Human Development A June 30, 2005 Lecture by Gordon Vessels, Ed.D. INTRODUCTION: Certainly we change from the moment of conception until our last breath, but do we continually develop? Even physical development cannot be viewed as an automatic, genetically determined process of growth unless the needed environment supports are there including adequate nutrition. I recall seeing the dramatic physical differences between persons who were sent out of Vietnam as young children and raised in the U.S. by relatives or adoptive parents, and their siblings who did not leave and who were raised without a comparable level of nutrition and health care in Vietnam. You would never have guessed they were siblings if you did not know since the American-raised siblings towered over their brothers and sisters and appeared younger. So let’s put to rest right away the notion that development is automatic. With respect to other forms of development, particularly personality and social-emotional, I have seen many cases where development apparently halted at a point for some reason and never resumed. Take as an example the “adult” male who is only interested in teenage girls or the married adult male who feels compelled to imprison his wife because he cannot allow her to show any interest in other men even on a casual level. The latter suggests a level of friendship development that is typical of young teens but not most older teens; the former suggests that the person may not have been successful in relating to the opposite sex as a teenager and/or that he fled from challenges that Robert Havighurst and Erik Erikson would have seen as necessary in order for him to continue developing normally. Recall from the lecture on perception that a person’s visual-perceptual potential is never realized if they experience significant sensory deprivation as an infant and toddler. So development occurs in a social-environment context, and this context and the individual’s experiences and successes within it, irrespective of who might be responsible, influence (a) whether or not, (b) to what degree, (c) and how smoothly development occurs. Various theorists account for this better than others such as Martin Hoffman (2000) with his inductive discipline encounters, Diana Baumrind’s (1967) types of parenting, Erik Erikson’s and Sigmund Freud’s explanations of personality problems, Vygotsky’s (1989) implications with respect to the effects of inadequate instructional scaffolding on knowledge and skill acquisition, Mary Ainsworth’s (1978) and Jonathan Bowlby’s (1982) ethological studies of mother-infant attachment or lack thereof, and the central proposition our ecological theorists including Urie Bronfenbrenner. It might even be reasonable to say that the best of our developmental researchers and theorists explain both the normal course of development under conditions of adequate support, and related causes of abnormal development. Both parents and teachers need to have a sense of both and need to know what to do to foster the former and prevent the latter. REVIEW OF PHILOSOPHICAL ASSUMPTIONS: A. Keep in mind as you read through this information that there are philosophical assumptions that underlie development theories, and they vary from theory to theory.1 Below, I’m listing dichotomies that will help us to pinpoint the philosophical assumptions that underlie each scientific theory of development. As you read through these, ask yourself if I took philosophical positions in my introduction to this lecture, and be on the alert as you read further as to the theorists that share my assumptions, or vise versa, and ask yourself if I should have allowed myself to conclude they are on the right track. 1 I will be shifting back and forth between types of theories, which can be categorized as cognitive, biological, behavioral, etc., and domains of development, which will be identified in the next section. 1 1. Free will vs. Determinism or Active Agent vs. Passive Organism: Is the free will to make choices an illusion? Are we completely shaped by genetic and environmental events? Are we active agents who direct, shape, and control our own development and destiny? 2. Nature vs. Nurture or Stability vs. Placticity: To what extent are we a product of our genetic inheritance ("nature") or a product of our experiences ("nurture")? Are people essentially programmed by their heredity and evolutionary past or can they be effectively shaped by others through intentional acts? 3. Unconscious vs. Conscious Motivation: Is much or all of our behavior and determined by unconscious factors? Or is little or none so determined? How much of our behavior is determined by conscious forces? 4. Uniqueness vs. Universality: Are we each unique, or will psychology eventually discover laws that explain all our behavior and our seemingly unique combinations of personal traits? 5. Physiological vs. Purposive Motivation: Are we more "pushed" by physiological needs? Are we more "pulled" by our perceptions, knowledge, virtues, higher-level needs, and personal goals, values, and principles? 6. Cultural Determinism vs. Cultural Transcendence: Do our cultures shape and control? Can we rise above or transcend cultural influences? This repeats the free-will question with specific reference to environmental as culture. 7. Continuous vs. Discontinuous Growth — Can development be characterized as a gradual change process, or does it present sudden, distinct bursts of change and/or sudden delays or permanent arrests? Some theorists view development as a continuous process. In contrast, stage theories assume that development is discontinuous and involves periodic qualitative milestone changes. DOMAINS OF DEVELOPMENT: A. I have looked at the contents of many developmental psychology textbooks and have yet to find one that does what I feel is an adequate job of identifying all domains of development and treating them as equally important. For the last ten years I have stressed the importance of addressing all domains of development in our schools, as first called for by M.D. James Comer and his School Development Program. To do otherwise is to be foolish and to allow education to be other than child-centered, or more accurately, developmental-need-centered. There are indeed needs that correspond to levels within each of the nine domains listed below, and it is incumbent upon educators to be aware of (a) the domains, (b) the apparent levels for each child within each domain, and (c) what they can do to promote development by recognizing and addressing developmental needs associated with every level of every domain. This may simply be the right kind of stimulation, the right kind of supervision, the right amount of activity or freedom, or the most promising social grouping arrangement. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. Physical-Maturational Cognitive-Intellectual Artistic-Creative Linguistic-Communicative Knowledge-Skill Moral-Ethical Social-Interpersonal Personality-Individuality Emotional-Affective B. Major contributing researchers/theorists for each domain: 1. Physical-Maturational — Gesell 2 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. Cognitive-Intellectual — Piaget, Damon Artistic-Creative — Lowenfeld, Gardner, Torrance Linguistic — Chomsky Knowledge-Skill — Vygotsky, Damon Moral-Ethical — Piaget, Kohlberg, Kagan, Hoffman, Damon Social-Interpersonal — Youniss, Selman, Damon Personality-Individuality — Freud, Erikson, Dowlby, Ainsworth Emotional-Affective — Hoffman, Kagan, Erikson, Dowlby, Ainsworth C. Descriptions of these theorists’ models: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. Psychosexual Personality Development (Freud) Psychosocial Personality Development (Erikson) Developmental Tasks as Developmental Milestones (Havighurst) Cognitive Development (Piaget) Moral Reasoning Development (Kohlberg, Piaget, Havighurst) Moral Emotion Development (Hoffman, Kagan) Social-Conceptual Development (Damon, Selman, Youniss) Scaffolded Knowledge and Skill Development (Vygotsky, Damon) Ecological-Social Development (Bronfenbrenner) Maturational-Biological Milestones (Gesell) Ethological Personality-by-Attachment (Bowlby, Ainsworth) COGNITIVE-INTELLECTUAL AND KNOWLEDGE-SKILL DEVELOPMENT: A. Cognitive or intellectual development can be distinguished from knowledge-skill development. Cognitive has to do with thinking and forms of information processing and perception that are involved in different types of thinking and reasoning. Short-term memory and working memory capacities, which are among the first processing abilities to reach full maturity, can be viewed as the bridge between cognitive-intellectual and knowledge-skill areas of development. Long-term memory is part of this bridge as well but is more centrally important to knowledge and skill acquisition. Many motor skills require a prerequisite level of physical and sensory development as evidenced by the visual scanning necessary for reading, scanning that a three year old cannot do and a six year old can do. Many gross-motor and fine-motor skills also require a specific level of physical maturation. The fact that we remember so little prior to age five suggests that sensory experiences and prerequisite levels of physical and conceptual (categorization and beginning non-abstract concepts) apparently need to be in place in order for us to formulate places, files, or schematic files where we can store information long-term. I don’t claim to have a lot of knowledge in this area, but I do want to make the point of a loose interconnectedness or interdependency among the domains of development. B. I think it is interesting that in the many decades since Jean Piaget (1948, 1952) put together his theory of cognitive development, no other theorist has contributed a viable competing theory, and no theorist has carried his theory much beyond where he left it. Jerome Bruner went the furthest with his stages of enactive, iconic, and symbolic representation of the world around us, and his constructivist, discovery learning model that builds on Piaget. The mountains of research that Piaget’s theory has generated largely support his original theory. Lev Vygotsky, who worked in Russia at the same time Piaget was at work, is credited in some texts for addressing better than Piaget the issue of the social and cultural context of development, that is, for considering external influences on cognitive development. But for anyone who has read all that Piaget has had to say about development including moral development, the conclusion that he failed to address external and social factors is unfounded. His concepts of adaptation to the environment via assimilation, accommodation, and 3 conceptual schemas (assimilation involves interpreting the environment based on existing schemas, and accommodation involves the construction or reconstruction of schemas when novel information requires) imply an interaction with the environment as does his extensive explanation of autonomy (intrinsically driven goodness) and heteronomy (externally imposed goodness) and how relationships with parents (adults) and peers bring about each. Vygotsky (1989) does address the adult-child or teacher-learner relationship in more detail as this pertains to knowledge and skill development, and defining this relationship as central to socialization and cultural transmission, but beyond his concept of the “zone of proximal development” (things the learner is ready to learn with guidance in the form of scaffolding, support gradually removed), he says nothing about qualitative changes in how children think as they mature. Put the two theories together and you perhaps have a near complete theory of mental growth complete with internal and external variables addressed. 1. Piaget’s Stages of Cognitive Development: Piaget’s theory of cognitive development identifies four stages marked by qualitatively different modes of thinking. Interaction with the environment and maturation gradually alter the way children think. a. Sensorimotor: The child begins to interact with the environment; coordination of sensory input and motor responses; development of object permanence; begin to explore environment; experience the world through the senses and exploration (looking, hearing, touching, mouthing, etc.); object permanence; stranger anxiety b. Pre-Operational: The child begins to represent the world symbolically; early symbolic thought marked by irreversibility, concentration, and egocentrism; assume you know what they know; cannot decenter; represent things with words and images but have no logical reasoning abilities; pretend play; egocentrism; rapid language development. c. Concrete Operational: Children learn rules such as game rules and the law of conservation, and they take them very seriously; mental operations are applied to concrete events only; mastery of conservation and hierarchical classification; cannot think abstractly; thinking logically about concrete events; grasping concrete analogies and performing math operations; conservation; mathematical transformations d. Formal Operational: Mental operations are applied to abstract ideas; begin logical, systematic thinking; imagine hypothetical events; manipulate symbols in their minds the adolescent can transcend concrete situations and think about the future and their own thinking; abstract reasoning; reflection; thinking about thinking; potential for moral reasoning 2. Vygotsky’s theory includes the following components: a. Children’s cognitive development is heavily influenced by social and cultural factors via relationships. b. Children’s thinking develops through dialogues with more capable people, usually parents and teachers. c. The Zone of Proximal Development is the range of tasks a child cannot master alone. Even though they may be close to having the necessary mental skills, they need guidance in order to complete the tasks. d. Scaffolding is a framework of temporary support. Adults help children learn how to think by using scaffolding or by supporting their attempts to solve problems and discover principles. Scaffolding must be responsive to children’s needs. Scaffolding involves changing the level of support over the course of teaching something — the more skilled person/teacher adjusts the amount of guidance to fit students’ current performance level. 4 e. Language and Thought: young children use language to plan, guide, and monitor their behavior in a self-regulatory fashion – Vygotsky called this “inner speech” or private speech. 3. Jean Piaget (1948, 1952), William Damon (1983, 1988), Robert Selman (1980, 1990), Martin Hoffman (2000), Lawrence Kohlberg (1970, 1983), and Jerome Kagan (1984) have added to our understanding of cognitive development by identifying qualitative changes in their concepts of authority, fairness, self, justice, right and wrong, and perspective taking, etc., concepts related to social-moral development, but concepts nonetheless. ARTISTIC-CREATIVE: A. Converging Perspectives on Creativity: Now that Robert Sternberg has included creative intelligence as one of his three forms of intelligence along with analytical and practical, and Howard Gardner has included among his eight types many that imply creative problem solving and imaginative selfexpression, we are likely to see in the near future researchers and theorists attempting to identify qualitatively distinct stages of creative and artistic development. Gardner back in the seventies published a book entitled The Arts and Human Development, but it did not lead to a stage model of artistic development, at least not in the sense of Piaget’s cognitive stages or Kohlberg’s moral reasoning stages. The study of creative thinking has in the past been an isolated area of study but one that has received lots of attention from Paul Torrance and Sidney Parnes, and to a lesser extent Guilford and Renzulli who are more interested in intelligence and gifted education, respectively. Torrance and Parnes have not laid out a complete series of stages, but they have observed age-specific changes such as the third-grade slump in creative fluency, flexibility, and originality. The work of researchers such as Florence Goodenough who used children’s drawings to reflect conceptualcognitive development may also prove useful to those looking at stages of artistic and/or creative development. B. Lowenfeld’s Stages of Artistic Development: The one person I am aware of who has identified stages of artistic development, with emphasis on visual art, is Lowenfeld. “Viktor Lowenfeld (1970) emphasizes the unfolding character of the child’s developmental stages and urges teachers to avoid intervening in the natural course of the child’s artistic expression. According the Lowenfeld, this natural progression yields two differing orientations of the world. The ‘haptic’ person who relies mainly on kinesthetic responses to the environment, and the ‘visually’ minded person who perceives that world in a more literal way. He believes that these types are genetically determined. Lowenfeld’s theory includes the idea that the child’s sensory perception of the world needs to be developed and that the child’s imagination is developed through direct experiences. Lowenfeld’s most valuable contribution to the study of child art is the concept of systematic creative and mental stages of development through which individual children progress. He believes that a child’s artistic development is both stage and age related. In his book Creative and Mental Growth, Lowenfeld identifies these stages that children progress through as” (Grenfell, 1999): 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. The scribbling stage: 2 to 4 Years The pre schematic stage: 4 to 7 Years The schematic stage: 7 to 9 Years The gang age: 9 to 12 Years The age of reasoning: 12 -13 Years The Crises of adolescence: after 13 Years LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT: 5 16 of 25 consonant sounds by 30 months Early Language Development See cat! Telegraphic means lacking connection words Grpmph ! Cat! said with gestures; serves as whole sentence 9-12 months is the quiet period since there is a decrease in vocalization Gradual narrowing of sounds to meaningful phonemes of the language being learned PERSONALITY AND RELATED AFFECTIVE-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT: A. Personality is a relatively stable pattern of thinking, feeling, and behaving that distinguishes one person from another (Davis and Palladino, 2005, p 459). Some are more convinced by the stability of our distinctive characteristics across time and situations than others, with Erving Goffman ( ) perhaps being the social psychologist least convinced that there is anywhere near the consistency that many assume. Personality takes time to solidify, and it is in the teenage years that people consolidate their roles, characteristics, beliefs, values, etc. into a clear sense of who they are and where they are going. This is why personality disorders are not ordinarily diagnosed until the age of 18. Before this age, there is typically too much change, and even at the age of 18, people are still trying on different behaviors, attitudes, values, goals, etc. to see how comfortably the fit the personality molding still in progress. 6 B. Although all theoretical schools of thought in psychology would like to think that they have adequately explained personality development, only two areas have contributed explanations that I view as significant. The first is psychoanalytic, with emphasis on Erik Erikson, and other is the ethological attachment theory or Mary Ainsworth (1978) and Jonathan Bolby (1982). To round things out, we need to acknowledge the contribution of psychologists who have studied temperament. So I will devote some space to each of the three beginning with temperament. 1. Temperament is the biological-genetic basis for the self-expressive, arousal, and self-regulatory components of personality. In 1984 Buss and Plomin proposed the following criteria for temperament: inherited, present early in development, predictive of later personality development. These rudimentary roots of personality are evident in infancy in the forms of activity level, irritability, fearfulness, sociability, etc. In 1977 Thomas and Chess stated that childhood temperamental characteristics are relatively innate and well-established by 2-3 months of age. They identified three types of temperament evident in infancy: a. Easy ─ high approach response; positive mood (mild to moderate intensity); quick adaptability; b. Difficult ─ high withdrawal response; frequent negative mood of high intensity; slow adaptability; c. Slow-to-warm-up ─ many withdrawal responses (mild to moderate intensity); slow adaptability. 2. The attachment theories of Ainsworth and Bolby include types of attachment, or lack thereof, between the mother and baby that are strangely similar to those identified by our temperament researchers and thus assumed to be present at birth. So could it be that one of the two schools is attributing cause to the wrong thing? Could it be either the type of mother-child attachment that produces these types and related personalities later in life or genetically determined predispositions but not both? Ainsworth and Bolby offer the postulate that the human infant is pre-adapted to respond to its caregiver. They contend that attachment behaviors promote close proximity to the caregiver so that the child can be protected from danger — an evolutionary function. They identify four types of attachment behavior that are largely determined by the mother’s or caregiver’s behavior toward her baby. These types are as follows: a. Secure — After separation from the mother, the baby is positive greeting by mother and is comforted b. Ambivalent — After the mother returns the baby is not comforted by a passive mother even though comfort is sought by her baby, and the baby shows anger or resistance. Mothers of ambivalent infants tend to be inconsistent, insensitive, and unpredictable in their interactions with their babies. c. Insecure-Avoidant — After separation from the mother, the baby avoid contact when she returns and avoids her gaze. The mothers of insecure-avoidant babies tend to be averse to physical contact, are inclined to interfere unnecessarily, and generally appear emotionally unavailable or dismissive. d. Insecure-disorganized — After the mother returns the baby appears disorganized and confused. The mothers of insecure-disorganized infants are typically suffering from an unresolved trauma, such as abuse or the unresolved loss of an attachment figure, which results in their babies being afraid of them. The mother may actually be abusive or neglectful. (Ainsworth, 1982) 7 CHILD ATTACHMENT STYLE PARENT ATTACHMENT STYLE SECURE: Limited distress, continued exploration after initial reunion SECURE/AUTONOMOUS: developmentally appropriate interaction; recognizes significance of attachment. AVOIDANT: child appears indifferent DISMISSING: dismissive about attachment; withdrawn and rejecting RESISTANT OR AMBIVALENT: child appears distressed and is preoccupied with caregiver and clingish PREOCCUPIED: recognizes significance of attachment but is preoccupied with past and appears angry; blurred or unclear boundaries DISORGANIZED/DISORIENTED: difficult to categorize reunion with caregiver; describes 80% of maltreated children. UNRESOLVED/DISORGANIZED: frightened by memory of past; trauma promotes momentary disassociation; scripts child into past dramas Bolby has also identified stages of normal attachment for behavior: a. b. c. d. Birth to 2-3 months — Undiscriminating social responsivenss 2-3 months to 6-7 months — Discriminating social responsiveness 6-7 months to 3 years — Active proximity seeking /true attachment 3 years and older — Goal-corrected partnership 3. The third major contribution to personality theory development comes from the psychoanalytic school and the psychosocial theory of Erik Erikson in particular. I’m passing over Freud’s psychosexual theory because it just doesn’t make sense to me. Erikson on the other hand identified psychosocial crises that we must address and resolve at different points in our lives and explained how the resolution of these depends on significant others including, for babies and children, their mothers or primary caregivers. Erikson assumes a genetically determined unfolding of maturation, but he proposes that how we turn out is a function of social/ environmental forces and experiences and their interaction with genetics. He calls this the epigenetic principle. Erikson’s theory proposes eight stages during our lives with each bringing us a psychosocial crisis or conflict that needs to be resolved interactively. Each involves confronting a question such as, “Who am I and where am I going?” The stages are described above in terms of personality traits that are potential outcomes from handling these crises. Each stage provides pivotal opportunities for personality qualities or ego strengths and virtues to develop interactively The crises are as follows: 8 a. Trust versus Mistrust — infants and babies who are deciding, “Is my world predictable and supportive?” A successful resolution yields hope and faith; an unsuccessful resolution brings withdrawal and sensory distortion. b. Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt — toddlers are asking, “Can I do things myself, or must I depend on others?” A successful resolution yields a strong will and independence; an unsuccessful resolution brings impulsivity, compulsivity, and self-doubt.. c. Initiative versus Guilt — young children are deciding, “Am I good, or am I bad?” A successful resolution yields purpose, courage, and a good imagination; an unsuccessful resolution brings cruelty, inhibition, and fear of failure. d. Industry versus Inferiority — older children are asking, “Am I good at things or am I a failure?” A successful resolution yields competence, skill, pride, and a conscience; an unsuccessful resolution brings a lack of confidence and a feeling of inferiority. e. Identity versus Role Confusion — adolescents are asking, “Who am I really, and where am I going in life?” A successful resolution yields fidelity and loyalty; an unsuccessful resolution brings fanaticism and repudiation. f. Intimacy versus Isolation — young adults are asking, “Shall I share my life with others or go it alone?” A successful resolution yields a capacity to love and trust; an unsuccessful resolution brings promiscuity and exclusivity. g. Generativity versus Absorption — middle age people are asking, “Will I produce something of value or leave a legacy?” A successful resolution yields caring and altruism; an unsuccessful resolution brings a tendency to over-extend and to be rejecting. h. Integrity versus Despair — older people are asking, “Have I lived a full life and taken advantage of what life offers?” A successful resolution yields wisdom; an unsuccessful resolution brings despair and presumption. SOCIAL-MORAL: The last two domain I will address as a pair, social and moral, are difficult to separate. Moral development, in fact, is not only linked with social development, but also with cognitive and emotional development. You can’t take the perspective of others or engage in moral reasoning and understand justice, equality, equity, fairness, authority, etc. unless you can reason on some level; you can’t empathize or acquire and follow the dictates of conscience unless you have a normal range of feelings. So separating and combining these domains of development tends to be somewhat arbitrary and related to the purpose at hand. I refuse to separate moral and social since moral development emerges from relationships and communities and since the strength of community is directly proportional to the moral functioning and concern for others among its individual members. Social and moral development and the moral emotion and thinking on which they depend can be broken down into qualitatively different stages, and all researchers and theorists who have worked in this combined area or these component areas have offered stage models of development. Before I briefly describe some of these, I think it would promote your understanding to cover first some of what we know about our emotionality as a species, presumably determined by evolution. A. Foundations of Moral Emotion: I’ve already touched on temperament with its social-emotional aspects, and some of the social-affective characteristics that Erikson, Bolby, Ainsworth, Thomas, and Chess have found us to have as a species and as individuals. Beyond this, Jerome Kagan, Martin Hoffman, and others have pretty much established that we are wired to be empathetic. This is evident even in infancy when a newborn baby while still in the hospital will become distressed and begin crying when it hears another baby crying. This is referred to as global empathy. Then as the toddler and young child develop caregivers are able to use this natural ability to feel others’ distress as a way to get them to begin taking the perspective of others, at least affectively, including their victims. 9 Hoffman devoted a good part of his 2000 book to explaining how this foundation and the related use of disciplinary inductions by adults largely ensures the normal emergence of conscience. Conscience normally begins to form at about age six, and prior to this, the child is dependent on parents to do the right thing. The parents have at their disposal clear expectations, the strategic use of consequences, and their understanding of the child’s natural affective empathy. Young children cannot take the perspective of others intellectually, that is, they cannot describe to you how a situation looks from another’s point of view. It is not until formal operational thinking and at least late childhood that this capacity develops. Without this, moral autonomy or intrinsically motivated goodness isn’t possible. Many including Piaget, Hoffman, Baumrind, Kagan, William Damon, Selman, and Youniss have described the kinds of relationships with peers and adults needed in order for this internal, conscience-driven motivation to do the right thing to result. It does not happen suddenly nor does it first appear in fully mature form. Havighurst (1953) introduced the concept of an “authoritarian conscience,” which is essentially the internalization of the parents’ moral voice about right and wrong, and he said it forms if discipline is moderate thus allowing for identification, and if love is combined with discipline. Diana Baumrind, William Damon, Jerome Kagan, and Martin Hoffman have added details to this early but accurate description of the ideal parent-child relationship. Baumrind (1967), for example, mentions (a) firm but fair discipline, (b) high expectations for morally maturity, (c) extensive communication but not necessarily lectures about right and wrong, and (d) lots of love and nurturing. It sounds simple and straightforward, and it is, but maintaining the right balance and choosing the right level of monitoring are not easy things to do. Affection for children Affection for adults Elation Joy Delight Excitement Distress Anger Jealousy Disgust Fear Months 0 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 Emotions are rapidly differentiated from an initial capacity for excitement (K.M.B. Bridges, 1932). The chart above shows at what age a baby/toddler acquires each of the emotions shown. This unfolding is presumably a result of brain growth as well as environmental stimulation and learning. 10 B. Moral-Affective Stages of Development: The researchers/theorists whose findings/propositions are juxtaposed below have collectively brought what we know about moral-affective development up to the level of understanding we have had about moral-cognitive development for decades now thanks to the work of Jean Piaget, Lawrence Kohlberg, and, most recently, William Damon. Those shown in the three columns to the right still living and have contributed hugely to our understanding of empathy, how it eventually comes complete through the acquisition of perspective-taking, and how conscience emerges through a combination of inborn affective empathy (without intellectual perspective-taking), parent-child relationships that take advantage of this natural ability to feel others’ pain and add such things as love and firm yet fair discipline, and perhaps mental health maintained through the resolution of developmental crises as described by Erikson. Moral-Social-Affective Development Havighurst Infants no information Age 0-1 Toddlers Early Elementary Middle Childhood Late Elementary Late Childhood Need to become Trusting, open, and no information discomfort at another's distress Need to become Independent, and Self-Regulatory Empathy Willful feelings of concern that limit aggression or be self-doubting Beginning of moral Need to take Initiative responsibility; the and Imagine or may Dawn of be cruel and critical throughout life Conscience Authoritarian Conscience: voice of parent taken in as a moral guide via love & discipline Rational Conscience: through cooperation with peers and an understanding of rules Middle School Early Adolescence Complete Set of Moral Principles High School Late Adolescence no information Hay Global Empathy Hopeful or will be fearful through life Age 2-3 Preschool Early Childhood 4-5 Hoffman Kagan Erikson Moral emotion of guilt presumably experienced when aggression is not controlled no information natural nonselective prosocial tendency emotions of shame and guilt Perspective Taking the Move from a need for initiative to need for Industry, Skill, and competence Need to be Competent or do things well or will feel inferior and be unable to work well with others thereafter cognitive component of empathy combines with affective component that is present at birth; guilt and self-scorn related to irresponsibility and over-indulgence are presumably experienced here Need to form an Moral emotion of Identity or consolidate Anxiety related to roles, identifications, and characteristics or will be insecure, compulsive, or even deviant; tend to be clannish and preoccupied with how they are perceived by peers inconsistency between beliefs and actions presumably emerges sometime after late childhood or during adolescence Selman Damon prosocial behavior more selective and declining no information no information no information Can’t distinguish their perspective from that of others; know self in terms of unrelated surface characteristics Know people have different viewpoints but take one at a time and favor their own; understand self in terms of comparisons Better understanding of different view- points and know they can have more than one & mixed feelings; same as above for self Step outside situation and see as complex; have third-party view of self, others, and relationships; know self in terms of effects on otherpeople. Understand self in terms of personal philosophy and plans for the future Developed by Gordon Vessels © © C. Stages of Friendship Development, Perspective Taking, and Self: There is another group of researching theorists who have focused even more on the social-interpersonal domain of development with specific emphasis on how friendships, play, and perspective-taking change with age. Youniss looked at friendship through children’s stories as shown in the left column in the chart that follows; Selman looked at friendship in terms of how children relate to one another, how much reciprocity is involved in these relationships, and how enduring they are. I especially like his use of “unevenhanded reciprocity” for children in the seven to ten range (roughly) wherein they buy into 11 give and take but will take more from their friends than they give if they can get away with it. Coupled with these terms are the terms “fair-weather friendships” that last as long as there is no conflict. Selman also looks at perspective-taking ability as shown in the third column. Finally, William Damon looks at self-concept, and as you can see from the descriptions below, this becomes increasingly interpersonal or reflective of comparisons with and effects on others. Although not shown in this chart, Damon also looks at changes in children’s perceptions of fairness, which evolves from a purely self-centered notion to an understanding of equality, and eventually an understanding of equity. Like Jean Piaget, he also looked at children’s conceptions of and attitudes toward authority, which not surprisingly perhaps moves toward more and more questioning of authority rather than blind acceptance. This look at fairness and authority carries Damon into the area of moral-cognitive development that is typically associated with Lawrence Kohlberg and Jean Piaget. Friendship / Self / Perspective Taking Youniss Children's Friendship Stories Damon Selman self-concept and view of self Friends Are . . . Friendship Is . . . Perspective Taking Ability those who live nearby; those with whom they are playing; those whose toys they want cannot distinguish their own perspective from that of others understand self in terms of unrelated surface characteristics subjectivity and unevenhanded reciprocity; know feelings, not just activities, keep them together recognize others may have different viewpoints but can consider only one at a time and favor their own; a one-way social perspective understand self in terms of comparisons with others, particularly peers more cooperative, evenhanded reciprocity; fairweather friendships don't withstand conflict have a better understanding of peoples’ different viewpoints and know they can have more than one or mixed feelings; two-way perspective play activities reciprocal interest; friendships withstand conflicts 13 13 year old children tell stories about assisting each can step outside a situation and view its complexities and have a third-party perspective on self, others, and relationships 13-18 other autonomous interdependence: close and intimate friends grant each other the right to have other friends 3-6 6 6-8 8 8-10 10 10-13 18 18-25 6 year old children tell stories about sharing toys and play activities no stories gathered 10 year old children tell stories about playing and mutual understanding and sharing exclusive trust replaces 18 year old adolescents tell stories about sharing private thoughts and same as above understand self in terms of effects on others of personal characteristics understand others in terms of personal philosophy and plans for the future feelings Developed by Gordon Vessels © D. Stages of Moral Reasoning and Conceptions of Fairness: Below you will find a graphic illustration of Lawrence Kohlberg’s states of moral reasoning, which reflect how children and adults conceptualize justice. I do not have a similar graphic illustration of William Damon’s findings about children’s changing ideas about fairness and authority, but I will try to describe these stages in this section. 1. Kohlberg looked only at reasoning and the issue of justice and has been criticized for ignoring moral emotion and caring in particular. What he offers is a model that reflects the gradually expanding external world of children as they grow older as shown in the fourth column: egocentric, individualistic, interpersonal, organizational, societal, and universal. As this scope broadens, the child’s understanding of justice also changes. It begins with children viewing right 12 as that which brings rewards, and wrong as that which brings punishment — strictly external and what Piaget has termed heteronomy. As the social sphere broadens, the notion of justice begins to involve relations with and acceptance by others. They then come to understand the purpose of societal rules and laws and hopefully reach the point where they formulate universal moral principles that are not time, situation, or culture specific. Following the chart directly following, I have broken down the moral reasoning levels and described each with bulleted points. KOHLBERG'S BEHAVIORAL-SOCIAL-COGNITIVE THEORY View of "Right" Primary Levels That Which Gains Approval From Others PreConventional (self-serving) That Which Adheres to Rules or Principles Conventional (otherserving) PostConventional (principleserving) Motivation Perspective Age/Grade Punishment Avoiding Egocentric Pleasure/ Reward Seeking Individualistic Acceptance/ Approval Seeking Interpersonal Grades 3-5 Late Childhood Rule Following/ Status Seeking Organizational Grades 6-8 Early Adolescence Law Abiding/ Rights Respecting Societal Grades 9-12 Late Adolescence Justice Seeking/ Conscience Driven Universal Adulthood Preschool Early Childhood Grades K-2 Middle Childhood Developed by Gordon Vessels © a. Preconventional 1 ● ● ● ● ● They display heteronomous or adult-dependent morality. They think in absolutes of right and wrong. They have an egocentric viewpoint. They are good to avoid punishment or gain rewards. They view the value of life the way they do the value of objects. b. Preconventional 2 ● ● ● ● They see right as that which satisfies their needs. They have a concrete, pleasure/ reward-seeking, individualistic perspective. Their cooperation is instrumental, and they exchange favors to satisfy needs. The value of life is viewed as instrumental to need satisfaction. c. Conventional 3 ● ● ● ● They view right as what gains approval. They have an interpersonal, Golden Rule, good-child/bad-child perspective. They gain approval by being caring and accommodating toward significant others. They view the value of life in terms of affectional bonds. 13 d. Conventional 4 ● They view right as doing one's duty, showing respect to authority, and maintaining social order. ● They have an organizational-need, societal-need, law-maintaining view. ● They view life as sacred within the context of a scheme or moral rights. e. Post-conventional ● They view right as guarding basic rights and legal contracts, or as meeting mutual obligations in context of societal rights and standards. ● They have a law-creating, moral-legal view that obligates them to honor social commitments. ● Principled moral reasoning. 2. Damon’s (1983, 1988) model is quite a bit different. He has his stages numbered and lettered, but I will add a descriptive term for each pair of stages. a. Pre-Moral: ● ● 0-A: (4 years old): They make no attempt to justify choices and feel they should get more because they want more. They distort adult orders to fit their wishes. 0-B: (5 years old): They justify choices in a selfish, after-the-fact way and view authority only as a block to satisfying their own desires. b. Egalitarianism: ● ● 1-A: They view fairness as equality. Authority is confused with the power to enforce. 1-B: They view fairness in terms of merit and reciprocal obligation. Fairness takes on value in its own right. Children see obedience as legitimate trade for adult favors and help. c. Equity and Benevolence: ● ● 2-A: They view fairness as a right of all, and they view leaders with knowledge as more legitimate. 2-B: They view justice (by age 10) as context dependent and can make reasoned decisions based on claims and conditions, but their perspective is limited to the situation. d. General ideas about early adolescents: ● ● Self-understanding is based on social and personality traits rather than the abilities of childhood or the beliefs of late adolescence. Will gain ability to view situations that involve disparate claims to justice from a wider perspective than the situation and can apply moral principles. e. General ideas about late adolescents: ● Self understanding or self-concept is based on beliefs, philosophies, and thoughts rather than personality qualities as was the case in early adolescence. PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT OR MATURATION: 14 A. “The two main processes that cause individuals to change across their lifetimes are maturation and learning. Maturation refers to developmental changes that occur as a result of the aging process, not from injury, illness or other life experience, including learning. Maturation information is encoded in an individual's genes. On the other hand, learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior (or potential behavior) as a result of experience or practice” (Pritchard, 2005). REFERENCES: Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1982). Attachment: retrospect and prospect. In C.M. Parkes and J. Stevenson-Hinde, (Eds.) The Place of Attachment in Human Behavior. (pp 3-30) New York: Basic Books. Buamrind, Diana (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monograph, 75, 43-88. Bowlby, John. (1982). Attachment and Loss. Vol. 1. NY: Basic Books Bridges, K. M. B. (1932). Emotional development in early infancy. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 37. Bronfenbrenner, Urie (1972). The roots of alienation. In Urie Bronfenbrenner , ed, Influences on Human Development. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden Press. Buss, A. and Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early personality traits. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Damon, William (1977). The Social World of the Child. San Francisco: Josse-Bass. Damon, W. (1983). Social and Personality Development: Infancy Through Adolescence. New York: W.W. Norton. Damon W. (1988). The Moral Child. New York: The Free Press. Davis, S. and Palladin, Joseph (2005). Psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prrentice-Hall. Erikson, E.H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York:Norton. Erikson, E.H. (1964). Insight and Responsibility. NewYork: Norton. Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York: W. W. Norton & Company; Toronto: George J. McLeod Limited. Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the Life Cycle. New York: W. W. Norton & Company; Toronto: George J. McLeod Limited. Gesell, Arnold, Halverson, H., Thompson, H., Ilg, F.L., Castner, B., Ames, L.B., and Amatruda, C. The First Five Years of Life: A Guide to the Study of the Preschool Child. New York: Harper and Row. 15 Goffman, Erving ( ). The Presentation of Self In Everyday Life, Grenfell, Jenny (1999). Defining child art. Part of the website, Arts Education: Teaching the Visuals. Defining child art was retrieved at http://www.deakin.edu.au/education/visarts/defining_childart.htm The homepage for Grenfell’s website is http://www.deakin.edu.au/education/visarts/ She is at the School of Social and Cultural Studies in Education at Deakin University, Geelong. Havighurst, Robert. J. (1953). Human Development and Education. New York: Longmans, Green and Company. Hoffman, Martin (2000). Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Cambridge University Press. Hoffman, Martin (1977). Moral internalization: current theory and research. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Psychology, Vol. 10, New York: Academic Press. Hoffman, Martin (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.) The Development of Prosocial Behavior. New York: Academic Press. Hoffman, M. (1983). Empathy, guilt, and social cognition. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), The Relationship Between Social and Cognitive Development. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates. Hoffman, Martin (2000). Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Kagan, J. (1984). The Nature of the Child. New York, NY: Basic Books. Kohlberg, L. (1970). Education for justice: A modern statement of the Platonic view. In N. F. Sizer and T. R. Sizer (Eds.), Moral Education: Five Lectures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kohlberg, Lawrence (Ed.) (1983). The Psychology of Moral Development. San Francisco: Harper & Row. Piaget, Jean (1948/1965). The Moral Judgment of the Child. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press; New York: Free Press. Piaget, Jean (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press. Piaget, Jean (1978). The Development of Thought. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Selman, Larry (1980). The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding: Developmental and Clinical Analyses. New York: Academic Press. Selman, Larry and Schultz, L. H. (1990). Making a Friend in Youth. Chicago Press. Chicago: The University of Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 16 Vygotsky, L. S. (1989). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Werner-Wilson, Ronald J. (2005). Types of attachment. A PPT slide show retrieved from http://www.public.iastate.edu/~hd_fs.511/lecture/Types_of_Attachment.ppt Youniss, J. and Volpe, J. (1978). A relational analysis of children's friendship. In W. Damon (Ed.), NewDirections for Child Development, Vol. 1: Social Cognition. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 17