitive Development Jean Piaget's observations of children convinced him—and almost everyone else—that the mind of the child is not that of a miniature adult. Pia­ get theorized that the mind develops by forming schemas that help us assimilate our experiences and that must oc­ casionally be altered to accommodate new information. In this way, children progress from the sensorimotor sim­ plicity of the infant to more complex stages of thinking. For example, at about 8 months, an infant becomes aware that things still exist even when out of sight. This sense of object permanence coincides with the development of stranger anxiety, which requires the ability to remember who is familiar and who is not. Piaget believed that preschool children are egocentric 85 86 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n and unable to perform simple logical operations. How­ ever, he thought that at about age 7 children become ca­ pable of performing concrete operations, such as those required to comprehend the principle of conservation. Recent research indicates that young children are not so incapable as Piaget believed. It seems that the cognitive abilities that emerge at each stage are developing in a ru­ dimentary form in the previous stage. Social Development Although the experiences of in­ fancy are not for long consciously remembered and their effects may largely be reversed by later experiences, they can nevertheless have a lasting influence on social devel­ opment. A social response of infancy that can critically affect later social development is attachment. Infants become attached to their mothers and fathers not simply because mothers and fathers gratify biological needs, but, more important, because they are comfortable, familiar, and responsive. If denied such care, both monkey and human infants may become pathetically withdrawn, anxious, and eventually abusive. Once an attachment forms, infants who are separated from their caregiver will, for a time, be distressed. Human infants who display secure attachment to their mothers generally become socially competent pre­ schoolers. accommodation Adapting one's current understandings (schemas) to incorporate new information. assimilation Interpreting one's new experience in terms of one's existing schemas. attachment An emotional tie with another person; evi­ denced in young children by their seeking closeness to the caregiver and showing distress on separation. basic trust According to Erik Erikson, a sense that the world is predictable and trustworthy; said to be formed during infancy by experiences with responsive caregivers. chromosomes Threadlike structures made of DNA mole­ cules that contain the genes. A human cell has twentythree pairs of chromosomes, one member of each pair coming from each parent. cognition All the mental activities associated with think­ ing, knowing, and remembering. concrete operational stage In Piaget's theory, the stage of cognitive development (from about 7 to 12 years of age) during which children acquire the mental operations that enable them to think logically about concrete events. conservation The principle (which Piaget believed to be a part of concrete operational reasoning) that properties such as mass, volume, and number remain the same de­ spite changes in the forms of objects. As with cognitive abilities, the concept of self develops gradually. At about age 18 months, infants will recognize themselves in a mirror. By age 10, children's self-images are quite stable, and are linked with their independence, optimism, and sociability. Children who develop a posi­ tive self-image and a happy, self-reliant manner tend to have been reared by parents who are neither permissive nor authoritarian, but authoritative without depriving their children of a sense of control over their own lives. Decisions about child-rearing involve value judgments about what traits in children should be encouraged. R E F L E C T I O N S ON A DEVELOPMENTAL I S S U E : GENES AND EXPERIENCE Studies of the inheritance of temperament, along with adoption studies and studies of twins, provide scientific support for the idea that genes influence one's developing personality. Developmentalists generally agree that genes and environment, biological and social factors, direct our life courses, and that their effects often intertwine, partly because our genetic predispositions influence our forma­ tive experiences. critical period A restricted time period during which an organism must be exposed to certain influences or experi­ ences if proper development is to occur; in humans there appear to be critical periods for the formation of attach­ ments and the learning of language. DNA (deoxyribonucleic acid) In cells, a complex mole­ cule containing genetic information. egocentrism In Piaget's theory, the inability of the pre­ operational child to take another's point of view. embryo The early developmental stage of an organism after fertilization; in human development, the prenatal stage from about 2 weeks to 2 months. fetus The developing human organism from 9 weeks after conception to birth. fraternal twins Twins who develop from separate eggs and sperm cells, thus ordinary brothers and sisters who have shared the fetal environment. genes The biochemical units of heredity that make up the chromosomes; a segment of DNA capable of synthesizing a protein. identical twins Twins who develop from a single fertil­ ized egg that splits in two, creating two genetic replicas. imprinting The process by which certain birds and mam- CHAPTER mals form attachments during a critical period very early in life. maturation Biological growth processes that enable or­ derly changes in behavior, relatively uninfluenced by ex­ perience. nature-nurture issue The longstanding controversy over the relative contributions of genes and experience to the development of psychological traits and behaviors. object permanence The awareness that things continue to exist even when they are not perceived. ovum The female reproductive cell, or egg, which after fertilization develops into a new individual. preoperational stage In Piaget's theory, the stage (from about 2 to 7 years of age) during which a child learns to use language but does not yet comprehend the mental operations of concrete logic. rooting reflex A baby's tendency, when touched on the cheek, to open the mouth and search for the nipple. schema A concept or framework that organizes and in­ terprets information. Berger, K. (1986). The developing person through childhood and adolescence (2nd ed.). New York: Worth. A comprehensive, and readable textbook summarizing what we know about infancy, childhood, and adolescence. Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1987). Know your child. New York: Basic Books. A respected wife-and-husband child psychiatrist team discuss how effective child-rearing adjusts to the temperamental traits of the individual child. Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 3 T h e Developing Child sensorimotor stage In Piaget's theory, the stage (from birth to about 2 years of age) during which infants know the world mostly in terms of their sensory impressions and motor activities. stranger anxiety The fear of strangers that infants com­ monly display beginning at about 8 months of age. temperament A person's characteristic emotional reac­ tivity and intensity. teratogens Agents, such as chemicals and viruses, that can reach the embryo or fetus during prenatal develop­ ment and cause harm. X sex chromosome The sex chromosome found in both men and women. Females have two X chromosomes; males have one. An X chromosome from each parent pro­ duces a female. V sex chromosome The sex chromosome found only in males. When it pairs with an X sex chromosome from the mother, a male is produced. zygote The fertilized egg; it enters a 2-week period of rapid cell division and develops into an embryo. A comprehensive overview of intellectual growth from infancy through adolescence. Includes discussions of the development of perception, memory, and language. Park, R. D. (1981). Fathers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni­ versity Press. A prominent developmental psychologist describes his own and others' studies of fathering. Scarr, S. (1986). Mother care/Other care. New York: Basic Books. An award-winning guide to child care by a leading developmen­ tal researcher. CHAPTER 4 Adolescence and Adulthood For much of this century, psychologists echoed William Wordsworth's sentiment that "the child is father of the man." In this view, by the end of childhood one's traits have nearly set, like clay. Although it remains for life experiences to smooth the rough edges, the really pliable period of development is over. The heritage of infancy and childhood will reach decades into the future, for the characteristic features of one's personality have been set. So long as psychologists held this view, they focused their atten­ tion on the "critical" early years. Now, among a new generation of developmental psychologists, the belief that no important changes in personality occur after childhood is giving way to a growing sense that development is lifelong. Yes, we are shaped during infancy and child­ hood, but the shaping continues during adolescence and well beyond. At a 5-year high school reunion, friends may be surprised at the diver­ gence of their paths. A decade after college, two former soul mates may have trouble communicating. As long as we live, we develop. ADOLESCENCE Adolescence extends from the beginnings of sexual maturity to the achievement of independent adult status. In preindustrial societies, the adolescent transition from childhood to adulthood typically lasts but a few days or weeks (Baumeister & Tice, 1986). Adult status and responsibilities are bestowed rather abruptly at the time of sexual mat­ uration, often marked by an initiation ceremony. In pre-twentiethcentury North America, young teenagers often labored as adults but were expected to behave as obedient children until marriage (Kett, 1977). Not until this century, when biological maturity began occurring earlier (because of improved nutrition) and adult labor was largely postponed until after compulsory schooling, did people begin to think of adolescence as a distinct period of life. What are the teen years like? To St. Augustine, these years were a time of fiery passions involving the hot imagination of puberty. . . . Both love and lust boiled within me and swept my youthful immaturity over the precipice. In Leo Tolstoy's Anna Karenina, the teenage years were, rather, that blissful time when childhood is just coming to an end, and out of that vast circle, happy and gay, a path takes shape. N o t until this century were y o u n g teen­ agers routinely spared the d e m a n d s of adult labor. Adolescents were expected to In her diary, written as she and her family hid from the Nazis, teenager Anne Frank observed, work as soon as they were physically able. 90 PART 2 Development over the Life Span My treatment varies so much. One day Anne is so sensible and is allowed to know everything; and the next day I hear that Anne is just a silly little goat who doesn't know anything at all and imagines that she's learned a wonderful lot from books. . . . Oh, so many things bubble up inside me as I lie in bed, having to put up with people I'm fed up with, who always misinterpret my intentions. To G. Stanley Hall (1904), the first American psychologist to de­ scribe adolescence, it was a period of "storm and stress"—of emotional turbulence caused by the tension between biological maturity and emotional and economic dependence. Indeed, after age 30, many peo­ ple look back on their teenage years as a time they would not like to relive (Macfarlane, 1964). They recall those years as a period when the social approval of peers was imperative, pressures for achievement ,. , <• i . .• • - n i were nerve-racking, one s sense of direction in life was in flux, and alienation from parents was deepest. Other psychologists have noted that for many, adolescence is often as Tolstoy described it—a time of vitality without the cares of adult­ hood, a time of congenial family relationships punctuated by only oc­ casional tensions, a time of rewarding friendships, a time of height­ ened idealism and a growing sense of life's exciting possibilities (Coleman, 1980). These psychologists would not be surprised that nine out of ten high school seniors agreed with the statement, "On the whole, I'm satisfied with myself" (Public Opinion, 1987a). Despite such conflicting observations, we can make some general statements about the most common physical, cognitive, and social changes of the adolescent years. PHYSICAL Have y o u any idea h o w y o u will look b a c l < y o u d ° n o i v° u r l i f e n g f a c t i o n t h e 1 t n i n 0 years from n o w ? Are s S k i n g the choices that s o m e d a y you will recollect w i t h s a t j s a n d m a ? DEVELOPMENT Adolescence begins at puberty, the time of rapid growth and sexual maturation. Puberty commences with a surge of hormones, which trig­ gers a 2-year period of rapid development that usually begins in girls at about age 11 and in boys at about age 13. Boys grow as much as 5 inches a year, compared with about 3 inches for girls—propelling the average male, for the first time in his life, to become noticeably taller than the average female (Figure 4 - 1 ) . During this growth spurt, the reproductive organs, or primary sex characteristics, develop dramati­ cally. So do the secondary sex characteristics, the nonreproductive traits of females and males, such as enlarged breasts and hips in girls, facial hair and a deepened voice in boys, pubic and underarm hair in both sexes (Figure 4 - 2 ) . The landmarks of puberty are the first ejaculation in boys, which usually occurs by about age 14, and the first menstrual period in girls, by about age 13. (These events do not necessarily signify fertility; it may be another year or more before ejaculations contain sufficient live sperm and the menstrual cycle includes ovulation [Tanner, 1978].) The first menstrual period, called menarche (meh-NAR-key), is an especially memorable event, one that is recalled by nearly all adult women. Most recall it with a mixture of feelings—pride, excitement, embarrassment, and apprehension (Greif & Ulman, 1982; Woods & others, 1983). Girls who are well prepared for menarche are the most likely to experience it as a positive life transition. And a transition it is: Regardless of the age at which their menarche occurs, shortly after­ ward most girls increasingly see and present themselves as different from boys and function more independently of their parents (Golub, 1983). Figure 4 - 1 T h r o u g h o u t childhood, boys a n d girls are similar in height. At puberty, girls surge ahead briefly, but then boys overtake them at a b o u t a g e 14. (From Tanner, 1978.) C H A P T E R 4 Figure 4 - 2 As in the earlier life stages, the sequence of physical changes (for example, breast buds before visible pubic hair before menarche) is far more predictable than their timing. Some girls start their growth spurt at 9, some boys as late as age 16. Such variations have little effect on height at maturity. However, they may have psychological conse­ quences. Studies performed in the 1950s by Mary Cover Jones and her colleagues revealed that for boys early maturation pays dividends. Early-maturing boys, being stronger and more athletic during their early teen years and seemingly less childlike, tend to be more popular, self-assured, and independent, and some of their greater sociability may continue into early adulthood. Adolescence and Adulthood Puberty c o m m e n c e s with a surge of hormones that trigger a variety of physical c h a n g e s . For girls, early maturation is less advantageous (Petersen, 1987). The 11-year-old who towers over her classmates and becomes sexually attractive before her peers do may temporarily suffer embarrassment and be the object of teasing. But as her peers catch up, in junior and senior high school, her postpubertal experience helps her to enjoy greater prestige and self-confidence. In some situations—dance train­ ing, for example—girls are more successful if they mature late (latematuring girls tend to have physical characteristics, such as leanness, preferred by dance masters [Brooks-Gunn, 1986]). This illustrates what makes for smooth adjustment: It's not only when we mature that counts, but also how people around us react to our physical develop­ ment. This exemplifies a recurring theme: Heredity and environment interact. In this case, how the environment responds to the youngster depends on the timing of maturation, as programmed by heredity. COGNITIVE D E V E L O P A A E N T Adolescents' developing ability to reason allows a new level of social awareness and moral judgment. As young teenagers become capable of thinking about their own thinking, and thinking about what other people are thinking, they become prone to imagining what other peo­ ple are thinking about them. As their cognitive abilities continue to grow, many adolescents begin to think about what is ideally possible and become quite critical of their society, their parents, and even their own shortcomings. Girls often begin their " a s c e n t " into p u ­ berty earlier than boys. T h i s can lead to social embarrassment for early a n d late bloomers alike. 92 PART 2 Development over the Life Span Formal Operations According to Piaget, preadolescents are re­ stricted to reasoning about the concrete, but adolescents become capa­ ble of thinking logically about abstract propositions. They can reason hypothetically and deduce consequences: if this, then that. Unlike chil­ dren, adolescents can deduce that when an investigator hides a poker chip and says, "Either this chip is green or it is not green," the state­ ment logically must be true (Osherson & Markman, 1974-1975). These developing reasoning skills define Piaget's final stage of cognitive growth, formal operations. Formal operations are the summit of intellectual development. Early adolescents may already have enough command of formal opera­ tions to learn algebra. But their ability to reason systematically, as a scientist might in testing hypotheses and deducing conclusions, awaits the full development of their ability for formal reasoning (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958). Piaget's critics have raised a number of objections to his depiction of formal operational intelligence. They maintain that the rudiments of logic can begin earlier than Piaget believed. Given a simple problem such as " I f John is in school, then Mary is in school. John is in school. What can you say about Mary?," many 7-year-olds have no trouble answering correctly (Patrick Suppes, cited by Ennis, 1982). Another criticism, which Piaget (1972) acknowledged, is that he overestimated the number of people who attain the level of formal logic. Many adolescents and adults seldom achieve formal operational reasoning, particularly if they are uneducated in the logic of science and mathematics. Consider this conversation between researcher Syl­ via Scribner (1977) and an illiterate Kpelle farmer in a Liberian village: " T h e principal novelty of this period is the capacity to reason in terms of verbally stated hypotheses and no longer merely in terms of concrete objects and their manipulation." Jean Piaget (1972) More on logical thinking in Chapter 11, Thinking and L a n g u a g e . Sylvia Scribner: All Kpelle men are rice farmers. Mr. Smith is not a rice farmer. Is he a Kpelle man? Kpelle farmer: I don't know the man. I have not laid eyes on the man myself. Kpelle villagers who had had formal schooling could answer Scribner logically. Nevertheless, it seems fair to conclude that the mind usually be­ comes capable of such reasoning in adolescence. One of the ways this new cognitive power manifests itself most frequently is in adolescents' pondering and debating such abstract topics as human nature, good and evil, truth and justice. Adolescents' logical thinking enables them to detect logical inconsistencies in others' reasoning, which can lead to heated debates with parents (Peterson & others, 1986). As Roger Brown (1965, p. 233) observed, As an adolescent one is amazed at adults who do not seem to realize the logical implications of their own ideas and who, still more unaccountably, do not make their actions consistent with their beliefs. The adolescent vows that he will never get to be like that; he will fight off whatever it is that clouds the adult intelligence. And suddenly he is ten years older, uncertain about everything and thoroughly compromised, trying to recall what it was he vowed to preserve. Moral Thinking A crucial task of childhood is learning right from wrong. But how do our notions of right and wrong develop? Following Piaget's (1932) contention that children's moral develop­ ment is tied to their cognitive development, Lawrence Kohlberg (1981, 1984) proposed that moral thought also develops through stages. To study people's moral thinking, Kohlberg (1981, p. 12) posed stories to children, adolescents, and adults in which the characters face a moral dilemma. Ponder for a moment his best known dilemma: The adolescent's g r o w i n g ability to think logically about abstract propositions illus­ trates Piaget's final stage of cognitive growth, formal operations. C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and A d u l t h o o d 93 In Europe, a woman was near death from a very bad disease, a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the doctors thought might save her. It was a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was charg­ ing ten times what the drug cost him to make. He paid $200 for the radium and charged $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick woman's hus­ band, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could get together only about $1,000, which was half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said, "No, I discovered the drug and I'm going to make money from it." Heinz got desperate and broke into the man's store to steal the drug for his wife. What do you think: Should Heinz have stolen the drug? Why was what he did right or wrong? Kohlberg would not have been interested in whether you judged Heinz's behavior as right or wrong—either an­ swer could be justified—but rather in the reasoning process by which you arrived at your judgment. We all are moral philosophers, Kohlberg proposed, and our moral reasoning helps guide our judgments and behavior (Berkowitz & others, 1986; Candee & Kohlberg, 1987). On the basis of his research, Kohlberg argued that as we develop intellectually we pass through as many as six stages of moral reasoning (see Table 4 - 1 ) . These six stages are divided into three basic levels: Table 4-1 KOHLBERC'S STAGES OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT Stage description Examples of moral reasoning in support of Heinz's stealing Examples of moral reasoning against Heinz's stealing Preconventional morality: morality of self-interest 1. Avoids punishment "If you let your wife die, you will get in trouble." "You shouldn't steal the drug because you' get caught and sent to jail if you do." 2. Gains concrete rewards "If you steal the drug, your wife will live, "He may not get much of a jail term if he steals the drug, but his wife will probably die before he gets out." Conventional morality: morality of law and social convention Gains approval/ avoids disapproval "If you steal the drug, your wife will live and you'll be a hero." "It isn't just the druggist who will think you're a criminal, everyone else will, too." Does duty to society/avoids dishonor or guilt "If you have any sense of honor, you won't let your wife die because you're afraid to do the only thing that will save her." "You'll always feel guilty for your dishonesty and lawbreaking." Postconventional morality: morality of abstract principles 5. Affirms agreedupon rights "His obligation to save his wife's life must take precedence. The value of human life is logically prior to the value of property." "It is so hard for people to live together unless there are some laws governing their actions." 6. Affirms own ethical principles "If you don't steal the drug, you would have lived up to the outside rule of the law but you wouldn't have lived up to your own standards of conscience." "If you steal the drug, you won't be blamed by other people but you'll condemn your­ self because you won't have lived up to your own conscience and standards of honesty." Source: Second a n d third columns adapted from The philosophy of moral development: Essays on moral development (Vol. I) b y L. Kohlberg, 1 9 8 4 , San Francisco: Harper & R o w . 94 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n preconventional, conventional, and postconventional. Before age 9, most children have a "preconventional morality" of self-interest: One obeys to avoid punishment (Stage 1) or to gain concrete rewards (Stage 2). By early adolescence, morality usually evolves to a more "conventional morality" that upholds laws and conventions simply because they are the laws and conventions. Being able to take others' perspectives, adolescents may approve actions that will gain social approval (Stage 3) or that will help maintain the social order (Stage 4). Those who become sophisticated in the abstract reasoning of formal operational thought may come to a "postconventional morality" that affirms people's socially agreed-upon rights (Stage 5) or that follows what their individual conscience perceives as universal ethical princi­ ples that may sometimes conflict with society's rules (Stage 6). Kohlberg's controversial claim was that these six stages form a moral ladder that extends from the immature, preconventional moral­ ity typical of the 7-year-old to, at its top rung, a recognition of what the person perceives to be fundamental ethical principles. As with all stage theories, the sequence is assumed to be unvarying. People begin at the bottom rung, and ascend to varying heights. Kohlberg's theory of moral development is provocative. It spells out some of the ways people determine right from wrong. It suggests why people who operate from different levels of moral thinking often clash in their political and social judgments. And it implies a strategy for moral education. In the tradition of Piaget, Kohlberg believes that moral thinking matures as children's minds actively confront moral challenges. Moral education is therefore consciousness-raising— raising a person's moral consciousness to a higher stage through dia­ logue concerning rules and moral issues. Provocative, yes. But is the theory valid? Does it accurately and helpfully describe moral development? Kohlberg's detractors offer two criticisms. The first emphasizes that, though our moral reasoning may largely determine our moral talk, it is but one of several influences on our actions. Morality, some critics say, involves what you do as well as what you think, and what we do is powerfully influenced by the social situation as well as by our inner attitudes. Many of the guards in the Nazi concentration camps were rather ordinary people who were cor­ rupted by a powerfully evil system (Arendt, 1963). Given the imperfect link between moral reasoning and moral action, say the critics, we should also concentrate on instructing children how to act morally in given situations and on setting a good example ourselves (Blasi, 1980; Gibbs & Schnell, 1985). T h e relationships between attitudes a n d a c t i o n s a r e explored in C h a p t e r 19, Soci influence. T h o s e w h o conform to society's rules often do not take kindly to those w h o seek to c h a n g e those rules. Kohlberg c o n ­ tends that S t a g e 6 moral thinking, e m ­ bodied here by Martin Luther K i n g , Jr., m a y be rejected by those w h o , not c o m ­ prehending, feel threatened by it. C H A P T E R The second criticism is that Kohlberg's theory has a Western cul­ tural bias. Children in various cultures do seem to progress sequen­ tially through the first three or four of Kohlberg's stages (Edwards, 1981, 1982; Snarey, 1985, 1987). However, the postconventional stages are most frequently found in educated, middle-class people in coun­ tries, such as the United States, Canada, Britain, and others that value individualism. The schema may therefore be biased against the moral reasoning of those in communal societies such as China and Papua New Guinea. Moreover, who is to say that the nonconformity of what Kohlberg calls the "highest" and most "mature" postconventional level is indeed morally preferable? Because Kohlberg's formative studies involved male subjects only, Carol Gilligan (1982) offered another critique: Stage 6 is morality from a male perspective. For women, she argues, moral maturity is less an impersonal morality of abstract ethical principles and more a morality of responsible social relationships. Thus, measured by Kohlberg's yardstick, women's moral differences are seen as moral deficits. Actu­ ally, contends Gilligan (1982, p. 173), women's concern for social re­ sponsibilities is a moral strength that complements men's concern with abstract ethics: "In the different voice of women lies the truth of an ethic of care." The accumulating evidence has provided little direct support for Gilligan's views (Blake & Cohen, 1985; Friedman & others, 1987; Rothbart & others, 1986; Thoma, 1986; Walker, 1986). Nevertheless, polls continue to find that women are more likely than men to favor the Democratic party, which generally promotes programs for the poor and disadvantaged. Might an "ethic of care" also help explain why women are more likely than men to select helping professions such as child care and social work, and why daughters rather than sons so often take primary responsibility for their elderly parents (Troll, 1987)? Kohlberg's critics agree that moral reasoning is linked with cogni­ tive development. Nevertheless, they caution that moral reasoning is but one determinant of moral action and that Kohlberg's postconven­ tional moral stages may be those of individualists in a Western culture. SOCIAL 4 Adolescence and Adulthood W h a t w o u l d happen to society if every­ one adopted S t a g e 6 moral reasoning a n d acted according to their o w n perception of universal ethical principles, with little regard for society's conventions? " I t is obvious that the values of w o m e n differ very often from the values which have been m a d e by the other s e x . " V i r g i n i a Woolf, A Room of One's Own, 1929 "This might not be ethical. problem for Is that a anybody?" DEVELOPMENT Theorist Erik Erikson (1963) contended that each stage of life has its own "psychosocial" task. As we saw in Chapter 3 (Table 3 - 2 , page 74), young children deal with issues of trust, then autonomy, then initiative. Later, between the ages of 7 and 11, they develop industry, the sense that they are competent and productive human beings. For adoles­ cents, the task is to synthesize their past, their present, and their future possibilities into a clear sense of self. Erikson calls this attempt to estab­ lish a sense of self the adolescent's search for identity. Forming an Identity "Who am I as an individual? What do I want to do with my life? What values should I live by? What do I believe in?" According to Erikson, arriving at answers that provide a stable and consistent identity is essential to the adolescent's finding a meaningful place in society. To gain this sense of identity, adolescents usually try out different "selves" in different situations—perhaps acting out one self at home, another with friends, and still another at school and work. If two of these situations overlap—as when a teenager brings home friends with whom he is Joe Cool—the discomfort can be considerable. The teen asks, "Which self should I be? Which is the real m e ? " Often, this role " I am b e c o m i n g still more independent of my parents; y o u n g as I a m , I face life with more c o u r a g e than M u m m y ; m y feeling for justice is immovable, and truer than hers. I k n o w w h a t I want, I have a g o a l , an opinion, I have a religion, a n d love. Let me be myself a n d then I am satisfied. I k n o w that I'm a w o m a n , a w o m a n with inward strength a n d plenty of c o u r a g e . " A n n e Frank, Diary of a Young Girl, 1947 95 96 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n confusion gets resolved by the gradual forging of a self-definition that unifies the various selves into a consistent and comfortable sense of who one is—an identity. But not always. Erikson believes that some adolescents form their identity early, simply by taking on their parents' values and expecta­ tions. Others may form a negative identity, one that defines itself in opposition to parents and society, complete perhaps with shaved head or multicolored, spiked hair. Still others never quite seem to find them­ selves or to develop strong commitments. For most, the struggle for identity continues throughout the teen years and reappears at turning points during adult life. The late teen years, when many people begin attending college or working full-time, provide new opportunities for trying out possible roles. As college seniors, many students have achieved a clearer iden­ tity than they had as first-year students (Waterman & others, 1974). This identity tends to incorporate a more positive self-concept than existed before. In several nationwide studies, researchers have given young Americans tests of self-esteem (sample item: "I am able to do things as well as most other people"). Between ages 13 and 23, the sense of self usually becomes more positive (O'Malley & Bachman, 1983). A clearer, more self-affirming identity is forming. During the teen years identity also becomes more personalized. Daniel Hart (1988) asked youth of various ages to imagine a machine that would copy either (a) what you think and feel, (b) your exact appearance, or (c) your relationships with friends and family. Asked "Which of these persons is closest to being you?," three-fourths of seventh graders chose the clone with the same social network; threefourths of ninth graders chose the clone with their individual thoughts and feelings. Erikson contends that the adolescent identity stage is followed in young adulthood by a developing capacity for intimacy, the ability to form emotionally close relationships. But to Carol Gilligan (1982), the "normal" struggle to create one's separate identity characterizes indi­ vidualistic males more than relationship-oriented females. Gilligan be­ lieves that females are less concerned than males with viewing them­ selves as separate individuals, and more concerned with intimate relationships. Thus females are less likely to exhibit Erikson's identitybefore-intimacy sequence (Kahn & others, 1985). By trying out different roles, adolescents try out different " s e l v e s . " A l t h o u g h some of their roles are uncomfortable for both the adolescents a n d their parents, most teenagers eventually forge a consistent a n d comfortable identity. C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood Relationships with Parents and Peers Are adolescents indeed pre­ occupied with separating themselves from their parents in order to form their own identities? Is adolescence a time of undeclared war between restrictive parents and their rebellious, independence-seek­ ing, identity-craving offspring? In Western cultures, adolescence is typically a time of growing peer influence and diminishing parental influence, especially on matters of personal taste and life-style. For example, the best predictor of whether a high school student smokes marijuana is simply how many of the student's friends smoke it (Oetting & Beauvais, 1987). Another predictor is working long hours at a job; high school students who do so are more likely to use drugs, perhaps because they spend more time with older co-workers who use drugs; perhaps, too, the need for cash to purchase drugs motivates them to work long hours (Bachman, 1987). Those who continue to live with their parents after high school show little change in drug use, while those who move in with peers become more likely to use drugs (Bachman & others, 1984). As peer influences grow, parental influ­ ences diminish. Does this mean that parents and their adolescents are estranged? For a small minority, it does. But for most, disagreement at the level of bickering is not destructive. "We usually get along but . . . ," adoles­ cents often report (Steinberg, 1987). Positive relations with parents ac­ tually support positive peer relations. High school girls who have the most affectionate relationships with their mothers tend also to enjoy the most intimate friendships with girlfriends (Gold & Yanof, 1985). Moreover, in most families the generation gap is easily bridged because it is rather narrow. In response to a 1977 Gallup poll that asked adolescents how they got along with their parents, 56 percent—the "teen angels," we might call them—said they got along with them "very well," 41 percent indicated "fairly well," and only 2 percent indicated they got along "not at all well." Indeed, researchers have been surprised at how closely most adolescents reflect the social, politi­ cal, and religious views of their parents (Gallatin, 1980). As often as not, what "generation gaps" there were on such issues merely in­ volved differences in the strength with which adolescents and their parents held their shared opinions and values (Figure 4 - 3 ) . M o s t teenagers say they generally have a g o o d relationship with their parents. " W h e n I was a boy of 14 my father was so ignorant I could hardly stand to have the old man around. But when I got to be 2 1 , I was astonished at how much he had learnt in seven years." Mark Twain, 1 8 3 5 - 1 9 1 0 Figure 4 - 3 H i g h school seniors' atti­ tudes appear to be in m u c h closer agree­ ment with their parents' than m a n y s u p ­ pose. A g r e e m e n t is greater, however, on basic values than on life-style choices. (From B a c h m a n & others, 1987.) 98 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n A D U L T H O O D AND AGING Until recently, adulthood, especially the center-of-life years between adolescence and old age, was commonly viewed as one long plateau. No longer. As we will see, those who have closely followed the unfold­ ing of people's adult lives have been impressed by the degree to which development continues. Physically, cognitively, and especially so­ cially, people at age 50 are quite different from their 25-year-old selves. Recognizing that adults do change, developmental theorists have proposed various stages of adult development, complete with transi­ tion periods. When people become independent of their parents and assume work roles, a transition from adolescence to early adulthood oc­ curs—at about age 20, give or take a few years depending on the cul­ ture and the individual. This period extends to about age 40, at which time a transition to middle adulthood is said to occur. Within the later adult years, many developmentalists distinguish the "young-old" postretirement years (65 to 75) from the "old-old" years (after 75) of more rapid physical decline. The labeling of life's phases is a convenient way to organize the adult years. But the labels are arbitrary, and the transition points are fuzzy. Moreover, by itself, age causes nothing. People do not get wiser with age; they get wiser with experience. People do not die because of old age; they die of the physical deterioration that accompanies aging. For that matter, during the adult years, age only modestly predicts people's traits. If you know only that Maria is a 1-year-old and Mere­ dith is a 10-year-old, you could say a great deal about each. Not so with adults who differ similarly in age. The boss may be 30 or 60; the mara­ thon runner may be 20 or 50; the reader of this book may be a teenager or a grandparent. Likewise, to be 19 can mean that one is a parent who supports young children or a student who still gets an allowance. The unpredictability of adult lives reflects the increasing impor­ tance of individual experiences. During the first 2 years of life, biologi­ cal maturation narrowly restricts our course. The infant who is strapped on a cradleboard and the one who moves freely will both walk and talk at about the same age. But as the years pass, we sail a widening channel, allowing the winds of experience to diverge our courses more and more. Individual life experiences make it much more difficult to general­ ize about adulthood than about life's early years. Yet our life courses are in some ways similar. Our bodies, our minds, and our relationships undergo changes in common with childhood friends who in other ways now may seem very different. PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT Although few of us are aware of it at the time, our physical abilities peak in early adulthood. Muscular strength, reaction time, sensory acuity, and cardiac output all crest by the mid-twenties. Like the de­ clining daylight after the summer solstice, the decline in physical prowess begins imperceptibly. Athletes are often the first to notice. World-class sprinters and swimmers generally peak in their teens or early twenties, with women (who mature earlier) peaking earlier than men. But most people—especially those whose daily lives do not re­ quire peak physical performance—hardly perceive the early signs of decline. " I a m still l e a r n i n g . " M i c h e l a n g e l o ' s motto, 1 5 6 0 , at a g e 85 C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood Physical Changes in Middle Adulthood In middle adulthood, physical decline gradually accelerates (Figure 4 - 4 ) , but even dimin­ ished vigor is sufficient for normal activities. Moreover, during early and middle adulthood a person's health and exercise habits have more to say about physical vigor than does aging. Many of today's physically fit 50-year-olds can run several miles with ease, while sedentary 25year-olds find themselves huffing and puffing on a jog around the block. The physical changes of adult life can trigger psychological re­ sponses, which vary depending on how one views growing older. In some Eastern cultures where respect and power come with age, out­ ward signs of one's advancing years are generally accepted, even wel­ comed. In Western cultures where the perceived ideal is youthful, smooth skin and a slim torso, the wrinkles and bulges that frequently accompany middle age can be a threat to self-esteem—something to try to avoid. But nature will not be denied; inevitably the lines appear, the youthful form begins to change its shape. For women, the most definite biological change related to aging is menopause, the cessation of the menstrual cycle, usually beginning within a few years of age 50. Menopause is caused by a reduction in the hormone estrogen and is sometimes accompanied by physical symp­ toms such as hot flashes and profuse perspiring. Some women also experience periods of anxiety, emotional instability, or depression. But like the stereotype of adolescent storm and stress, the image of meno­ pausal upheaval has given way to a recognition that menopause usu­ ally does not create significant psychological problems for women, nor does it greatly diminish sexual appetite or appeal (Newman, 1982). What determines the emotional impact of menopause is the wom­ an's attitude toward it. Does she see menopause as a sign that she is losing her femininity and sexual attractiveness and beginning to grow old? Or does she look on it as liberation from contraceptives, menstrual periods, fears of pregnancy, and the demands of children? To ascertain women's attitudes toward menopause, Bernice Neugarten and her col­ leagues (1963) did what, amazingly, no one had bothered to do: They asked questions of women, including those whose experience of men­ opause had not led them to seek treatment. When asked, for example, whether it is true that after menopause "women generally feel better than they have for years," only one-fourth of the premenopausal women under age 45 guessed yes; of the older women who had experi­ enced menopause, two-thirds said yes. As one woman said, "I can remember my mother saying that after her menopause she really got her vigor, and I can say the same thing myself." Social psychologist Jacqueline Goodchilds (1987) quips: " I f the truth were known, we'd have to diagnose [older women] as having P.M.F.—Post-Menstrual Freedom." Men experience no equivalent to the menopause—no cessation of fertility, no sharp drop in sex hormones. But they do experience a gradual decline in sperm count and testosterone level. Some may also experience psychological distress related to their perception of de­ creased virility and declining physical capacities. Physical Changes in Later Life Is old age "more to be feared than death" (Juvenal, Satires)? Or is life "most delightful when it is on the downward slope" (Seneca, Epistulae ad Lucilium)? What have we to look forward to? What is it like to be old? To gauge your own attitudes, take the following true/false quiz: D u r i n g middle adulthood, physical vigor depends more on exercise than on a g e . Kareem A b d u l - J a b b a r (left), w h o turned 40 in 1 9 8 7 , remains in great shape. 99 PART 100 2 Development over the Life S p a n 1. By 2030, 1 in 9 Americans will be 65 or older (see below). 2. The average life expectancy for a person 65 years old is 10 more years (see page 101). 3. During old age there usually occurs a substantial loss of neu­ rons in the brain (see page 101). 4. Older people become more susceptible to short-term illnesses (see below). 5. Approximately one-fourth of people age 65 and older live in institutions (nursing homes, hospitals, and homes for the aged) (see page 101). 6. If they live long enough—-to be 90 or more—most elderly people eventually become senile (see page 101). 7. Recognition memory—the ability to identify things previ­ ously experienced—declines with age (see page 102). 8. Life satisfaction peaks in one's fifties and then gradually de­ clines after age 65 (see page 109). 9. Among the elderly, there are twice as many widows than widowers (see page 110). 10. Many older people are preoccupied with a fear of death (see page 111). Life Expectancy The above statements—all false—are among the myths about aging that have been exploded by current research on North America's most rapidly growing population group. In 1900, those 65 and older accounted for 1 in 25 Americans; by 1987, thanks partly to strides in combating childhood diseases, the proportion in­ creased to 1 in 8. By 2030, when living members of the baby-boom generation born in the 1950s and early 1960s will all have reached 65, the 65 and over group will include 1 in 5 Americans. In the Third World, the elderly population is rising even faster—and will have dou­ bled between 1980 and 2000. The number of childless adults also is rising. Consider China, where there were 5 children per elderly person in 1955. In 2040, there will be but 2 children per elderly person (Hugo, 1987). Clearly, countries that depend on children to look after elderly persons are destined for major social changes. Sensory Abilities As we have seen, physical decline begins in early adulthood, but it is usually not until later in life that people become acutely aware of it. As visual acuity diminishes and the speed of one's adaptation to changes in light level slows, older people tend to have more accidents. Most stairway falls taken by older persons occur on the top step, precisely where the person typically descends from a window-lit hallway into the darker stairwell (Fozard & Popkin, 1978). Muscle strength, hearing, reaction time, and stamina also diminish noticeably. Thus, in later life it may seem as if the stairs are getting steeper, the newspaper print smaller, and people are mumbling more than they used to. Health Despite these signs of aging, the "young-old," especially those in good health with a positive attitude, continue to enjoy the vitality to maintain an active life. Indeed, although older people are more subject to long-term ailments, such as arthritis, they less often suffer short-term ailments, such as flu (Palmore, 1981). The similar good health in the preretirement years helps explain why older work­ ers have lower absenteeism than do young workers (Rhodes, 1983). C H A P T E R Although at birth the average American has a 75-year life expect­ ancy, current statistics show that those who survive to age 65 can ex­ pect to live until they are 82—or even longer if they are women, had parents who lived past 80, and maintain a healthy way of life, free of cigarettes and with regular exercise. One survey revealed that most elderly people think that the majority of their peers have some sort of serious health problem—although when asked about their own health fewer than one in four report that they have such a problem (National Council on the Aging, 1976). So it should not surprise us that only 5 percent of all those over 65 are residing in institutions such as nursing homes. Bear in mind, too, that age-linked changes need not be age-deter­ mined. For example, blood pressure and blood cholesterol are affected not so much by age per se as by age-linked changes in nutrition and exercise, and by the accumulated effects of smoking and drinking (Rowe & Kahn, 1987). To the extent that consumption and activity patterns rather than age itself determine "usual aging," we have it within our power to age in good health. However, aging does slow neural processes, and with age there is a small, gradual loss of brain cells, contributing to a 5 percent or so reduction of brain weight by age 80. But the proliferation of neural connections, especially in people who remain active, helps compensate for the cell loss (Coleman & Flood, 1986). Perhaps this helps explain the common finding that adults who remain active—physically, sexu­ ally, and mentally—tend to retain more of their functional capacity for such activities in later years (Jarvik, 1975; Pfeiffer, 1977). In general, "use it or lose it" appears to be sound advice. We are more likely to rust from disuse than wear out from overuse. A small number of adults do, however, suffer a tragic loss of brain cells. A series of small strokes, a brain tumor, or alcoholism can result in progressive brain damage causing senility. The most feared of all brain diseases, Alzheimer's disease, strikes 10 percent of the aging by age 75, and 20 percent of those 85 and older (Heckler, 1985). Some 2 million Americans are afflicted at a cost of some $40 billion a year, and the numbers are expected to double by the year 2000 as the population ages (Holden, 1987c). Alzheimer's destroys even the brightest of minds. Memory, and eventually reasoning and language, gradually deteriorate. As the dis­ ease runs its course, after some 3 to 20 years, the patient may become disoriented, then incontinent, and progressively lose mental function— a sort of living death preceding actual death. In its early stages, Alzheimer's disease is easily mistaken for men­ tal laziness. Robert Sayre (1979) recalls his father shouting at his af­ flicted mother to "think harder" when she could not remember where she had put something, while his mother, confused, embarrassed, on the verge of tears, randomly searched the house. Caregiving family members of increasingly confused and helpless sufferers can them­ selves easily become Alzheimer's exasperated and exhausted hidden victims. We now know that underlying the disease is a deterioration in neurons that produce the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, and that drugs that block the normal activity of this neurotransmitter produce Alzheimer-like symptoms (Weingartner, 1986). Investigators have also located on the twenty-first chromosome the genetic defect that causes one form of Alzheimer's. With continuing advances in our understand­ ing of the genetics, brain chemistry, and neural underpinnings of Alz­ heimer's, hopes for an eventual treatment grow. 4 Adolescence and Adulthood M o s t older people are in g o o d e n o u g h health to maintain an active life. A n d the more active they remain, the more vigor they retain. 101 102 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT One of the most controversial questions in the study of human devel­ opment is whether cognitive abilities such as memory, creativity, and intelligence follow a similar course of gradually accelerating decline during adulthood. Employers may wonder what they should do with their older workers. Retire them—or capitalize on their experience? Voters may wonder if a 75-year-old person retains the agility of mind, the flexibility of thought, and the judgment required to lead a nation. Aging and M e m o r y We do know that early adulthood provides the peak years for some types of learning and remembering (Craik, 1977). For example, David Schonfield and Betty-Anne Robertson (1966) asked adults of various ages to learn a list of twenty-four words. Some were then asked, without being given any clues, to recall as many words as they could from the list. As Figure 4 - 5 indicates, younger adults had better recall—a finding that parallels the greater ease with which younger adults recall new names and process complicated information (Zacks & Hasher, 1988). Others, given multiple-choice questions that asked them simply to recognize which words they had seen, exhibited no memory decline with age. So it seems that while our ability to recall new learning gradually declines during adulthood, our ability to recog­ nize what we have learned remains strong. Moreover, part of the recall difficulty that the elderly often complain of is attributable to normal forgetting. When Grandpa forgets where he put his car keys, he and we are more likely to blame his age than when his 20-year-old grand­ daughter mislays hers. The proficiency of adults in retaining newly acquired learning is also evident in classrooms. In recent years, American adults have re­ turned to school and turned to leisure education programs in increased numbers. Since 1973 the percentage of college students 35 years and older has nearly doubled (Grant & Snyder, 1986). By 1985, 38 percent of college students were age 25 and older (Center for Education Sta­ tistics, 1987). Despite occasional difficulties in adjusting to the de­ mands of coursework and testing, older students are generally success­ ful in their academic efforts. In fact, most older students do better than the typical 18-year-old, perhaps because they have clearer goals and are better motivated (Badenhoop & Johansen, 1980). Figure 4 - 5 In this experiment of recall a n d recognition in a d u l t h o o d , the ability to recall n e w information declined during early a n d middle a d u l t h o o d but the ability to recognize n e w information s h o w e d lit­ tle decline. (From Schonfield & Robertson, 1966.) C o l l e g e enrollments of p o s t - 3 5 - y e a r - o l d s are rising. Despite occasional adjustment problems, older students usually do better than the typical 1 8 - y e a r - o l d . C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood Aging and Intelligence Describing the cognitive abilities of older people is complicated. As we just noted, how well they remember depends: Are they being asked simply to recognize what they have tried to memorize (little decline) or to recall it without clues (greater decline)? Actually, the truth is even more complicated. If the information being recalled is meaningless—remembering nonsense syllables or saying five digits backward—then the older you are, the more errors you are likely to make. But the elderly's rich web of existing knowledge helps them catch meaningful information; thus their capacity to learn and remember meaningful material shows little decline with age (Labouvie-Vief & Schell, 1982; Perlmutter, 1983). What happens to our broader intellectual powers as we age? Do they gradually decline, like our ability to remember nonsense material, or do they remain nearly constant, like our ability to recall meaningful material? The evolving answer to this question makes an interesting research story, one that illustrates psychology's self-correcting process. At any stage of scientific inquiry, conclusions may be reached that seem sound, that meet ready social acceptance, and that shape social policy. But then an awareness grows of shortcomings in the research, and new studies must be done. This particular research story has pro­ gressed through several phases (Woodruff-Pak, 1989). Phase I: Cross-Sectional Evidence for Intellectual Decline In crosssectional studies, people of various ages are tested at the same time. When administering intelligence tests to representative samples of people, researchers consistently found that older adults gave fewer correct answers than younger adults (Figure 4 - 6 ) . As David Wechsler (1972), creator of the widely used adult intelligence test, put it, "the decline of mental ability with age is part of the general [aging] process of the organism as a whole." Until the 1950s, this rather dismal view of intelligence declining with age remained essentially unchallenged. Many corporations estab­ lished mandatory retirement policies under the presumption that the company would benefit by replacing aging workers with younger, pre­ sumably more capable, employees. As everyone "knew," you can't teach an old dog new tricks. Phase II: Longitudinal Evidence for Intellectual Stability Colleges began administering intelligence tests to entering students about 1920. So by the 1950s it was possible to find 50-year-olds who had taken an intelligence test 30 years earlier. Several psychologists saw their chance to study intelligence longitudinally, by retesting the same people over a period of years. What they expected to find was the usual decrease in intelligence after about age 30 (Schaie & Geiwitz, 1982). What they actually found was a surprise: Until very late in life, intelligence re­ mained stable, and on some tests it even increased (Figure 4 - 7 ) . How then are we to account for the previous findings from the cross-sectional studies? In retrospect, researchers saw the problem. Whenever a cross-sectional study compares people of different ages, age is not the only factor that influences the result. A cohort (genera­ tion) factor is also at work. The average 70-year-old and the average 30-year-old who were tested in 1950 were born and raised in different eras and circumstances, offering different educational opportunities. So when comparing 70- and 30-year-olds, one compares not only peo­ ple of two different ages and eras, but also generally less educated people with more educated people, people raised in large families with people raised in smaller families, and so forth. Figure 4 - 6 W h e n intelligence tests were administered to representative samples of adults of various ages (the cross-sectional method), older adults consistently g o t fewer questions correct than y o u n g e r adults. But see Figure 4 - 7 . (From Geiwitz, 1980.) Figure 4 - 7 In this test of verbal intelli­ g e n c e , w h e n the cross-sectional method w a s used (the same method as in the Figure 4 - 6 studies), scores were observed to drop with a g e . But w h e n the longitudi­ nal method w a s used (in which the same people were retested over a period of years) scores rose well into adulthood. (From Schaie & Strother, 1968.) 103 104 PART 2 D e v e l o p m e n t over the Life S p a n According to this more optimistic view, the myth that intelligence sharply declines with age had been laid to rest. As everyone "knows," you're never too old to learn. Witness Dr. John Rock, who at age 70 developed the birth control pill, Grandma Moses, who at age 78 took up painting and was still painting after age 100, and architect Frank Lloyd Wright, who at age 89 designed New York 's Guggenheim Mu­ seum. Moreover, when people have kept alive their expertise—typing, playing chess, playing the piano—their abilities often remain intact well into their eighties (Schaie, 1987). As psychologist David Krech (1978) said, "He who lives by his wits, dies with his wits." Phase III: It All Depends But the controversy was not, and still is not, over. For one thing, longitudinal studies have their own pitfalls. More intelligent people live longer and suffer less decline in intelli­ gence with age (Botwinick, 1977). Thus longitudinal studies may be selecting the very people whose intelligence is least likely to decline. If so, such studies underestimate the average decline in intelligence. Psychologists are increasingly convinced that intelligence is not a single trait (see Chapter 12, Intelligence). Intelligence tests that were designed to assess academic abilities—including speed of thinking— may place older adults at a disadvantage, because their neural mecha­ nisms for processing information are slower than those of younger people. And, in fact, some researchers find that on tests involving speed of response, senior citizens do perform relatively poorly, espe­ cially when the mental tasks are complex (Cerella, 1985; Poon, 1987). A 70-year-old is generally no match for a 20-year-old at video games. But slower need not mean less intelligent. Given other tests that assess general vocabulary, knowledge, and ability to integrate informa­ tion, older adults generally hold their own. Researcher Paul Baltes (1987) is developing tests of "wisdom" that assess traits such as exper­ tise and sound judgment in the problems of daily life. His results sug­ gest that older adults more than hold their own on such tests. Building on a distinction originally suggested by intelligence expert Raymond Cattell, John L. Horn (1982) proposed that we all possess two distinct types of intelligence, and that these are quite differently affected by age. Crystallized intelligence is basically the accumulation of stored information that comes with education and experience. For ex­ ample, tests of verbal ability, such as vocabulary tests, tend to reflect crystallized intelligence. Fluid intelligence reflects one's ability to rea­ son abstractly, and is less closely associated with one's stored knowl­ edge. For example, being able to identify the next letter in the series dfi m r x e reflects fluid intelligence. It turns out that crystallized intelligence increases with age, while fluid intelligence decreases. Per­ haps this helps to explain why mathematicians and scientists often do their most notable work during their late twenties or early thirties and psychologists peak about age 40, while those in literature, history, and philosophy tend to produce their best work later—in their forties, fif­ ties, and beyond, after more knowledge has accumulated (Denney, 1982; Horner & others, 1986). So, whether intelligence increases or decreases with age depends on what type of intellectual performance is measured. Born in 1 8 9 8 , A r m a n d H a m m e r continues his lifelong involvement in bettering SovietA m e r i c a n relations t h r o u g h his friendships with leaders in both c o u n t r i e s - a n d still guides a successful major corporation. " I n youth we learn, in a g e we u n d e r s t a n d . " Marie v o n E b n e r - E s c h e n b a c h , Aphorisms, 1883 Erik and J o a n Erikson have focused their attention on the end of the life cycle as they themselves have a g e d . N o w in their eighties, the Eriksons maintain that wis­ d o m has little to do with formal learning: " W h a t is real w i s d o m ? It comes from life SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT Most of the differences between early and middle adulthood are cre­ ated not by the physical and cognitive changes linked to aging but by life events—events often associated with family relationships and experience, well digested. It's not what comes from reading great books. W h e n it c o m e s to understanding life, experiential learning is the only worthwhile k i n d ; everything else is h e a r s a y . " C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood 105 work. A new job means new relationships, new expectations, and new demands. Marriage brings the potential for both the joy of intimacy and the stress of merging your life with someone else's. The birth of children introduces new responsibilities and significantly alters your life focus. A sudden shift in economic fortune may turn your world upside down, for better or for worse. The death of a loved one creates a sense of irreplaceable loss, and generally produces a need to reaffirm your own life. Any such life event represents a challenge that may significantly change you. To the extent that these major events of adult life are common, we may expect their influence to shape a predictable sequence of life changes. Ages and Stages of Adulthood As we have seen, Erikson (1963) theorized that the challenge facing the individual in young adulthood is that of achieving intimacy, of forming close, loving relationships. In middle age, the challenge is to achieve generativity—to become less self-absorbed, more productive, more caring for the world and its fu­ ture generations. This is generally experienced through the raising of a family and building of a career or business. The final task, or crisis, of adulthood is achieving a sense of integ­ rity—that is, arriving at a feeling that one's life has been worthwhile. Those who cannot do this—who feel that they have failed to realize their goals or to make a contribution to others' well-being—are likely to approach their final days with a sense of despair. Those who do achieve integrity look back on their lives with a sense of completion. Keenly aware of their mortality, they review their relationships and accomplishments and judge that, yes, life has been good (Table 4 - 2 ) . Table " P e r h a p s m i d d l e - a g e is, or should be, a period of s h e d d i n g shells; the shell of ambition, the shell of material a c c u m u l a ­ tions a n d possessions, the shell of the ego." A n n e Morrow Lindbergh, Cift from the Sea, 1955 " I t is a blessed thing to dispatch the busi­ ness of life before we die, a n d then to expect death in the possession of a happy life." Seneca, Of a Happy Life, A . D . 54 4-2 ERIKSON'S STAGES OF P S Y C H O S O C I A L DEVELOPMENT Approximate age Description of stage Infancy (1st year) Trust vs. mistrust If needs are met, infant develops a sense of basic trust. Toddler (2nd year) Autonomy vs. shame and doubt Toddler strives to learn independence and self-confidence. Preschooler (3-5 years) Initiative vs. guilt Preschooler learns to initiate tasks and grapples with self-control. Elementary School (6 years to puberty) Competence vs. inferiority Child learns either to feel effective or inadequate. Adolescence (teen years) Identity vs. role confusion Teenager works at developing a sense of self by testing roles, then integrating them to form a single identity. Young Adulthood (20-40 years) Intimacy vs. isolation Young adult struggles to form close relationships and to gain the capacity for intimate love. Middle Adulthood (40-65 years) Generativity vs. stagnation Middle-aged person seeks a sense of contributing to the world, through, for example, family and work. Late Adulthood (65 years and up) Integrity vs. despair Reflecting on life, the elderly person may experience satisfaction or a sense of failure. 106 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Other developmental psychologists have attempted to describe more precisely how people feel and act at various stages of adulthood. In the best known of these investigations, psychiatrist Daniel Levinson and his associates (1978) spent 10 to 20 hours talking with each of forty successful middle-aged men. Based partly on his impressions of their recollections, Levinson proposed a series of distinct stages of adult development (Figure 4 - 8 ) , which he considers confirmed by his more recent interviews with forty-five women (1986). Levinson believes that after a transitional time of breaking away from their preadult world, people devote their twenties to entering and exploring the adult world and "to creating a stable life structure" by embarking on careers and beginning a family. At the end of their twen­ ties, most begin a stressful transitional period in which they take stock of their lives and seek to restructure them in more satisfying ways. This accomplished, they settle down, tending to family life and seeking advancement in their careers. Then the cycle of stability and turbulence repeats. As they enter their forties they undergo a transition to middle adulthood, which for many is a crisis, a time of great struggle or even of feeling struck down by life. The dream of fame and fortune (or the illusion that such brings happiness) is given up, work and family com­ mitments are called into question, and turmoil and despair may result. They realize that they are no longer starting out, but rather drawing closer to the end. When this painful growth period is concluded, at about age 45, they again settle into new or deepened attachments, set about completing their careers, and become more compassionate and reflective without being tyrannized by external demands. Figure 4 - 8 Levinson's basic idea, then, is that life progresses in a predictable cycle of stability followed by rapid change. "Everyone goes through the same basic sequence," says Levinson (1986), and each "develop­ mental period begins and ends at a well-defined [average] age, with a range of about two years above and below this average." Other researchers are skeptical about any attempt at defining adult life as a series of neatly packaged stages, especially one based merely on interviews with a select few people, most of them high achievers. To generalize from their career-oriented lives or to use their "midlife crises" to explain or justify the renouncing of old relationships is both misleading and dangerous (Gilligan, 1982). The fact is that job dissatis­ faction, marital dissatisfaction, divorce, anxiety, and suicide do not surge during the early forties (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Scanzoni & Scanzoni, 1981; Schaie & Geiwitz, 1982). Moreover, the social clock—the cultural prescription of "the right age" to leave home, get a job, marry, have children, and retire—varies from culture to culture and era to era. In Turkey, 76 percent of brides are in their teens; in Belgium, only 35 percent are (United Nations, 1980). In Western nations, contemporary women are increasingly en­ tering the workplace and college classroom during middle adulthood, if not before. In earlier times, such ventures outside the home were often frowned on. Life Events and Commitments Given variations in the social clock and individual experience, the critics suspect that any proposed time­ table of adult ages and stages will have limited generality. More impor­ tant than one's chronological age are life events and the historical and cultural setting (being divorced in 1989 does not mean what it meant in 1955) (Harris & others, 1986). Marriage, childbearing, vocational changes, divorce, nest-emptying, relocation, and retirement mark transitions to new life stages whenever they occur—and increasingly Developmental periods i n early a n d middle male adulthood as pro­ posed by Levinson. (Adapted from Levin­ son, 1986.) Transitions in adult lives tend to be w o r k or family-related. Just as marriage, at a g e 2 8 , 4 8 , or 6 8 , heralds significant c h a n g e s , so does the attainment of a college d e ­ gree. C H A P T E R they are occurring at unpredictable ages. The social clock is still ticking, but people feel freer to be out of sync with it. Even chance encounters with people and events can have lasting significance, deflecting us down one road rather than another (Bandura, 1982). The 1950s actress Nancy Davis might never have met her future husband had she not, through a mix-up, begun to receive an­ nouncements of communist meetings intended for another person of the same name. Fearing that her career might be jeopardized by this mistaken identity, she went to see the president of the Screen Actors Guild. Before long she married this man, Ronald Reagan, and the rest is history (Reagan & Libby, 1980). Given the impact of chance encoun­ ters, it is small wonder that researcher Bernice Neugarten (1979, 1980) concludes that "adults change far more, and far less predictably, than the oversimplified stage theories suggest." It is therefore "a distortion to describe adulthood as a series of discrete and nearly bounded stages, as if adult life were a staircase." 4 Adolescence and Adulthood " T w o roads diverged in a w o o d , and I — I t o o k the one less traveled by, A n d that has made all the difference." Robert Frost, The Road Not Taken, 1916 Two basic aspects of our lives do, however, dominate adulthood. Erikson called them intimacy and generativity. Personality theorist Abraham Maslow (1968) described them as a need to love and belong. Researchers have chosen various terms—affiliation and achievement, attachment and productivity, commitment and competence. But Sigmund Freud (1935) put it most simply: The healthy adult, he said, is one who can love and work. For most adults, love is centered on family commitments toward spouse, parents, and children. Work is one's other productive activities, whether for pay or not. Love "Traditional" families—father, mother, and children under 18—compose only 28 percent of U.S. households (Bureau of the Cen­ sus, 1987). Who are the other 72 percent? They are the divorced and their children, the widowed, the older couples with an empty nest, the singles, the cohabiting men and women, and the childless or voluntar­ ily child-free married people. To judge from the U.S. divorce rate— now one-half the marriage rate—marriage has become a union that often defies management. Despite the alternatives to marriage and the celebration of single life, most adults marry and most who divorce remarry. Indeed, 95 percent of U.S. citizens age 40 and older have been married, and among women of this age group 9 in 10 have had a child (Bureau of the Census, 1987). At the same time, in many countries women are having fewer children. Since 1960, the proportion of American women in their T h e g r o w i n g number of single-parent households has altered the picture of the North A m e r i c a n family. Single parents must find new social supports, as these mothers have done with c o m m u n a l din­ ners, a n d learn n e w skills, as this father has done. 108 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n late twenties who are childless has more than doubled, from 20 percent to over 40 percent, while the average fertility rate has dropped from 3.6 children per woman to 1.8 (Kantrowitz, 1986; Public Opinion, 1986a). Perhaps the most enduring and significant of all life changes, hav­ ing a child is a happy event for most people. As children begin to absorb one's time, money, and emotional energy, however, satisfac­ tion with the marriage itself often declines, especially among employed women who find themselves bearing the traditional burden of in­ creased chores at home (Belsky & others, 1986). Another significant event in family life is the children leaving home. Consider your parents' experience: If you have left home, did your parents suffer an "empty nest syndrome"—was either of them distressed by a loss of purpose and relationship? Or did your parents discover renewed freedom, relaxation, and satisfaction with their own relationship? Contrary to the myth, the empty nest for most couples is a happy place. As Neugarten (1974) commented, Just as the major problem of middle-aged women is not the menopause, it is also not the empty nest. Most women are glad to see their children grow up, leave home, marry, and have their careers. The notion that they mourn the loss of their reproductive ability and their mother role does not seem to fit modern reality. No matter what the stereotypes tell us, it is not the way women talk when you listen. " A p p e a r a n c e s notwithstanding, for w o m e n , at least, midlife is not a stage tied to chronological a g e . Rather, it be­ longs to that point in the life cycle of the family w h e n the children are g r o w n a n d g o n e , or nearly s o — w h e n , perhaps for the first time in her adult life, a w o m a n can attend to her o w n needs, her o w n desires, her o w n development as a s e p a ­ rate a n d a u t o n o m o u s b e i n g . " Sociologist Lillian B. Rubin ( 1 9 7 9 ) Work A large part of the answer to "Who are you?" is the answer to "What do you do?" Much of what adults do to fulfill their need to feel productive and competent is (1) to raise children and (2) to undertake a career. Predicting people's career choices and guiding them toward satis­ fying occupations is a complex matter. Because it often takes time for people to settle into an occupation and because of the impact of chance encounters, there will always remain a large element of unpredictabil­ ity in career choices. During the first 2 years of college, most students cannot accurately predict their later career path. Most students shift from their initially intended majors while in college, many find their postcollege employment in fields not directly related to their majors, and most will change careers (Rothstein, 1980). To many career coun­ selors, this means that the best education is not a narrow vocational W o r k is a major focus of adulthood. C H A P T E R training, but rather a broad liberal education, an education that fosters "the critical qualities of mind and the durable qualities of character that will serve [people] in circumstances we cannot now even predict" (Gardner, 1984). Does work, including a career, indeed contribute to personal ful­ fillment as Freud supposed? During the 1970s and 1980s, one approach to answering this question has compared self-reported happiness among the roughly equal numbers of North American women who have or have not been employed. Despite changing employment rates (see Figure 4 - 9 ) and shifts in social attitudes regarding women's roles, the happiness difference between the two groups—which slightly fa­ vors employed women—has always been far smaller than the personto-person differences within each group (Adelmann, 1988; Campbell, 1981). Grace Baruch and Rosaline Barnett (1986) of the Wellesley Col­ lege Center for Research on Women have found that what matters is not which role a woman occupies—as paid worker, wife, and/or mother—but the quality of her experience in that role. Happiness is having work that fits your interests and provides a sense of compe­ tence and accomplishment; having a partner who is a close, supportive companion and who sees you as special; having loving children whom you like and feel proud of. 4 Adolescence and Adulthood Figure 4 - 9 109 T h e rising percentage o f e m p l o y e d A m e r i c a n w o m e n as reported by the U . S . Bureau of Labor Statistics. " O n e c a n live magnificently in this world if one k n o w s h o w to w o r k a n d h o w to love." Weil-Being Across the Life Span Researchers have also compared the sense of well-being among young and old. Who do you suppose are the happiest? The carefree youth? The up-and-coming young adults? The successful and secure middle aged? Or those enjoying the leisurely retired life? To live is to grow older, which means that all of us can look back­ ward with satisfaction or sorrow and forward with hope or dread. Ad­ olescents are buffeted by mood swings and insecurity, parental power and peer pressures, identity confusions and career worries. In later life, income shrinks, work has been taken away, the body deteriorates, recall fades, energy wanes, family members and friends die or move away, and the great enemy, death, looms ever closer. Small wonder that we presume the teen and over-65 years to be the worst of times (Freedman, 1978). Surprisingly, they are not. When people of all ages describe their own feelings of happiness or sense of life satisfaction, there is no tend­ ency for people of any particular age to report greater feelings of wellbeing. One statistical digest of results from 119 studies revealed that less than 1 percent of the person-to-person variation in well-being was attributable to age (Stock & others, 1983). Illustrative are the pooled data from 5 years of recent surveys in eight Western European coun­ tries. How many Europeans reported themselves "very happy"? By age group, 19 percent of the 15- to 24-year-olds, 17 percent of the 35- to 40-year-olds, and 19 percent of those 65 and older! And how many were "satisfied" or "very satisfied" with life as a whole? Equal propor­ tions: 78 percent of the 15- to 24-year-olds, 78 percent of the 35- to 44-year-olds, and 78 percent of those over 65 (Inglehart & Rabier, 1986). Whatever the explanation—reduced stress, lowered aspirations, newfound sources of pleasure—the bottom line from hundreds of thousands of interviews with people of all ages in many countries is this: Older people report as much happiness and satisfaction with life as younger people do. Given that growing older is one sure conse­ quence of living, an outcome that most of us prefer to its alternative, we can all take comfort in this. Leo Tolstoy, 1 8 5 6 " H o w m a n y of us older persons have really been . . . prepared for the second half of life, for old a g e , death and eternity?" Carl J u n g , Modern Man in Search of a Soul, "/ used to be old, but it wasn't my cup 1933 too, of tea." 110 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n W i t h the tasks of the earlier adult years behind t h e m , m a n y older adults have more time to enjoy pursuing their per­ sonal interests. No w o n d e r their satisfac­ tion with life is generally h i g h , especially if they are healthy. The astonishing stability of well-being across the life span obscures some interesting age-related emotional differences. As the years go by, feelings mellow (Costa & others, 1987; Diener & others, 1986). Highs become less high, lows less low. Thus while the average feeling level may remain stable, with age we find ourselves less often feeling ex­ cited, intensely proud, and on top of the world, but also less often depressed. Compliments provoke less elation and criticisms less de­ spair as both become merely additional feedback atop a mountain of accumulated praise and blame. University of Chicago psychologists Mihalyi Csikszentmihalyi and Reed Larson (1984) mapped people's emotional terrain by periodically signaling them with electronic beep­ ers to report their current activities and feelings. They found that teen­ agers typically come down from elation or up from gloom in less than an hour. Adult moods are less extreme but more enduring. Old age offers less intense joy but more feelings of contentment. Death and D y i n g Most of us will suffer and cope with the deaths of relatives and friends. Usually, the most difficult separation is from one's spouse—a loss suffered by five times more women than men. Grief is especially severe when the death of a loved one comes before its expected time on the social clock. The accidental death of a child or the sudden illness that claims a 45-year-old spouse may trigger a year or more of mourning flooded with memories, eventually subsiding to a mild depression that sometimes continues for several additional years (Lehman & others, 1987). Contrary to popular myths, those who ex­ press the strongest grief immediately do not resolve their grief more quickly (Wortman & Silver, 1987). The death of a child is one of the most traumatic events imaginable; especially if the child was healthy, the mother's anguish can be excruci­ ating (Littlefield & Rushton, 1986). Although most parents emerge from the tragedy intact, for some the shadow of separation and loss endures; many become less materialistic and more religious (Knapp, 1987). Those who suffer a terminal illness live with the realization of their impending death. In analyzing how people cope with the prospect of death, the stage theorists have once again arrived ahead of us. From " G r o w old a l o n g with m e ! T h e best is yet to be.' Robert B r o w n i n g , Rabbi Ben Ezra, 1864 For survivors, s u c h as this grieving w i d o w on M e m o r i a l D a y , death is the ultimate separation. C H A P T E R her interviews with dying patients, Elizabeth Kiibler-Ross (1969) pro­ posed that the terminally ill pass through a sequence of five stages: denial of the terminal condition, anger and resentment ("Why me?"), bargaining with God (or physicians) for more time, depression stemming from the impending loss of everything and everyone, and, finally, peaceful acceptance of one's fate. The critics of Kiibler-Ross's proposal first question the generality of the stages, stressing that each dying person's experience is unique. Moreover, they argue, the simplified stages ignore many important factors; for example, that people who are old usually view death with less expressed fear and resentment (Wass & others, 1978-1979). Critics also express concern about the eagerness with which the death-anddying formula has been popularized in courses and books. The danger, they fear, is that rather than having their feelings respected, dying people may be analyzed or manipulated in terms of the stereotyped stages: "She's just going through the anger stage." Nevertheless, the death-education movement has enabled us to deal more openly and humanely with death and grief. A growing num­ ber of individuals are aided by hospice organizations, whose staff and volunteers work in special facilities and in people's homes to support the terminally ill and their families. We can be grateful that deathdenying attitudes are being dislodged. Facing death with dignity and openness helps a person to complete the life cycle with a sense of the meaningfulness and unity of life—the sense that their existence has been good and that life and death have their places in an ongoing cycle. Although death may be unwelcome, life itself can be affirmed even at death. REFLECTIONS ON DEVELOPMENTAL ISSUES Our survey of developmental psychology began in Chapter 3 by identi­ fying three pervasive issues: (1) whether development is steered more by genes or experience, (2) whether development is a gradual, continu­ ous process or a discrete series of stages, and (3) whether development is characterized more by stability over time or by change. We con­ cluded there that heredity and environment jointly affect human de­ velopment. Let's now take stock of current thinking on the latter two issues. CONTINUITY A N D S T A G E S Chapters 3 and 4 have described three major stage theories: Jean Pia­ get's theory of cognitive development, Lawrence Kohlberg's theory of moral development, and Erik Erikson's theory of psychosocial devel­ opment. As we have seen, these stage theories have been vigorously criticized as failing to recognize the early rudiments of later abilities (Piaget), as biased by a worldview characteristic of educated people in individualistic cultures (Kohlberg), or as contradicted by research dem­ onstrating that adult life does not progress up a fixed series of steps (Erikson and Levinson). Nevertheless, there do seem to be some spurts of brain growth during childhood and puberty, corresponding roughly to Piaget's stages (Thatcher & others, 1987). Moreover, stage theories have served to encourage a developmental perspective on the whole life span by suggesting how people of one age think and act differently when they arrive at a later age. 4 Adolescence and Adulthood 111 " D o not g o gentle into that g o o d night, O l d a g e should burn a n d rave at close of d a y ; R a g e , rage against the dying of the light." Dylan T h o m a s , Do Not Co Gentle into That Good Night, 1 9 5 2 , a p o e m written to his father as he lay d y i n g peacefully Hospice workers seek to enable those w h o are d y i n g to live a n d die with dignity a n d to aid their families in dealing with the impending loss. T h e hospice m o v e ­ ment is but one sign of the more open a n d understanding attitudes toward death a n d grief in North A m e r i c a today. 112 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n STABILITY AND CHANGE This leads us to the final question: Over time, are people's personalities consistent, or do they change? If reunited with a long-lost grade school friend, would you instantly recognize that "it's the same old Andy"? Or is a person during one period of life likely to be quite different at a later period? Obviously, either extreme is false: If there were no stabil­ ity, we could not hope that the person we marry today would be the same person a decade later, or that the promising management trainee would in the future remain suited for management. If there were no change possible, all juvenile delinquents would become career crimi­ nals; all alcoholics would drink themselves into the grave; and life would be one long rut, without growth or the possibility of betterment. Still the issue is real: How well do infants' traits predict their child­ hood characteristics? Is the troubled adolescent likely to have a rocky adulthood? Will the assertive young woman still be noticeably asser­ tive at age 60? Researchers who have followed lives through time are debating the extent to which our past reaches into our future. For most of this century it was assumed that once personality is formed by life's early experiences, it remains set for life. Then, during the late 1960s and 1970s, new findings suggested that throughout much of life one's personality can still be shaped. Let us allow some of the researchers to speak for themselves. Jerome Kagan and Howard Moss related observations made of sev­ eral dozen Ohio children during their first 14 years of life to their adult traits. Kagan (1978) found "little relation between psychological quali­ ties during the first 3 years of life—fearfulness, irritability, or activity— and any aspect of behavior in adulthood." Not until 6 to 10 years of age did the child's behavior begin to predict the kind of adult the child would become. Kagan (1982, 1988) sums up: "The first two or three years of life generally represent a poor basis from which to predict anything important about adulthood. . . . The capacity for change in an ordinary child is enormous." Jean Macfarlane (1964) followed 166 lives from babyhood to age 30 and discovered that "many of our most mature and competent adults had severely troubled and confusing childhoods and adolescences." Often, the unhappy, rebellious adolescent became a stable, successful, and happy adult. Alexander Thomas and Stella Chess (1986) similarly followed 133 people from infancy to early adulthood and found that the troubled children among them usually became stable adults. In addition, some researchers have found that even adults may undergo surprising and unpredictable changes. Reflecting on her stud­ ies of such changes during the life cycle, for example, Bernice Neugarten (1980) reported that "the primary consistency we have found is a lack of consistency." So, shall we conclude that later experience records over early expe­ rience, erasing the voices of the past? If so, we can counsel parents of difficult babies and teenagers to be patient and hopeful. The de­ pressed, lonely young adult can be reassured that development never ends: The struggles of the present may lay the foundation for a happier future than now seems possible. On the other hand, a considerable body of recent research has found that there is also consistency to personality. After painstakingly comparing people in their forties with ratings of the same people as junior high students, Jack Block (1981) concluded that there is an un­ derlying stability to our basic social and emotional style. The troubled adolescent sometimes turned out better than we would have guessed, but on the whole, it was the cheerful teenager who tended to become "Mr. Coughlin of one over there of the was the founder first motorcycle gangs." " W h e t h e r o n e is an extreme hereditarian, an environmentalist, a constitutionalist, or an orthodox psychoanalyst, he is not likely to anticipate major c h a n g e s in per­ sonality after the first f e w years of life." E. Lowell Kelly ( 1 9 5 5 ) C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood Figure 4 - 1 0 T h e stability o f aggressive­ ness. Leonard Eron and Rowell H u e s m a n n ( 1 9 8 4 ) f o u n d that the degree of a g g r e s ­ siveness displayed by 8-year-old boys (low, m e d i u m , or h i g h — a s s h o w n by the colored bars) helped predict their a g g r e s ­ siveness t w o decades later, as testified to both by their w i v e s ' reports a n d by the n u m b e r of their criminal convictions. Similarly, Avshalom Caspi and his colleagues (1987) found that 9-year-olds with explosive temper tantrums were more likely than oth­ ers to have trouble keeping good jobs during adulthood. Compared to children who did not have tantrums, they were also more than twice as likely to have divorced by age 40. And Leonard Eron (1987) and others found that the most physically aggressive 8-year-olds often became the most aggressive (and potentially violent) 30-year-olds (Figure 4 - 1 0 ) . Once people reach adulthood, their dispositions become more sta­ ble. From their periodic retesting of Boston and Baltimore area adults, Robert McCrae and Paul Costa (1982) concluded: "For the great major­ ity of people, the self-concept at age 30 is a good guide to personality at age 80." During the adult years, people's outgoingness and emotional instability are equally persistent (Conley, 1985; Finn, 1986). So, what should we conclude from these somewhat conflicting findings—that we retain throughout the life span a considerable capac­ ity for change but that our basic social and emotional style becomes more ingrained as we grow older? Whatever their disagreements, re­ searchers agree that: 1. The first 2 years of life provide a poor basis for predicting a person's eventual traits. As people grow older, predictability increases. For example, there is less stability from age 14 to 18 than from 18 to 22 (Stein & others, 1986). 2. The greater the span of years between assessments, the greater the likelihood that personality will have changed. From assess­ ments of the traits of 25-year-olds, psychologists can better predict their personalities as 35-year-olds than as 55-year-olds (Conley, 1984). 3. Some characteristics, such as temperament, are more stable than others, such as social attitudes (Moss & Susman, 1980). 4. In some ways, we all change with age. Most shy, fearful tod­ dlers begin opening up by age 4, and during adulthood most of us mellow. Such changes can occur without modifying a per­ son's position relative to others of the same age. The hard-driv­ ing young adult may have mellowed by later life but may still be recognized as a relatively hard-driving senior citizen. Finally, it is important to remember that life contains both stability and change. The fact of stability enables us to depend on others and motivates our concern for the healthy development of children. The fact of change motivates our concerns about present influences and " A t 7 0 , I w o u l d say the a d v a n t a g e is that y o u take life more calmly. Y o u k n o w that 'this, too, shall p a s s ! ' " Eleanor Roosevelt, 1 9 5 4 113 114 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n SUMMING UP The overriding assumption of modern developmental psychology is that development is lifelong. We grow and change and adapt during infancy and childhood, and dur­ ing adolescence and adulthood. ADOLESCENCE Physical Development Adolescence typically begins at puberty with the onset of rapid growth and developing sexual maturity. Depending on how other people react, early or late maturation can influence adjustment; this il­ lustrates how genes and environment interact in shaping our development. Cognitive Development Piaget theorized that adoles­ cents develop the capacity for formal operations, which enables them to reason abstractly. However, some developmentalists believe that the development of formal logic depends on schooling as well, and that the rudiments of logic appear earlier than Piaget believed. Following Piaget, Lawrence Kohlberg contended that moral thinking likewise proceeds through a sequence of stages, from a preconventional morality of self-interest, to a conventional morality concerned with gaining others' approval or doing one's duty, to (in some people) a postconventional morality of agreed-upon rights or universal ethical principles. Other theorists respond that morality also lies in one's actions, which are influenced by one's social situation and inner attitudes as well as by one's moral reasoning. Moreover, say Kohlberg's critics, the postconventional stages represent morality from the per­ spective of individualistic, liberal-minded males. Social Development Erik Erikson theorized that a chief task of adolescence is to form one's sense of self—one's identity. For many people the struggle for identity contin­ ues in the adult years as new relationships emerge and new roles are assumed. While adolescence has tradition­ ally been viewed as a time of storm and stress, researchers have found that most teenagers relate to their parents rea­ sonably well and generally affirm their parents' beliefs and attitudes. ADULTHOOD AND AGING During early life, we sail a narrow channel, constrained by maturation. As the years pass, the channel widens, allowing us to diverge more and more. By adulthood, age no longer neatly predicts a person's experiences and traits. Yet in some ways our bodies, minds, and relation­ ships still undergo predictable changes. As long as we live, we develop. Physical Development The barely perceptible physical declines of early adulthood begin to accelerate during middle adulthood. For women, a significant physical change of adult life is menopause, which generally seems to be a smooth rather than rough transition. After 65, de­ clining perceptual acuity, strength, and stamina are evi­ dent, but short-term ailments are fewer. Although neural processes slow, the brain nevertheless remains healthy, except for those who suffer brain disease, such as the pro­ gressive deterioration of Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive Development As the years pass, recognition memory remains strong, although recall memory begins to decline, especially for novel types of information. Research on how intelligence changes with age has progressed through several phases: cross-sectional stud­ ies suggesting a steady intellectual decline after early adulthood; longitudinal studies suggesting intellectual stability until very late in life; and an alternative view that, while fluid intelligence declines in later life, crystallized intelligence does not. Social Development Several theorists maintain that adults progress through an orderly sequence of stages. Erikson proposes that after the formation of an identity, the young adult must deal with intimacy. The develop­ mental tasks that follow involve generativity in middle adulthood, and in later adulthood a sense of integrity. Daniel Levinson contends that moving from one stage to the next entails recurring times of crisis, such as the earlyforties time of transition to midlife. Critics contend that people are not so predictable; life events involving love and work, and even chance events and encounters, influ­ ence adult life in unanticipated ways. Although few grow old gratefully, most do so grace­ fully, retaining a sense of well-being throughout the life span. Those who live to old age must, however, cope with the deaths of friends and family members and with the prospect of their own deaths. R E F L E C T I O N S ON THE D E V E L O P M E N T A L ISSUES Chapters 3 and 4 (and Chapter 5, Gender, which follows) touch three pervasive issues in developmental psychol­ ogy: nature versus nurture, continuity versus discrete stages, and stability versus change in personality. Al­ though the stage theories of Piaget, Kohlberg, and Erik­ son have been modified in light of later research, the theo­ ries usefully alert us to differences among people of different ages. The discovery that people's traits continue to change in later life has helped create the new emphasis that development is lifelong. Nevertheless, research dem­ onstrates that there is also an underlying consistency to most people's temperaments and personality traits, espe­ cially after infancy and early childhood. C H A P T E R 4 Adolescence and Adulthood TERMS AND CONCEPTS TO REMEMBER adolescence The period from puberty to independent adulthood; in industrialized nations, roughly the teen years. integrity In Erikson's theory, the positive outcome of later life: a nondespairing sense that one's life has been meaningful and worthwhile. Alzheimer's disease A progressive and irreversible brain disorder characterized by gradual deterioration of mem­ ory, reasoning, language, and finally, of physical func­ tion. intimacy In Erikson's theory, the ability to form close, loving relationships; the primary developmental task of early adulthood. cross-sectional study A study in which people of differ­ ent ages are tested or observed at a given time. crystallized intelligence One's accumulated information and verbal skills; tends to increase with age. fluid intelligence One's ability to reason abstractly; tends to decrease during later adulthood. formal operational stage In Piaget's theory, the stage of cognitive development (normally beginning about age 12) during which people learn to think logically about abstract concepts. generativity In Erikson's theory, the impulse to be pro­ ductive, such as by raising children and doing creative work; a major focus during middle adulthood. hospice Organizations whose largely volunteer staff members provide support for dying people and their fam­ ilies either in special facilities or in people's own homes. longitudinal study Research in which the same people are restudied over a long period of time. menarche [meh-NAR-key] The first menstrual period. menopause The cessation of menstruation. Also used loosely to refer to the biological and psychological changes during the several years of declining ability to reproduce. primary sex characteristics The body structures (ovaries and gonads) that make sexual reproduction possible. puberty The early adolescent period of rapid growth and sexual maturation. secondary sex characteristics Nonreproductive sexual characteristics such as female breasts and hips, male voice quality, and body hair. social clock The culturally preferred timing of social events such as marriage, childbearing, and retirement. identity One's sense of self. According to Erikson, the adolescent's task is to form a sense of self by integrating various roles. FOR FURTHER READING Berger, K. (1988). The developing person through the life span (2nd ed.). New York: Worth. A comprehensive, current, and readable textbook summary of what we know about infancy, childhood, adolescence, and adult­ hood. Butler, R. N. (1975). Why survive? Being old in America. New York: Harper & Row. A Pulitzer prize-winning book that portrays the experience of older people in the United States. Damon, W. (1988). The moral child: Nurturing children's nat­ ural moral growth. New York: Free Press. Drawing on recent research, a developmental psychologist charts the course of moral development, stressing the value of authori­ tative rather than authoritarian parenting. Henig, R. M. (1981). The myth of senility. New York: An­ chor Press/Doubleday. Discusses memory and intelligence changes with age and de­ scribes the causes of "senility," including Alzheimer's disease. Silverstone B . , & Hyman, H. K. (1981). You and your aging parent: The modern family's guide to emotional, physical, and financial problems (2nd ed.). New York: Pantheon. A useful book that provides information and practical advice on how, without feeling trapped, to care responsibly for one's aging parents. 115 IMAPTER 5 Gender Of all the aspects of our development, none is more central to our identity, or to how others regard and treat us, than our sex. When you were born, the first thing most people asked about you was, "Boy or girl?" Children's biological sex dictates their gender, their social cate­ gory of male or female. By age 3, you had acquired a strong gender identity—a sense of being male or female—and your play reflected this. From preschool at least through junior high school, most of your playmates were probably children of your sex. As you played with friends and interacted with family, you learned a gender role, a set of expectations that prescribed how you, as a female or a male, should act. Today, the first thing a stranger notices about you is whether you are female or male. Psychologists study gender development because our maleness or femaleness is indeed so fundamental to our personal identity and so­ cial relations. It is a curious fact of life that whether you were born a girl or a boy helps to predict your future social power and how you will ultimately spend much of your time. In the United States today, women devote more than twice as many hours to housework than do men, more than three times as many hours to child care (Fuchs, 1986). But for an hour's employment they earn only 68 cents for every $1 earned by the average man (Bureau of the Census, 1987). When sur­ veyed, 82 percent of married women report doing most or all of the housework (and nearly all their husbands concur) (Public Opinion, 1986b). Women constitute 51 percent of the population, but only 1.7 percent of the officers of major corporations (Von Glinow, 1986) and 2 percent of the 1988 U.S. Senate. Except in prisons, men are seldom victimized by rape, nor are they the ones injured by domestic violence, incest, or sexual harassment. Such disregard for women is not true only in the United States. Although female infants are no longer left out on a hillside to die of exposure, as was the practice in ancient Greece, even today we find evidence from around the world that male offspring are held in higher regard than their sisters. For example, during the 1976-1977 famine in Bangladesh, preschool-age girls were more malnourished than boys, and in many developing countries death rates are higher for girls than for boys (Bairagi, 1987). Can you anticipate, then, the replies of 2000 Colorado schoolchil­ dren when asked how their lives would be different if they woke up tomorrow and discovered they were the other sex? Virtually no boys envied girls (Tavris & Baumgartner, 1983). " I f I woke up and I was a girl, I would hope it was a bad dream and go back to sleep," said one boy. Girls were more likely to think the switch a good deal: " I f I were a boy, my whole life would be easier." There was no doubt in these children's minds that it is "better" to be a boy than a girl. Why? What is " A s the man beholds the w o m a n , As the woman sees the man, Curiously they note each other, As each other only c a n . " Bryan Waller Procter, 1 7 8 7 - 1 8 7 4 The Sexes 118 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n On some occasions, y o u r identity is your gender. society telling them about gender differences? What kinds of gender differences are there? Are our gender-related behavioral differences ordained by biology? By culture? By an intricate interplay between biol­ ogy and culture? B I O L O G I C A L I N F L U E N C E S ON G E N D E R : IS BIOLOGY DESTINY? Obviously, men and women differ physically. In the United States, the average man is 40 percent muscle, 15 percent fat, and 5 feet 9 inches tall; the average woman is 23 percent muscle, 25 percent fat, and 5 feet 4 inches tall. Do men and women vary in physical ways that also influ­ ence their gender identities and roles? The "biosocial" view of gender says yes—perhaps indirectly, through the social consequences of physical differences, but yes. HORMONES Males and females are variations on a single form. Eight weeks after conception, they are anatomically indistinguishable. (Thus both sexes have nipples, although only females will ever develop a use for them.) Then, our genes activate our biological sex: XY sex chromosomes direct development of a male; in the absence of a Y chromosome, a female develops. After a male embryo's testes form internally they begin to secrete testosterone, the principal male sex hormone. Testosterone trig­ gers the development of external male sex organs; lacking testosterone, the embryo continues its course toward the development of female sex Organs. What, then, do you suppose happens when, due to a glandular malfunction or injections received by the mother, a female embryo is exposed to excess testosterone? Genetically female infants are born with masculine-appearing genitals, which can be corrected surgically. Until puberty, such females typically act in more aggressive "tomboyish" ways than most girls, and dress and play in ways more typical of boys than girls (Ehrhardt, 1987; Money, 1987). Note to computer g e e k s : T h e sex varia ^' e n a s a default value of female. CHAPTER 5 Gender Is their behavior due to the prenatal hormones? Perhaps. (Experi­ ments with many species, from rats to monkeys, confirm that female embryos given male hormones later exhibit more masculine appear­ ance and behavior [Hines, 1982].) But the girls frequently look mascu­ line and are known to be "different," so perhaps people also treat them more like boys. Genes and hormones can affect gender identity indi­ rectly, by predisposing the expectations and life experiences that shape us. Biological appearances have social consequences. SOCIOBIOLOGY: DOING WHAT COMES NATURALLY? A century ago, the English novelist Samuel Butler remarked that "A hen is only an egg's way of making another egg." In the same vein, the relatively new and controversial field of sociobiology—the study of the evolution of social behavior—views organisms as but their genes' way of making more genes. We are elaborate survival machines for our immortal genes: When we die, our genes live on in our biological rela­ tives. Sociobiologists study how evolution may predispose the behavior of social animals, whether ants, dogs, or humans. The underlying ra­ tionale is simple: If a behavior tendency is genetically influenced and if it helps an organism to survive and reproduce, then the relevant genes will be favored in the competition for gene survival. Through natural selection—the process by which evolution favors organisms best equipped to survive and reproduce—such genes become more common. As an example of a sociobiological explanation, consider: Why are men quicker than women to perceive sexual intent in friendly behavior (Abbey, 1987; Shotland & Craig, 1988)? And why do males of most mammalian species, including our own, tend to initiate sexual rela­ tions more frequently and with more partners than do females (Hinde, 1984; Kenrick & Trost, 1987)? To maximize the survival and spread of their genes, each male and female must maximize the number of their offspring that survive to reproduce. Because of their limited number of eggs and the reproductive time it takes to carry and nurse their young, female mammals have far fewer potential offspring than do males. Sociobiologists suggest that to ensure survival of the maximum num­ ber, females tend to be cautiously selective in their choice of a mate. They look for evidence of health and vitality and, in some species, for a commitment in time and resources to help in raising their young. On the other hand, sperm are abundant, giving males a much larger number of potential offspring. (If you are male, you will have produced about 2000 new sperm during the time it takes to read this sentence.) Because their success at reproducing depends partly on the number of females they fertilize, males that most successfully seek out and compete for females should leave more offspring. And if, as socio­ biologists suppose, social behaviors in humans are genetically predis­ posed, then the continued reproductive success of these individuals should, over time, favor the increase of sexually assertive males. What is controversial here? Not the idea that inherited behaviors which help organisms survive are selected for over the generations. Most scientists agree with that. But many object when this idea is ap­ plied to complex social behaviors, such as marital fidelity. Critics con­ tend that two problems exist with sociobiological explanations. First, they question whether genetic evolution really explains very much of human social behavior. Thanks to our common biology, we do share some universal behaviors: A smiling face can be read across cul- N o t e : S o c i o b i o l o g y is not just the biology of social behavior, but the evolutionary biology of social behavior. Sociobiologists attempt to explain the evolutionary d e v e l o p m e n t of social b e h a v ­ iors, s u c h as g r o o m i n g . A m o n g primates such as b a b o o n s , g r o o m i n g is the most t i m e - c o n s u m i n g m o d e of social interac­ tion. It averts a g g r e s s i o n , reveals social d o m i n a n c e , a n d indicates sexual partner­ ship. T h e development of g r o o m i n g be­ haviors to replace fighting benefits both the individual a n d the g r o u p . Secretariat's reward for being the greatest racehorse of modern times w a s the o p ­ portunity to sire more than 4 0 0 foals. 119 120 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n A universal behavior. Y o u k n o w the m e a n i n g of these facial expressions, and tures. But there is also great diversity in such behaviors as fathers' involvement in infant care. Marriage patterns vary, too—from monog­ amy (one spouse) to serial monogamy (a succession of spouses) to polygamy (multiple wives) to polyandry (multiple husbands) to spouse-swapping. Our shared biological heritage does not predict such diversity, nor does it explain rapid cultural changes in behavior. Second, after-the-fact explanations may sound convincing, but as we saw in Chapter 1, you can hardly lose at this game. Given a sexual double standard, we can, in hindsight, imagine how natural selection might explain it. Now let's explain the opposite—men who mate with but one woman. Could we not just as easily claim that natural selection favors the genes of males who are loyal to their mates? Of course— such behavior helps to protect and support the young, thereby perpet­ uating the parents' genes. (Indeed, sociobiologists support this view, saying it helps explain why humans tend to pair off.) So we are left with two plausible explanations of men's sexual behavior: that men are genetically programmed to be promiscuous, and that men are geneti­ cally programmed to be faithful. The moral? Unless a theory makes testable predictions, such after-the-fact explanations should be viewed with a healthy skepticism. There is yet a third objection to the application of sociobiology to humans. Some critics fear that if we explain human behavior in terms of our genes, some people may assume, wrongly, that certain gender roles, sexual tendencies, or racial hostilities are natural—adaptive, genetic, inevitable, and unchangeable. For example, someone may ex­ cuse a sexual double standard that tolerates male promiscuity with "Men will be men—it's in their genes!" No one disputes that men and women are the products of our mammalian, primate, and human history. But neither does anyone dispute that nature has endowed us with an enormous capacity to learn and to adapt. each of these people w o u l d understand the same expression on your face. " M y o w n guess is that the genetic bias is intense e n o u g h to create a substantial division of labor even in the most free a n d most egalitarian of future societies. . . . Even with identical education a n d equal access to all professions, men are likely to continue to play a disproportion­ ate role in political life, business a n d science." Sociobiologist E . O . Wilson ( 1 9 7 5 ) CHAPTER 5 Gender THE S O C I A L CONSTRUCTION OF GENDER What biology initiates, culture accentuates. Social expectations mold our experiences as males or females. And when social expectations vary, so do gender role behaviors. In earlier decades, when ambiguous genitals made an infant's genetic sex uncertain, the physician and par­ ents had to choose the baby's gender. (Note that even when one's biological sex is ambiguous, human beings cannot tolerate ambiguity about gender—one must be considered either female or male.) What do you suppose the child's gender identity usually became—would the child's self-image as boy or girl agree with the socially assigned gender, even if tests later revealed it to be wrong? Or would the child be more likely to have a confused gender identity? Most of these children comfortably accepted whatever gender they were assigned, whether genetically correct or not (Ehrhardt & Money, 1967; Money & others, 1957). Like other children, by age 3 they knew themselves as boys or girls, and this identity was thereafter difficult to reverse. Our gender identity, it seems, is socially constructed. THEORIES O F G E N D E R - T Y P I N G During childhood we gain not only our gender identity but also many masculine and feminine behaviors and attitudes. Although nearly everyone has the gender identity associated with their sex, some chil­ dren become more strongly gender-typed than others. That is, some boys exhibit more traditionally masculine traits and interests than other boys, and some girls become more distinctly feminine than other girls. By what process do girls become feminine and boys masculine? Four theories of gender-typing attempt to explain how gender is so­ cially constructed from the biological base of sex. (Table 5 - 1 on page 123 summarizes these explanations.) Identification Theory The best known theory is also the oldest: Sigmund Freud's (1933) theory of identification. Freud proposed that 3- to 5-year-olds develop a sexual attraction to the parent of the other sex. By age 5 or 6, he said, such feelings make them anxious. So they re­ nounce the feelings by identifying with the same-sex parent, uncon­ sciously adopting his or her characteristics. Although historically influential and still well known outside psy­ chology, Freud's theory is now disputed by most researchers and many clinical psychologists. Children become gender-typed well be­ fore age 5 or 6 and may become strongly feminine or masculine even in the absence of a same-sex parent (Frieze & others, 1978). Moreover, children tend to imitate familiar people who are powerful yet warm, which generally includes both parents (Jackson & others, 1986). Social Learning Theory In contrast to Freud's assumption that gender-typing comes from within the child as the child identifies with the same-sex parent, social learning theory assumes that children are molded (socialized) by their social environment. Children learn behav­ iors deemed appropriate for their sex by observation and imitation, and by being rewarded and punished. Parents use rewards and pun­ ishments to teach their daughters to be feminine ("Susie, you're such a good mommy to your dolls") and their sons to be masculine ("Big boys don't cry, Dick"). Children are rewarded for imitating people of their Gender T h e social definition of male and female. Gender identity O n e ' s sense of being male or female. Gender-typing T h e acquisition of a m a s ­ culine or feminine gender identity a n d role. Gender roles Expected behaviors for males a n d females. For more information on Freud's theory of personality, see C h a p t e r 1 5 . Is this child identifying with the s a m e - s e x parent? Is he being socialized into tradi­ tional g e n d e r - t y p e d behaviors? 121 122 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n own sex. When Dick dresses up in Daddy's clothes, his parents are more amused than when he dons Mommy's dress and shoes. More­ over, by observing others in the home, the neighborhood, and on tele­ vision, children learn the consequences of various behaviors without having to experience them. Children learn g e n d e r - t y p e d behaviors by observing a n d imitating, a n d being rewarded or punished. Critics complain that children are not so passive as social learning theory assumes. Whether their social worlds encourage or discourage traditional gender-typing, children inevitably seem to know that "boys are boys and girls are girls." They organize their worlds accordingly and create and enforce their own somewhat rigid rules for socially acceptable behavior. Cognitive Developmental Theory To account for the child's active participation in the gender-typing process, Lawrence Kohlberg (1966) proposed a theory of gender-typing that applied Piaget's principles of cognitive development. As children struggle to comprehend them­ selves and their worlds, one of the first concepts, or schemas, they form is that of their own gender. Having identified themselves as male or female, they soon begin to organize their worlds on the basis of gender. As their cognitive machinery matures, so does their understanding of what defines the genders—their schema accommodates to the more sophisticated understanding. To a preschool girl, short hair may define a man and long hair a woman, in which case the girl may insist on having long hair. By the concrete operational stage, when a child knows that the amount of milk remains constant after being poured from a tall, narrow glass into a short, wide glass, the child also understands gender constancy—that a woman remains a woman whether she wears her hair long or short (Tavris & Wade, 1984). Once children's gender is firmly established, they then use members of their own sex as models for their behavior. Critics of this cognitive developmental theory generally agree that what children think is important. But why among all the possible ways of categorizing people do children so consistently do so in terms of gender? Why not eye color? Or religion? Or, in another culture, caste? Is it, as Kohlberg believed, because biological sex differences such as Recall from C h a p t e r 3 that a schema is a concept or framework that organizes and interprets information. CHAPTER 5 Gender size and strength are obvious? Then, asks Sandra Bern (1987), why even in multiracial societies are children more conscious of gender than they are of race? Gender Schema Theory To answer these questions, Bern proposed gender schema theory, which combines aspects of the cognitive devel­ opmental and social learning theories. In agreement with the cognitive developmental view, gender schema theory assumes that children's actions are influenced by their concepts—schemas—of gender, which serve as a lens through which they view themselves and the world. Many languages, including the European languages, use gender sche­ mas to classify objects as either masculine (in French, "le train") or feminine ("la table"). In English, pronouns classify people by gender. To speak, even a 3-year-old must learn the difference between " s h e " and "he," "hers" and "his." But why and how do we acquire these gender schemas? Here Bern agrees with social learning theory: Our conceptions of gender do not come from observing biological sex differences, which actually aren't apparent during the preschool years when children are becoming strongly gender-typed. We instead organize our worlds into female and male categories because of all the diverse and subtle ways in which our culture communicates that sex is a most important category of human life. Preschoolers are constantly being dressed as boys and girls, given boys' or girls' toys to play with, and taught songs in which, for example, the fingers are women (sung with a high pitch) and the thumbs are men (sung with low pitch). Several times a day the culture reminds the child of the distinction between male and female. Small wonder, then, that, given dolls varying in genitalia, physique, and hair length, 4- and 5-year-olds assign sex based on the culturally defined gender difference: hair length (Thompson & Bentler, 1971). To summarize, gender schema theory suggests that gender-typing occurs as children learn from their culture what it means to be male or female: "I am male—thus masculine, strong, aggressive," or "I am female—therefore feminine, sweet, and helpful" (or whatever are the socially learned associations with one's sex). Comparing themselves to their gender schema, they adjust their behavior accordingly. FOUR T H E O R I E S OF G E N D E R - T Y P I N G Freud's identification theory Sexual anxiety > Identifying with the same-sex parent > Gender-typed behavior similar to the parent's Social learning theory Rewards and punishments + observation and imitation of models > Gender-typed behavior Cognitive developmental theory Child's struggle to comprehend self and world models > Gender-typed behavior > Concept of gender > Imitation of same-sex Gender schema theory Cultural emphasis on gender s. Gender schema (looking at self and world through a gender "lens") > Gender-organized thinking + gender-typed behavior 123 124 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n GENDER ROLES Most theories of gender-typing assume that we somehow learn to play the social role of female or male. Like a theatrical role, a social role is a set of prescriptions concerning how those who occupy the role should talk, dress, and act. Knowing someone's role—as professor, student, parent, or child—tells us, in general terms, how others believe that person should behave, which usually strongly influences how that per­ son does behave. Gender roles similarly prescribe behavior, tradition­ ally indicating, for example, that men were expected to initiate the date, drive the car, and pick up the check. Women were expected to cook the meals, buy the children's clothes, and do the laundry. Such formulas serve to grease the social machinery. They free us from self-conscious preoccupation with awkward little decisions, such as who reaches for the check. When we already know how to act, our attention is freed for other matters. But these benefits come at a price: Roles also constrain us. When we deviate from them—as when a woman repeatedly initiates formal dates with a man—we may feel anxious or be regarded as weird (Green & Sandos, 1983). Several experiments show that contemporary men and women do adjust their behavior to fulfill others' gender-role expectations (Deaux & Major, 1987). In one, Mark Zanna and Susan Pack (1975) had college women answer a questionnaire in which they described them­ selves to a tall, unattached, male senior they expected to meet. Those led to believe that the man had traditional gender-role expectations described themselves as more traditionally feminine than did those who expected to meet a man who liked nontraditional women. More­ over, when given an aptitude test, those who expected to meet the traditional man solved, on average, 15 percent fewer problems. Their more modest performance illustrates how our gender-role expectations can be self-fulfilling. Expectations help create gender differences, espe­ cially when people present one self to their own sex and a different self to the other sex. For example, college women are more likely than college men to report having feigned intellectual inferiority on a date (Braito & others, 1981). A gender difference observed in research might therefore be due to men and women exhibiting the behavior they be­ lieve is desired of them. Variations in Gender Roles Gender roles vary across cultures and over time. Biological sex differences between males and females do not. Clearly, social rather than biological factors create such variations. Across Cultures In almost all primitive societies, men predominate in fighting wars and hunting large game, women in gathering food and caring for infants. Sociobiologists have argued that evolution has pre­ disposed this age-old division of labor. In today's industrial societies, where men do not hunt and women do not gather, such gender dis­ tinctions may no longer be adaptive. Yet, say the sociobiologists, they persist as relics of our evolutionary past. Sociobiologists notwithstanding, gender roles vary widely from society to society. In agricultural societies, women stay close to home, working in the fields and tending children, while men roam more freely; nomadic societies have less distinct gender roles (Van Leeuwen, 1978). Among industrialized societies, the roles assigned to men and women vary enormously from country to country. In North America, medicine and dentistry are predominantly male occupations; in Russia, most doctors are women, as are most dentists in Denmark. "Civilization advances by extending the n u m b e r of operations which we c a n per­ form without thinking a b o u t t h e m . " Alfred North W h i t e h e a d , 1 8 6 1 - 1 9 4 7 "What the happened rink, to Sis? Best and suddenly she skater on forgot how!" CHAPTER Over Time Marcia Guttentag and Paul Secord (1983) analyzed socie­ ties from classical Athens to modern America and found that gender roles vary over time as well as across cultures. In times when marriage­ able women are in short supply, women are protected and marital fidelity is strong. When migration or war creates a relative excess of younger women, men become more sexually promiscuous and women become more self-reliant—more likely to work outside the home and to organize social movements to improve their status. Consider the changing roles of North American women. Gender roles that may have been adaptive in past eras when women were pregnant or nursing for much of their adult lives may be less adaptive now that most women live longer, bear fewer children, and are em­ ployed outside the home. In some ways, gender roles have changed little. In the 1980s women are still, as in earlier decades, expected to be warm, expressive, and nurturant, and men to be independent, self-reliant, and assertive. Occupationally, nurses, secretaries, and kindergarten teachers are still overwhelmingly women. In other ways, gender role expectations have shifted dramatically over the past half century. The change is evident first in people's ex­ pressed attitudes (Public Opinion, 1978, 1980; Wilkins & Miller, 1985). In 1937,1 in 3 Americans said they would be willing to vote for a qualified woman whom their party nominated for President; in 1984, 4 in 5 said they would. In 1938, only 1 in 5 approved " o f a married woman earn­ ing money in business or industry if she has a husband capable of supporting her"; by 1978, 3 out of 4 approved. In 1970, Americans were evenly split in favoring or opposing "efforts to strengthen wom­ en's status." By 1985, such efforts were approved by a 4 to 1 margin. The changing role of women is also evident in their changing be­ havior. Most obvious is the increase in the rate of women's employ­ ment (see Figure 4 - 9 on page 109): In 1960, a third of wives with chil­ dren were employed; by the mid-1980s, over 50 percent of married women with preschoolers and 60 percent of those with school-aged children worked outside the home (Bianchi & Spain, 1986). Women's occupations are also changing. When the class of 1989 began college in the fall of 1985, 4 in 10 women were intending to pursue careers in law, business, medicine, or engineering—double the percentage in 1970 (Astin & others, 1987). Since 1972, the percent of doctoral degrees received by American women has also doubled (How­ ard & others, 1986). 5 Gender 125 First-year students who agree that the role of married women is best confined to home and family C o l l e g e student endorsement of the traditional v i e w of w o m e n ' s role has declined dramati­ cally. (From Astin & others, 1987.) T h e flow of married w o m e n into the workforce means that more a n d more w o m e n , like this attorney, o c c u p y multiple roles. A recent study ( C l e m i n s h a w , 1 9 8 8 ) suggests that the more roles a w o m a n plays, the more satisfied she is with her life. 126 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Joseph Pleck (1987) reports that a slower, more "subtle revolution" has also been occurring in men's roles. Compared with the mid-1960s, men are now devoting more time to family work. Increasingly, men are found in front of the stove, behind the vacuum cleaner, and over the diaper changing table. Still, even in countries that have sought to equalize the roles of women and men, gender distinctions persist. Whether in Russia, China, or Sweden, answers to "Who works in the child care nurseries?," "Who cooks dinner?," and "Who runs the country?" remain nearly as predictable as in the United States and Canada. Should There Be Gender Roles? Social scientists' personal con­ cerns motivate their interest in topics such as gender roles, and many share Sandra Bern's (1985) conviction that "human behaviors and per­ sonality attributes should no longer be linked with gender." Unlike some parents who are timid about "imposing" their values and beliefs on their children, Bern feels that parents who have deep social, politi­ cal, or religious convictions should not be bashful about transmitting their convictions to their children. Children are certainly going to absorb an ideology from somewhere—from the culture if not from the home. Bern suggests how those who share her values might raise children who are less gender-typed. At home, make gender irrelevant to who cooks and does dishes and to what toys are available; when children are young, censor gender-stereotyped books and television programs, and teach them that one's sex entails anatomical and reproductive traits and not much else. In order to help them process what their culture tries to tell them about gender, teach them how individuals differ, how gender roles vary, and how to detect sex discrimination. As they grow older, give boys and girls the same privileges and responsi­ bilities. Some psychologists doubt that gender roles will ever disappear. Douglas Kenrick (1987), for one, believes that try as we might to recon­ struct our gender concepts, "we cannot change the evolutionary his­ tory of our species, and some of the differences between us are un­ doubtedly a function of that history." Bern (1987) agrees there may be "biologically based sex differences in behavior." But she believes that the social construction of gender greatly exaggerates any biological basis. Thus if under egalitarian social conditions, CHAPTER 5 Gender it turns out that more men than women become engineers or that more women than men decide to stay at home with their children, I'll live hap­ pily with those sex differences as well as with any others that emerge. But I am willing to bet that the sex differences that emerge under those condi­ tions will not be nearly as large or as diverse as the ones that currently exist in our society. T h o s e w h o advocate raising their children without traditional gender schemas b e ­ Alternative Gender Roles In Bern's ideal world, gender roles would greatly diminish. In the real world they persist. Is this a bad thing? Are people who are less gender-typed mentally healthier and happier? Androgyny is the possession of both masculine and feminine qualities. In research studies, androgynous people are those who describe them­ selves with both traditionally masculine adjectives (independent, as­ sertive, competitive) and traditionally feminine adjectives (warm, tender, compassionate). Do such people in fact feel better about them­ selves than do strongly gender-typed "masculine" men and "femi­ nine" women? Initial reports suggested they do, but analyses of dozens of such studies by Marylee Taylor and Judith Hall (1982) and Bernard Whitley (1985) reveal that androgynous people do not consistently exhibit higher self-esteem or better adjustment. Rather, in both men and women, a positive self-concept is linked more strongly to traditional masculine traits. Describing oneself as independent, assertive, and self-confident contributes to high scores on both masculinity and selfesteem scales, and that hints at something deeper: Perhaps the entire culture measures individuals against a male yardstick, thereby valuing masculine traits over feminine traits. When it comes to relationships, traditional feminine qualities may prove an asset (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986; Orlofsky & O'Heron, 1987). In a study of 108 Australian married couples, John Antill (1983) found that when either the wife or husband possessed feminine traits such as gentleness, sensitivity, or warmth—or better yet, when both did— marital satisfaction was higher. Although traditional masculine traits may boost self-esteem, says Antill, "they are apparently not the quali­ ties that hold the key to a happy, long-term relationship." lieve g e n d e r should be irrelevant to w h o plays with w h i c h toys a n d w h o does which household tasks. 127 128 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n Should gender roles be preserved? Some say that men's and wom­ en's biological differences make their social differences—to some ex­ tent at least—not only inevitable but desirable. Yes, they say, the sexes share traits and abilities in common, but each sex also bears special gifts. To distinguish between two wines, composers, or sexes is to ap­ preciate each equally, by discerning their separate virtues. Equality and freedom of individual choice, yes; sameness, no. Others say that biological differences are socially trivial. Human beings—both women and men—should be unshackled from all that constrains their being fully human: assertive and nurturant, selfconfident and tender, independent and compassionate. Should gender roles be preserved? Science informs the debate, but personal convic­ tions decide it. "I w a s a better man as a w o m a n with a woman than I've ever been as a m a n with a w o m a n . " A n o u t - o f - w o r k actor (Dustin H o f f m a n ) , NATURE-NURTURE INTERACTION Our discussion has proceeded as if the biological and social influences on gender were two opponents in a fencing match. Biology scores points for observed genetic and hormonal sex differences; social influ­ ence responds with points scored for the creation of gender and all its cultural variations. But surely we create a false dichotomy by trying to partition as­ pects of gender neatly into nature and nurture, for biological and social factors interact. Biological factors that create male and female physical characteristics may predispose different cultural influences on males and females (Harris, 1978). If hormones predispose males to be slightly more aggressive, society may then amplify this difference by encourag­ ing males to be tough and females to be gentle. Such a gender effect would be both a biological and a social effect—culture developing what biology has initiated. We can conclude by restating the principle in general terms: Bio­ logical factors always operate within a social context, and social effects operate on what is biologically given. In the weaving of the human fabric, the biological and social threads act as warp and woof. GENDER DIFFERENCES Psychology studies many topics that are central to our lives as women and men. The most extensively researched and debated topic of all is how boys and girls, men and women, differ (Walsh, 1987). One book on gender differences synthesized more than a thousand research studies involving a half million participants (Hyde & Linn, 1986). Since 1968, Psychological Abstracts, the "reader's guide" to psychological re­ search, has indexed more than 20,000 articles on "human sex differ­ ences." So, how different are boys from girls? Young men from young women? Older men from older women? The phrasing of these ques­ tions draws our attention to the ways in which the sexes differ, not to their similarities. In many ways—the age of first sitting up, teething, walking, and in generosity, helpfulness, and overall intelligence, to name a few-—males and females are not noticeably different (Maccoby, 1980). But because our attention is invariably drawn to how we differ from others, such similarities are seldom mentioned. Similarities tend not to require explanation; differences do. In science, as in everyday life, differences excite our interest. w h o masquerades as a w o m a n to land a j o b , finds his transformation has some surprising consequences for his relationship with a real w o m a n (Jessica L a n g e ) in the movie Tootsie. CHAPTER 5 Gender THE P O L I T I C S O F S T U D Y I N G G E N D E R D I F F E R E N C E S Some critics of psychology are concerned about the study of gender differences. Might the reporting of such studies exaggerate people's perceptions of the differences between women and men? Oblivious to the social construction of gender, might such perceptions in turn lead people to suppose that gender differences are genuine biological sex differences—innate, immutable, and even desirable? Believing the answers to be yes, sociologist Jessie Bernard (1976, p. 13) argued that scientific demonstrations of gender differences therefore serve as "bat­ tle weapons against women." Historically, however, research on gender differences has under­ mined popular myths about women's inferiority—myths that had the political consequences of limiting women's social power by excluding them from educational and employment opportunities. By reducing overblown gender stereotypes, contends researcher Alice Eagly (1986), recent "gender-difference research has probably furthered the cause of gender equality." The concern nevertheless remains, because when one group has more status and power than another, differences between them are usually viewed as the less powerful group's deficiencies. Recall from Chapter 4 the caution that women's moral differences are not moral deficits. Similarly, Jacquelynne Eccles (1987a) notes that women's dif­ fering educational and vocational choices reflect different but equally important goals and values. Women who choose to become low-paid preschool teachers rather than high-paid business managers surely do reflect our society's definition of gender-appropriate behavior; but they may also reflect women's ethic of care. The differing pay levels for traditionally male and female occupations says more about the values of those who assign wages to work than about the inherent value of women's and men's choices. " T h e r e should be no qualms a b o u t the forthright study of racial a n d g e n d e r dif­ ferences; science is in desperate need of g o o d studies that . . . inform us of w h a t we need to do to help underrepresented people to succeed in this society. Unlike the ostrich, we cannot afford to hide our heads for fear of socially uncomfortable discoveries." Developmental psychologist S a n d r a Scarr ( 1 9 8 8 ) HOW D O M A L E S A N D F E M A L E S D I F F E R ? Just knowing that you are male or female triggers certain perceptions of you in people's minds. Perceived gender differences exceed actual gender differences. This is most obvious in infancy, when boy-girl dif­ ferences in appearance and behavior are negligible. Without the obvi­ ous clues of pink or blue, people will struggle over whether to call the new baby a " h e " or a " s h e . " Nevertheless, fathers have been found to rate their day-old daughters as softer, smaller, and more beautiful, and their sons as firmer, stronger, and better coordinated (Rubin & others, 1974). And when John and Sandra Condry (1976) showed people a videotape of a 9-month-old infant reacting strongly to a jack-in-thebox, those told the child was "David" perceived "his" emotion as mostly anger; those told the child was "Dana" perceived "her" identi­ cal reaction as mostly fear. Some "gender differences" exist merely in the eyes of their beholders. Among the gender differences that seem actually to exist between males and females are those involving physical aggression, social dom­ inance, empathy, and spatial ability. Here, as elsewhere in this book, we do well to remember that average differences between groups, such as between males and females, may tell us little about individuals. For the psychological characteristics we are about to consider, the varia­ tions among women and among men far exceed those between the average woman and the average man. And that is why judgments about the suitability of people for particular tasks are best made on an Is this 7 - m o n t h - o l d a b o y or a girl? (See p a g e 1 3 3 . ) D u r i n g infancy, boy-girl differ­ ences are minimal, but o n c e we k n o w the child's sex we interpret a n d guide behavior accordingly. 129 130 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n individual basis, without prejudgments based on sex. Could you be­ come a competent engineer? Or child care worker? Knowing that you are a man or a woman tells us little; knowing you as an individual tells us much more. Aggression By aggression, psychologists refer not to assertive, ambi­ tious behavior ("Claire is an aggressive saleswoman") but rather to physical or verbal behavior that is intended to hurt someone. In sur­ veys, men admit to considerably more hostility and aggression than do women. To some extent, such admissions may simply reflect people's acceptance of the common perception that males are more aggressive. But in laboratory experiments that assess physical aggression, men in­ deed behave more aggressively, for example by administering what they think are higher levels of hurtful electric shock (Eagly, 1987; Hyde, 1986a). In everyday American life, men are arrested for violent crimes eight times more often than women (Federal Bureau of Investi­ gation, 1987). Indeed, in every society that has kept crime records, males have committed more physical violence (Kenrick, 1987). Throughout the world, hunting, fighting, and warring are primarily men's activities. The male sex hormone testosterone seems partly responsible. In various animal species, one can increase aggressiveness by administer­ ing testosterone. In humans, violent male criminals average higher than normal testosterone levels (Rubin & others, 1980). Moreover, the aggression difference appears early in life and across many species of mammals. In humans, this may be linked with males' greater physical activity (Eaton & Enns, 1986). (Ninety percent of children diagnosed as rambunctiously hyperactive are boys [McGuinness, 1985].) No one of these findings would be conclusive by itself, but the convergence of evidence suggests that male aggressiveness has biological roots. With­ out doubt, it also has social roots; as we noted earlier, social learning encourages aggressiveness in boys more than in girls. In physical as well as psychological traits, individual differences a m o n g men a n d a m o n g w o m e n far exceed the small aver­ a g e differences between the sexes. H a d he been c o m p e t i n g against the w o m e n ' s 5 0 0 - m e t e r speed skaters in the 1 9 8 8 O l y m p i c s , Erhard Keller, winner of the m e n ' s 5 0 0 - m e t e r race in the 1 9 6 8 O l y m ­ pics, w o u l d have finished in sixth place, 1 5 meters behind 1 9 8 8 g o l d medalist, Bonnie Blair. " T h e r e is little doubt that we w o u l d all be safer if the world's w e a p o n systems were Social Dominance Across the world, men are perceived as more dominant. From Finland to France, from Peru to Pakistan, from The Netherlands to New Zealand, people rate men as more dominant, ag­ gressive, and achievement-driven, women as more deferential, nurturant, and affiliative (Williams & Best, 1986). And indeed, in virtually every known society, men are socially dominant. When groups are formed, leadership tends to go to males. When people interact, men are more likely to utter opinions, women to express support (Aries, 1987; Wood, 1987). In everyday behavior, men are more likely to act as powerful people do—to talk assertively, to interrupt, to initiate touch­ ing, to smile less, to stare (J. Hall, 1987). Such behaviors help maintain the inequities of social power. When political leaders are elected, they are usually men. When salaries are paid, those in traditionally male occupations are judged more valuable. When asked what pay they deserve, women often expect less than do men with the same qualifications or performance (Major, 1987). controlled by a v e r a g e w o m e n instead of b y average m e n , " Melvin Konner, The Tangled Wing- on the Human Spirit, Constraints 1982 " W h e n rewards are distributed, the w o m a n gets one half the pay that a man does, and if disgrace is given out she bears it a l l . " Elbert H u b b a r d , Empathy To have empathy is to understand and feel what another feels—to rejoice with those who rejoice, and weep with those who weep. If you are empathic, you identify with others. You are able to imagine what it feels like to live with a handicap, what it must be like to try so hard to impress people, what a thrill it must be to win that award. When surveyed, women are far more likely than men to de­ scribe themselves as empathic. Biological The Philistine, 1897 CHAPTER But are they? In the laboratory, females are more likely to cry and to report feeling distressed when observing another's distress, perhaps partly because they are also slightly better than males at reading other people's nonverbal emotional cues (Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983; Hall, 1987). Curiously though, measures such as heart rate, taken while someone observes emotional distress, indicate that women do not con­ sistently exhibit more physiological reaction than males. Similarly, when rating their reactions to pictures or videotapes of babies, women report much stronger reactions than men do, but measures of heart rate and perspiration fail to confirm that women physically respond more strongly (Berman, 1980). Perhaps women respond much as men do but are considered more empathic because they are more aware of their emotional reactions and more willing to report them. If so, that too may help explain why both men and women seem to turn to women for intimacy and understand­ ing (Rubin, 1985), and why women report feeling stronger emotions (Diener & others, 1985). 5 Gender W o m e n report more e m p a t h y . Do they actually feel more e m p a t h y ? Studies indi­ cate that w o m e n surpass men at sending a n d reading nonverbal emotional cues. Differences in aggression, dominance, and empathy peak in late adolescence and early adulthood—the very years most commonly studied. Several studies have found that during middle age, gender differences diminish as women become more assertive and self-confi­ dent and men more empathic and less domineering (Helson & Moane, 1987; Turner, 1982). For example, Florine Livson (1976) studied the evolving gender differences among forty females and forty males as young teenagers in Oakland, California, and periodically thereafter up to age 50. During the teen years, the girls became progressively less assertive and more flirtatious, while the boys became more domineer­ ing and unexpressive. But by age 50, these differences had diminished. The men were expressing warmer feelings for others, and many of the women had become more assertive. In another study, M. F. Lowenthal and his colleagues (1975) asked married couples, "Who's the boss in your family?" Both husbands and wives were, with age, progressively less likely to single out the husband. Even trivial behaviors such as the ; customary masculine and feminine ways of carrying books (Figure 5 - 1 ) become less predictable in middle age (Jenni & Jenni, 1976). H o w e v e r , society also encourages w o m e n to express empathy more freely. ! Figure 5 - 1 T h e customary masculine a n d feminine w a y s of carrying books. (From Jenni & Jenni, 1976.) 131 C o r a z o n A q u i n o , s h o w n here with her f a m i l y — i n her o w n words, " j u s t a house­ w i f e " — e m e r g e d from 28 years in the s h a d o w of her h u s b a n d , a slain Philippine political leader, to lead a peaceful revolu­ tion that o v e r w h e l m e d dictator Ferdinand Marcos. W h e n she returned to her college in N e w Y o r k at age 51 to receive an hon­ orary degree, former teachers and class­ mates were startled by her new selfconfidence a n d authority. " I t w a s n ' t the C o r y I r e m e m b e r e d , " said one. Why do gender differences first increase and then decrease as life progresses? Personality theorist Carl Jung (1933) speculated that both masculine and feminine tendencies exist in everyone, and that during the second half of life people develop their previously repressed femi­ nine or masculine aspects. Others have speculated that during court­ ship and early parenthood social expectations have traditionally led each sex to deemphasize traits that interfere with their roles at that time. So long as men are expected to provide and protect, they forgo their more dependent and tender sides (Gutmann, 1977), and so long as women are expected to nurture, they forgo their impulses to be assertive and independent. When they graduate from these early adult roles, men and women then become freer to develop and express their previously inhibited tendencies. Verbal and Spatial Abilities Until 1974, studies indicated that fe­ males slightly surpassed males on tests of verbal abilities, such as spell­ ing, vocabulary, and comprehending difficult material. Since then, report Janet Hyde and Marcia Linn (1988), studies involving hundreds of thousands of test-takers have found no meaningful gender differ­ ence. But among children there is an exception at the low-scoring ex­ treme: Boys more often are slow to develop language. In remedial reading classes, boys outnumber girls by three to one (Finucci & Childs, 1981). Speech defects such as stuttering are also predominantly boys' problems. On spatial tasks, such as the speed of mentally rotating objects in space, males tend to surpass females (Halpern, 1986; Linn & Peterson, 1986). (See Figure 5 - 2 . ) Spatial abilities are helpful when playing chess, mentally rotating suitcases to see how best to fit them into a car trunk, finding one's way around an unfamiliar town, or doing geometry prob­ lems. This may help explain the gender difference in mathematics achievement. On the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), males and fe­ males score similarly on the verbal test, but on the mathematics test males average about 50 points higher (on a 600-point scale). Males have a similar edge on the American College Testing (ACT) mathematics " I n the long years liker must they g r o w ; T h e m a n be more of w o m a n , she of m a n . ' Alfred, Lord T e n n y s o n , The Princess, Figure 5 - 2 1847 T h e a v e r a g e male's spatial abilities surpass those of the average fe­ male. (From H y d e , 1 9 8 1 . ) H o w e v e r , given the overlap of scores between the sexes, the small average differences do not c o m ­ pletely explain w h y 96 percent of A m e r i ­ can architects are m e n , nor w h y 96 per­ cent of rated chess players are men (Gilbert, 1 9 8 6 ) . CHAPTER 5 Gender test, an edge that is only partly reduced when comparing males with females who have had the same number of math courses (ACT, 1987). W h i c h two circles contain a configuration of blocks identical to the one in the circle at the left? Standard Responses A test of spatial abilities: the mental rota­ tion test. W h i c h two responses s h o w a different v i e w of the standard? (Answers are upside d o w n at the bottom of this page.) As always when the overlap between the sexes is considerable, small average differences can look bigger when sampling from the ex­ tremes of a distribution. Thus among mathematically precocious sev­ enth graders (those scoring above 700 on the SAT mathematics test in nationwide talent searches), boys have outnumbered girls by more than 10 to 1 (Benbow, 1988). These whiz kids, more than others their age, are male, left-handed, nearsighted, and suffer allergies or asthma (see also page 48)—characteristics that brain scientists Norman Geschwind and Peter Behan (1984) attributed to exposure to excess testosterone during prenatal development. But Jacqueline Eccles (1987b) has found that social expectations also shape and constrain boys' and girls' academic and career interests and abilities. Parents more willingly send their sons to computer camps; daughters receive more encouragement in English. Seventh graders scoring in the 700s on the SAT math test is ex­ treme and unusual. But it is a curious feature of statistical distributions that even a small average difference between two groups (such as in Figure 5-2) can create noticeable differences at the extremes. Only 5 percent of the variation in individuals' activity levels is attributable to gender (Eaton & Enns, 1986), but that's all it takes to make extreme activity—diagnosed as hyperactivity—much more common in boys than girls. Thus observing the extremes—say, the preponderance of males among the hyperactive, the criminally violent, and the seventh grade math whiz kids—can mislead us into exaggerated perceptions of differences between groups. What can we conclude? There appear to be detectable differences in the social behavior and cognitive abilities of males and females. But the gender similarities also are impressive, enough so that researcher Lauren Harris (1978) cautioned against thinking of females and males as opposite sexes: "Neither in any physiological nor in any psychologi­ cal sense are males and females 'contrary or antithetical in nature or tendency; diametrically opposed, or altogether different.'" We might instead think of our own sex and the other sex as like the two halves of an oyster shell—very similar but not identical, equally important, and fitting together because they grow together and change around each other. T h e infant in the photo on p a g e 1 2 9 is a girl. " T o d a y , in the 1980s, there is the g r o w ­ ing realization that it is possible for w o m e n to be equal a n d different." Psychologist Beatrice B. W h i t i n g ( 1 9 8 7 ) S9A!}BUJ3}|B p j j q i puB pUOD3S s i a 'pjBpuBjs wo:uoq a i u J O J i s a A ^ B U j a j - | B q u n o j p u B \si\i a q i 'pjEpuEjs d o ; a q j JOJ :}S9j uon,B}OJ IBJUSUJ aq; 0} S J S M S U V 133 134 PART 2 Development over the Life S p a n SUMMING UP Few aspects of our lives are more important to our exis­ tence than our being born male or female. Examining the development of our maleness and femaleness, and of the behaviors expected of males and females, enables us to see biological and social factors at work and to appreciate how intertwined these factors are. BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON G E N D E R : IS BIOLOGY DESTINY? Hormones The male hormone testosterone has mascu­ linizing effects on the developing fetus and can influence social behaviors, such as aggressiveness. Sociobiology Sociobiologists theorize that evolution fa­ vors social behaviors that enable men and women, in their own ways, to survive and reproduce. For males, whose sperm are cheap, this is said to mean being aggressive and taking sexual initiative. For women, whose investment in reproduction is far greater, this is said to mean being se­ lective and then nurturant to their children. Critics main­ tain that sociobiological explanation often starts with be­ havior and reasons backward to conjecture an explanation. Furthermore, they say, human behaviors are less constrained by genes and are therefore more varied (adaptable) than those of other species for which sociobio­ logical explanations were first developed. Finally, they point out, the effects of culture must not be ignored. THE SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION OF GENDER Theories of Gender-Typing Theorists have sought to explain how we acquire gender identity and roles. Freud believed that young children resolve sexual anxiety by identifying with their same-sex parent. Social learning theory assumes that children learn gender-linked behav­ iors by observing and imitating, and being rewarded or punished. Cognitive developmental theory proposes that as children struggle to understand themselves and their world, they form a concept (schema) of gender, which then influences their behavior. Sandra Bern's gender schema theory combines cognitive developmental and social learning views of gender-typing by showing how society influences the child's schema of gender. Gender Roles Expectations for men's and women's be­ havior vary widely across cultures, and across time within a culture. In North America, gender roles have been con­ verging, yet in no culture have they disappeared. Whether they can or should disappear is a debate that science can inform but not decide. Nature-Nurture Interaction Biological factors always operate within a social context, and social influences oper­ ate upon what is biologically given. GENDER D I F F E R E N C E S The Politics of Studying Gender Differences Differ­ ences—even small average differences between overlap­ ping groups of people—catch our attention. Some people worry that studying gender differences may therefore exaggerate people's perceptions of such differences; oth­ ers argue that such research has debunked gender myths and that men and women can be seen as different, yet be equally valued. How Do Males and Females Differ? Research studies have shown that males tend to behave more aggressively, to exert more social dominance, and to exhibit greater spatial and math ability. Women tend to show somewhat greater sensitivity to others' nonverbal messages, to re­ port feeling more empathy, and to have fewer language disabilities. In general, men and women differ more in their reports on their social behaviors than is observed in their actual behaviors. TERMS AND CONCEPTS TO REMEMBEI aggression someone. Physical or verbal behavior intended to hurt androgyny (andros, man + gyne, woman) Possession of desirable psychological traits traditionally associated with both men and women. cognitive developmental theory The theory that gendertyping occurs as children form a concept of gender, which then influences whom they imitate. empathy The ability to understand and feel what another feels, to put oneself in someone else's shoes. gender The social definition of male and female. gender identity One's sense of being male or female. Note: One's gender identity is distinct from one's sexual orientation (as heterosexual or homosexual) and from the strength of one's gender-typing (see page 135). gender role A set of expected behaviors for males and for females. gender schema theory The theory that children learn from their cultures a concept of what it means to be male and female and adjust their behavior accordingly. CHAPTER 5 Gender gender-typing The acquisition of a masculine or femi­ nine gender identity and role. such as gender-typed behavior, by observing and imitat­ ing, and by being rewarded and punished. identification Freud's term for the presumed process by which a child adopts the characteristics of the same-sex parent. More generally, the process by which people as­ sociate themselves with and copy the behavior of signifi­ cant others. sociobiology The study of the evolution of social behav­ ior using the principles of natural selection. Social behav­ iors that are genetically based and that contribute to the preservation and spread of one's genes are presumed to be favored by natural selection. natural selection The process by which evolution favors individuals within a species best equipped to survive and reproduce. testosterone The most important of the male sex hor­ mones. Both males and females have it, but the additional testosterone in males stimulates the growth of the male sex organs in the fetus and the development of the male sex characteristics during puberty. social learning theory The theory that we learn behavior, 135