Psychology of Eating Thursdays, 9-11:30 am, 5461 Franz Instructor: Traci Mann, Ph.D. Office: 5627 Franz Office Hours: Thursdays, 3-4 pm Class Format Because this course is a seminar, its success is based on both the efforts of the instructor and on the efforts of the participants: We will be learning together. To help stack the deck toward a successful course, I am making discussion participation and discussion leading worth a fairly large part of your grade. Each week you will be responsible for reading the articles in the reader. There are usually three articles per week, and I have tried extremely hard to not be unreasonable about the reading load. The reader can be purchased from Course Reader Material at 1137 Westwood Blvd (between Kinross and Lindbrook). The phone number there is 443-3303. Class Participation Part of your grade will be based on class participation. To prepare, you should carefully read and be familiar with the assigned readings. You may also want to make a note of a few issues or questions that you would like to discuss. To earn the maximum amount of points for participation, you need not (indeed, you should not) dominate discussion. You merely need to contribute to the discussion each session and help it move forward. While I strongly encourage you to do all the readings, if for some reason you have not thoroughly read all the articles on a particular week, you should not feel that you cannot add to the discussion. You can and you should. Leading Class Discussion Each class member will be required to lead class discussion one time during the quarter. You will choose your session on the first day. Leading class discussion is more difficult than most people realize. I encourage you to prepare thoroughly for this role. First, consider how you want to organize class. Depending on the nature of the readings, class might be ideally divided up into separate sections for each article, or in other cases, the articles can all be discussed together. However you decide, you are in charge of the clock, of keeping discussion on track, and of preventing ridiculous digressions and encouraging useful ones. I’ll be there to work on this with you, of course, and I won’t let anything awful happen. I find it helpful if the leader starts with an overview that includes a summary of the readings. Often in seminars people jump right to the critiquing of the readings before everyone in the room is clearly up to speed on what the readings say. I encourage you to allot a reasonable amount of time to just reviewing what the readings say before opening the floor for criticism. Once everyone is clear on what the readings say, you can move the discussion to a few key questions or thoughts that you want the discussion to be structured around. To get people talking (remember, the class meets at 9 am), you might want to start with a discussion question that allows people to talk about their personal experiences. Then you can move into the more technical issues. Feel free to be creative. You may want to try to structure a discussion around solving a fairly specific problem. Some of the topics of the course lend themselves well to this type of discussion, in which the goal is to design an experiment. It is often useful to prepare a handout with bullet points of discussion questions or topics. The hand-out might also contain a summary of the readings. This could help structure discussion. If you do prepare a handout, I will be happy to make copies of it for you the day before class. The day you lead discussion, you will be encouraged to bring a food item for the class that has particular meaning for you or that is particularly interesting to you. It should be a snack, not a meal, and it should be a food (or drink) that we can ponder. Be prepared to tell us what that food means to you, or in some other way to tie it into the discussion. If this assignment provides you with a financial hardship, please tell me and I will help out. (With that said, I don’t expect anyone to spend more than a tiny bit of money on their food item.) Reaction Papers For four of the remaining nine weeks (as you choose), you will be expected to turn in a short reaction paper on the assigned readings from that week. Papers will be due at the beginning of class, and will not be accepted late. Each paper should be approximately 2 pages, doublespaced. The purpose of these papers is twofold: First, they should help you read the weekly assignments critically and thoroughly, and second, they should help generate discussion. I will comment on the papers and grade them on a check plus/check/check minus basis. A reaction paper features your intellectual reaction to a topic covered in the course reader. A "topic" can be a phenomenon, a theory, a concept, an experiment, and so forth. The ideal start is to think about which issues in that week’s readings have grabbed, bothered, or puzzled you. Once you have an idea for a topic, choose the type of paper you want to write. You may critique an article you read; propose a new experiment that would clarify open questions; or apply issues or comments from previous sessions to the current readings. You may start with your own everyday observations and develop a theoretical analysis; or you may start with a theoretical prediction and apply it to your own life. All papers should be short and to the point. So tell your reader what you are planning to do in the first paragraph. Then, in the remaining space, implement this plan. No matter what type of paper you choose to write, you must clearly go beyond summarizing other people's thoughts. A mere summary of the reading material is not acceptable. Final Paper You will be required to write a final paper (5-7 pages, double-spaced). It will be due at noon on the Thursday of exam week. I will not accept late papers. If you give me your paper at least one week early, I will be happy to provide early feedback and editing comments. In the paper you should propose an experiment to test some hypothesis relevant to eating generated either from class discussion or the readings. The experiment should be ethically acceptable and pragmatically viable. Write the paper like an introduction and methods section for an APA empirical journal article. In the introduction, give a concise description of the literature that led to your hypothesis. Describe a sufficient amount of literature to justify your study. In the last paragraph of your introduction, clearly state your hypothesis and your independent and dependent variables. Divide your method section into the usual parts (participants, procedures, etc). As with all methods sections, the goal is to give enough information so that your readers can replicate the study, should they desire. After the methods section, discuss in a paragraph or two the strengths and weaknesses of your methods. Explain what your study would be able to address and what it would not be able to address. List your references in APA style at the end. I will not be particularly picky about the formatting, but I will be interested in seeing what you cite. Grading Your grade will be based on the following items: 1. Your final paper 30% 2. Your reaction papers 20% (4 papers @ 5% each) 3. Leading discussion 25% 4. Class participation 25% Reading List Week 2: Physical Effects of Human Starvation Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., Taylor, H.L. (1950). The Biology of Human Starvation, Volume I. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Chapter 4: General features of the Minnesota experiment Chapter 5: Body weight (selections) Chapter 17: Basal metabolism (selections) Week 3: Psychological Effects of Human Starvation Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., Taylor, H.L. (1950). The Biology of Human Starvation, Volume II. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Chapter 38: Behavior and complaints in experimental starvation and rehabilitation Chapter 41: Psychological case studies Chapter 42: Psychological effects – interpretation and synthesis Week 4: What Makes People Obese? Gladwell, M. (1998). The Pima Paradox. The New Yorker, February 2. Bouchard, C. (2002). Genetic influences on body weight. In C. Fairburn & K. Brownell (Eds.) Eating Disorders and Obesity (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Skim. Keesey, R. & Hirvonen, M. (1997). Body weight set-points: Determination and adjustment. Journal of Nutrition, 127, 1875S-1883S. Garner, D. & Wooley, S. (1991). Confronting the failure of behavioral and dietary treatments for obesity. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 729-780. Read pages 741-750 only. Week 5: Is it Unhealthy to be Fat and Will Dieting Fix it? Garner, D. & Wooley, S. (1991). Confronting the failure of behavioral and dietary treatments for obesity. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 729-780. Read pages 733-740 only. Wickelgren, I. (1998). Obesity: How big a problem? Science, 280, 1364-1367. Ernsberger, P. & Koletsky, R. (1999). Biomedical rationale for a wellness approach to obesity: An alternative to a focus on weight loss. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 221-260. Week 6: Restraint and Binge Eating Herman, C.P. & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of Personality, 43, 647-660. Polivy, J. & Herman, C.P. (1985). Dieting and binging: A causal analysis. American Psychologist, 40, 193-201. Week 7: Stress, Emotion, and Eating Greeno, C. & Wing, R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 444-464. Macht, M., Roth, S., & Ellgring, H. (2002). Chocolate eating in healthy men during experimentally induced sadness and joy. Appetite, 39, 147-158. Week 8: Social, Cognitive, and Environmental Aspects of Eating Clendenen, V., Herman, C.P., & Polivy, J. (1994). Social facilitation of eating among friends and strangers. Appetite, 23, 1-13. Rozin, P., Dow, S., Moscovitch, M., & Rajaram, S. (1998). What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychological Science, 9, 392-396. Young, L. & Nestle, M. (2002). The contribution of expanding portion sizes to the US obesity epidemic. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 246-249. Week 9: Developmental Factors in Food Selection Davis, C. (1939). Results of the self selection of diets by young children. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 41, 257-261. Birch, L. (1999). Development of food preferences. Annual Review of Nutrition, 19, 41-62. Week 10: Sociocultural Factors in Food Selection/Chocolate Craving Rozin, P. (1996). Sociocultural influences on human food selection. In E. Capaldi (Ed.), Why We Eat What We Eat: The Psychology of Eating. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rozin, P. & Schiller, D. (1980). The nature and acquisition of a preference for chili pepper by humans. Motivation and Emotion, 4, 77-101. Michener, W. & Rozin, P. (1993). Pharmacological versus sensory factors in the satiation of chocolate craving. Physiology & Behavior, 56, 419-422.