Am I Racist: Exploring Unconscious Biases and Prejudices among Fairfield University Students A report by: Rachael V. Harriman '08 Stephanie E. Chavarro '08 Fairfield University 1073 North Benson Road Fairfield, CT 06824 www.fairfield.edu Acknowledgments: First, we would like to thank Dr. Betsy Gardner, our advisor on the project, for her valuable assistance, support, and advice. We would also like to thank the Brinkman Student Diversity Grant Advisory Committee for all of their help along the way. Thank you very much to Joe Ginise, the FYE leaders, and Dr. Renee White for allowing us to come into your classes and conduct our activity with your students. And finally, thank you to the entire Brinkman Student Diversity Grant team and everyone involved in sponsoring and funding the project for giving us this wonderful opportunity. Am I Racist Introduction The idea for this project stemmed from a recognition of the fact that racial/ethnic biases and discrimination are widespread on Fairfield University’s campus, but ironically, very few students are willing to admit that they might be racist or that they use certain racial 1 stereotypes to evaluate and judge members of other groups. It is a common occurrence to hear phrases such as “I don’t mean to sound racist, but…” or “I'm not racist or anything, but Black/Hispanic/Asian people are so…”. Racial and ethnic jokes that perpetuate ignorant stereotypes of different groups are rampant; almost every student on this campus has undoubtedly told, heard, or laughed at a racist joke during his or her time at Fairfield. Most students have misconceptions about the true nature of racism, and they tend to define racial bias and racial discrimination in very narrow ways that only encompass the most blatant and obvious forms of such attitudes and behaviors. Whereas overt expressions of racial prejudice were once the norm in our society, racism has now become so politically incorrect and socially unacceptable that openly discriminating against or expressing hatred toward members of racial/ethnic minority groups is widely discouraged and frowned upon. However, racial stereotypes and biases have hardly disappeared; they have simply taken on a different form that is much more subtle and covert, but just as pernicious and destructive, serving to undermine the principles of equality and egalitarianism that our society is purportedly founded upon, as well as relegate racial and ethnic minorities to a marginalized and disadvantaged position in our society. Nowadays, no one wants to address issues of racism or consider the fact that they themselves might be racist, but the fact remains that many minority groups are still treated in ways which reflect the innumerable racial/ethnic stereotypes that are prominent in our society. Unfortunately, these racist views are usually so culturally ingrained and beyond conscious thought that it is extremely easy for people to ignore them and continue to believe that they are completely free from any racial biases or prejudices. The downside to living in a culture that outwardly condemns racism, and in which expressions of racism tend be very covert and unnoticeable, is that people are motivated to rationalize and justify their racist thoughts and behaviors without examining the root causes of them or working to eradicate and change them. When racism can be so easily disregarded and overlooked, it makes it all the more difficult to directly address the problem of racism in our society or encourage people to examine and alter their racist views. People cannot attempt to free themselves from racial biases and stereotypes when they are not even aware that they have them, or when they cannot admit to themselves that their treatment of racial/ethnic minorities is colored by ignorant and unfounded racial prejudices. Literature Review The decline in obvious and direct displays of racism over the past few decades has led to the misguided notion that White Americans have largely overcome their racist beliefs and attitudes. The Civil Rights Act and other forms of legislation, as well as changing social norms and values, have made many forms of overt discrimination not only immoral but also illegal, which has led Am I Racist 2 to a pronounced decline in their occurrence in recent years. The results of a number of surveys measuring attitudes toward African Americans have highlighted a “dramatic shift toward more egalitarian and less racist views over the last fifty years” (Spencer & Wolfe, 1996). However, this data does not by any means herald the end of racism in American society. Many researchers feel that the majority of Americans who report positive or egalitarian feelings toward African Americans may still harbor ambivalent or negative attitudes that are masked by their outward declarations of support and acceptance. Survey research reported by Dovidio and Gaertner (1991) has shown that although Whites claim to promote equal treatment for all races, they demonstrate opposition to specific policies intended to achieve racial equality, including affirmative action programs and government intervention in school integration. These results indicate that while many Whites endorse equal rights and fairness in theory, they are not as supportive of actual measures intended to achieve these goals. According to Brown (1995), there are three main characteristics of Whites’ present-day attitudes toward African Americans. The first is that the majority of Whites, no matter what their political orientation, do not approve of traditional forms of blatant or overt discrimination. Secondly, Whites tend to use non-racial factors and explanations to justify any behaviors that could be perceived as racist or which result in unequal outcomes for African Americans. Lastly, many Whites harbor feelings of ambivalence toward African Americans, which are compounded by American notions of individualism and meritocracy that cause Whites to blame the disadvantages of African Americans on the flaws or shortcomings of individual members of that race, rather than on larger societal or structural forces. The persistent patterns of subtle bias that have been demonstrated in numerous studies on race raise important questions about how expressions of racial prejudice have changed over time, the extent to which racism continues to play a role in today’s society, the various ways in which racial biases are manifested, and the consequences of modern forms of racism for both Whites and African Americans. The theory of aversive racism offers answers to many of these questions, providing an overview of the contemporary, indirect forms of prejudice that pervade modern-day society and which, despite their subtlety, have significant effects on the lives of many minorities. Aversive racism theory was first proposed by Kovel in 1970, but was subsequently expanded upon and extensively researched by Dovidio and Gaertner. Dovidio and Gaertner (1973) describe aversive racism as a subtle type of bias which takes on a much different form than the blatant, old-fashioned type of racism that was so common before the Civil Rights movement. Aversive racists view themselves as non-prejudiced, support racial equality in principle, and claim to sympathize with the victims of past discrimination. Despite these egalitarian self-images, aversive racists also possess negative racial feelings and beliefs that they are largely unaware of, but which become apparent during their day-to-day interactions with African Americans. Dovidio and Gaertner view aversive racism as a result of normal cognitive and cultural processes that are experienced by almost everyone in American society. People have an innate tendency to place others into clearly delineated categories that separate and distinguish one’s own group from other groups, and this classification process can easily lead to negative evaluations of the out-group that contribute to the formation of racial prejudices. People also Am I Racist 3 tend to adopt the dominant stereotypes of their culture, as well as the justifications for racial hierarchies that maintain inequalities and disparities between different races. However, in addition to these stereotypes and racial biases, many Americans are also presented with cultural values concerning racial equality, which can ultimately lead to a conscious, explicit endorsement of egalitarian principles paired with unconscious negative perceptions of African Americans. This complex set of attitudes is the main feature of aversive racism, and it leads to a distinctive pattern of discriminatory behavior. Since aversive racists harbor unconscious feelings of uneasiness toward African Americans, they do their best to avoid interracial interactions, and experience anxiety or discomfort when these interactions are unavoidable. Part of their anxiety stems from a concern about doing or saying something that would make them appear to be prejudiced, so they are very conscious about treating African Americans in ways that cannot be construed as reflecting any kind of racial bias. However, their prejudices will manifest themselves in subtle ways that they are unaware of or can rationalize in non-racial terms. The “aversive” in aversive racism refers to two different kinds of aversion: aversive racists view interracial interactions as aversive and try to avoid them as a result of their deeply ingrained biases, but they also find the idea that they might be racist to be aversive as well, since they believe that they are non-prejudiced and support egalitarian ideals (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2005). Despite their attempts to act in a non-discriminatory manner, the unconscious biases and prejudices of aversive racists can still be expressed in subtle, covert ways that may not be initially apparent. Aversive racists do not discriminate in every situation that involves an interracial interaction; certain conditions must be present in order for their negative racial feelings to be expressed. In situations where social norms and appropriate behavior are clear, and discrimination would be obvious, aversive racists will not treat Whites and African Americans unequally. However, in more ambiguous situations that are not guided by clear norms and in which the right thing to do is less evident, discrimination will be manifested in “indirect and rationalizable ways” (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2005). In other words, aversive racists will only discriminate when they can justify their behavior on the basis of factors other than race. In this way, they can maintain their non-prejudiced self-images and continue to believe that they treat members of all races fairly. Nevertheless, even though they are unaware of it, the behaviors that aversive racists engage in can have potentially harmful and debilitating effects on African Americans. An extensive collection of empirical research has been conducted on aversive racism and the many ways it can be demonstrated in everyday interactions. An early study conducted by Dovidio and Gaertner (1977) was inspired by the Kitty Genovese case, in which a woman was stabbed to death in front of numerous witnesses, none of whom intervened to help. Psychologists explained this behavior by claiming that when there are multiple witnesses to an incident in which someone is in peril, each individual person feels a diminished sense of responsibility and is therefore less likely to intervene than if they were the only bystander and carried the sole responsibility of helping. Dovidio and Gaertner created a laboratory experiment which aimed to determine if race would play a role in the occurrence of the bystander effect during an emergency intervention situation. Am I Racist 4 In the experiment, White participants witnessed a staged emergency that involved either a White or an African American victim. Some of the participants were led to believe that they would be the only witness to the scene, while other participants were led to believe that there would be other White witnesses. The results showed that when White participants believed that they were the only witness, they helped both Whites and African Americans about 85% of the time, showing no overt racial bias (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1977). However, when they believed that there were other witnesses, they helped White victims 75% of the time while only helping African American victims 37.7% of the time (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1977). In this case, they could rationalize their discriminatory behavior on the basis of a factor other than race, which was the belief that the other witnesses would get involved and take responsibility for helping. The results of this study are fairly alarming, since real-life situations in which Whites fail to provide assistance to African Americans as often as Whites could potentially have very serious effects on people’s lives. Another way in which African Americans are often discriminated against is through hiring practices, and it is very likely that aversive racism is one of the factors contributing to the persistent and widening gap between the economic statuses of Whites and African Americans. In a study by Dovidio and Gaertner (2000), White college students were presented with excerpts from an interview and asked to evaluate African American and White applicants for a position as a peer counselor at their university. The various candidates were classified as either strongly, moderately, or weakly qualified for the job. When candidates were either strongly qualified or weakly qualified, there was no discrimination against African American candidates relative to White candidates. In this case, the choice of whether or not to select a particular candidate was clear, and choosing White candidates more often than African American candidates would have been a blatant display of racial bias. However, when the candidates were moderately qualified, and therefore the decision of whether or not to hire them was more ambiguous, White participants recommended the White candidates 76% of the time while only recommending the African American candidates 45% of the time (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000). In this situation, the participants could attribute their decisions to factors other than race, and thereby avoid having to acknowledge their deep-seated racial prejudices. In addition to employment, African Americans are frequently discriminated against when it comes to college admissions decisions. In a study by Dovidio, Gaertner, Hodson, and Kawakami (2002), participants were presented with college applicants who had either consistent qualifications (strong SAT scores and strong high school performance, or weak SAT scores and weak high school performance) or mixed qualifications (strong SAT scores and weak high school performance, or weak SAT scores and strong high school performance). The results showed that discrimination against African Americans relative to Whites did not occur when the applicants’ credentials were consistently strong or consistently weak. However, when the applicants’ credentials were mixed, and therefore ambiguous and harder to evaluate, discrimination against African American applicants did occur. When African American applicants had high SAT scores and weak high school achievement, the participants tended to weigh SAT scores as low in importance and high school achievement as high in importance, and when the African Americans had low SAT scores and strong high school achievement, the participants tended to weigh high school achievement as low in importance and SAT scores as high as Am I Racist 5 importance. In other words, the participants weighed application criteria in ways that “systematically justified or rationalized discrimination against African Americans” (Dovidio, Gaertner, Hodson, & Kawakami, 2002). Other research on aversive racism has focused on how it relates to resistance to affirmative action. Studies have demonstrated that although Whites may support affirmative action in principle, they often oppose the implementation of specific affirmative action policies. One study (Dietz-Uhler, Dovidio, Drout, Gaertner, & Murrell, 1994) assessed attitudes toward affirmative action as a function of the targeted group (African American, handicapped, or elderly persons), and the way the policy was framed (with or without social justification). The results found that policies directed at benefiting African Americans were resisted more strongly than policies for physically handicapped or elderly persons. The type of justification also had an effect on the level of support for the policy. When policy descriptions were justified on a macro-level (remedying past discrimination or increasing cultural diversity), they received stronger support than policies that were justified on a micro-level (preferential treatment or reverse discrimination). All of this research demonstrates the profound effects that even subtle, unconscious forms of racism can have on African Americans; it can restrict their educational and economic opportunities, impede the implementation of affirmative action policies, and cause them to be denied assistance from Whites when they are in trouble. Aversive racism has many other consequences as well, one of which is attributional ambiguity. Because so many White people discriminate subtly against African Americans in their daily interactions with them, African Americans are often left wondering whether or not the ways in which they were treated or evaluated by Whites were fair or were the result of racial prejudice. Research indicates that “African Americans receive lower grades than Whites at all levels of schooling" (Spencer & Wolfe, 1996), and one explanation for this underachievement is that negative stereotypes, such as the belief that African Americans are lazy or less intelligent than Whites, undermine African Americans’ school performance and discourage them from trying to succeed. When African Americans are consistently discriminated against or treated unfairly by Whites, in ways that are obvious to them but unobvious to Whites, they begin to believe that every evaluation of them that is made by a White person is influenced by racial stereotypes, and they may begin to live up to these stereotypes in a kind of self-fulfilling prophecy. Clearly, aversive racism is a pervasive problem in today’s society, and has many negative consequences for its victims. Diminishing aversive racism is a worthy goal to undertake, but the task is certainly not an easy one. One method that has been suggested and tested through research is to educate people about aversive racism and encourage them to examine their own behavior and unconscious biases. Ford, Grossman, and Jordan (1997) used a case study technique in an introductory psychology course in order to teach students about the nature of aversive racism and the subtle ways it can manifest itself in everyday life. Students were presented with a case study describing a White professor’s behavior toward an African American student named Tim, who frequently missed class, did not do well on assignments, and did not take advantage of the professor’s offers of help. Students were asked to discuss possible reasons for Tim’s behavior and how they would have dealt with the situation if they were the professor. Students then learned that Tim had failed the class and that the professor Am I Racist 6 had attributed Tim’s failure to a lack of academic skills and motivation. However, Tim enrolled in the professor’s course again and earned an A the second time. When the professor asked Tim what had changed, Tim told the professor that he now owned a car and was therefore able to come to class; before, he had often missed classes because White bus drivers would not stop for him. The professor then concluded that he had acted in a racist manner because his conclusions about Tim’s poor performance had been premature and unfounded, and he had failed to consider the possible obstacles that Tim could be facing as an African American. Students had conflicting views about whether or not the professor’s actions were indicative of racial bias. Some students believed that the professor was not racist because he had treated Tim the same way he would have treated any other student, but others felt that the professor’s color-blind perspective was exactly what made him racist, and that the professor should have taken into consideration the fact that Tim might be facing unique circumstances or barriers because of his race. Some students also thought that the professor’s actions should not be considered racist because he did not deliberately discriminate against Tim, while other students felt that the professor’s thoughts and behavior, although not intentionally biased, still served to discriminate against Tim and were therefore racist. After this activity, the students were presented with a lecture on aversive racism, and were also introduced to the racist implications of the fundamental attribution error; Whites are more likely to attribute the behavior of African Americans to “internal characteristics and personality traits rather than external or situational factors” (thus committing the fundamental attribution error) than they are for Whites (Ford, Grossman, & Jordon, 1997). After the lecture, students were better able to understand how the professor’s unconscious racial biases and stereotypes had influenced his perceptions of the situation with Tim and the way in which he handled it. Many students admitted that they had also committed the fundamental attribution error by attributing Tim’s behavior to characteristics such as laziness, shyness, or irresponsibility. The researchers concluded that teaching about unintentional racism can help make people aware of how their own behaviors and thought processes may be serving to subtly discriminate against minorities. If people realize that racial prejudice is not always overt or blatant, and that it can often be the product of normal cognitive processes that are outside of one’s conscious awareness, they may be more willing to examine their own unconscious biases and attempt to change their attitudes and behaviors. Methods The intention of our project was to explore attitudes toward racism among students at Fairfield University and encourage students to reflect on their own racial prejudices that they may not even be aware of. We wanted students to realize that racial biases and discrimination are oftentimes very subtle or even unrecognizable, which makes it easy to believe that racism is no longer a problem in today’s society and allows students to perceive themselves as nonracist and egalitarian. However, despite the fact that racism today is much less obvious than it has been in the past, it is a safe assumption that the majority of the students on this campus possess at least some racial/ethnic biases and stereotypes that are deeply ingrained and most likely outside of their conscious awareness. Am I Racist 7 We conducted an educational activity with students that was designed to stimulate selfreflection and an increased understanding of the challenges and obstacles that so many minorities face in their everyday lives. Our participants included four Freshmen Year Experience (FYE) classes, with a total of 45 students, and one section of Dr. Renee White's Race, Gender, and Ethnic Relations course, with a total of 24 students. The participants were presented with a modified version of the aforementioned case study about the African American student named Tim (Ford, Grossman, & Jordan, 1997), who is subtly discriminated against by his professor and treated according to the professor’s unconscious and deeply rooted stereotypes about African Americans. The story is told from the professor’s point of view and is broken up into three segments; after each segment, the participants were asked to write down their answers to a few reflection questions which encouraged them to put themselves in the shoes of both Tim and his professor. The question we were most interested in was the question that asked the students to indicate whether or not they thought that Tim's professor had behaved in a racist manner. Students' responses to this question were coded on a scale from – 4 to 4, with a score of – 4 indicating that they strongly disagreed that the professor was racist, and a score of 4 indicating that they strongly agreed that the professor was racist. We also administered the Modern Racism Scale (McConahay, 1986), which is a seven-item survey that consists of various statements about African Americans, and the students were asked to indicate the degree to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement on the survey by choosing a number on a scale from – 4 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Students' responses on each of the seven items were averaged together to give them an overall score. Since the statements on the survey were all derogatory and portrayed African Americans in a negative light, a lower number (meaning that the students generally disagreed with the statements) indicated a low racial bias, while a higher number (meaning that the students generally agreed with the statements) indicated a high racial bias. After completing both the Tim story and the survey, participants were given handouts which explained social psychological concepts that are relevant to Tim’s story, including aversive racism, the fundamental attribution error, and institutionalized racism. We assessed the activity’s effect on our participants by administering the survey to some of the students before the activity and to other students after the activity. By comparing the responses of the students who took the survey before reading the story and those who took the survey after reading the story, we were able to determine whether or not the activity had a positive effect on students' attitudes toward African Americans. Am I Racist Results Figure 1: Mean scores on the Modern Racism Scale for students who completed the Tim story first compared with students who completed the Tim story second Unpaired t-test for Score Grouping Variable: Story 1st/2nd Hypothesized Difference = 0 Mean Dif f . DF t-Value 1st, 2nd -1.173 65 -3.132 Group Info for Score Grouping Variable: Story 1st/2nd Count Mean Variance P-Value .0026 Std. Dev. Std. Err 1st 46 -2.432 1.294 1.137 .168 2nd 21 -1.259 3.665 1.914 .418 0 ore -.5 8 As Figure 1 shows, the students who did the Tim story activity before completing the Modern Racism Scale had a mean score of – 2.43 on the scale, while the students who did the Tim story activity after completing the Modern Racism Scale had a mean score of – 1.26 on the scale. These results indicate that the students who read Tim's story prior to completing the survey were more inclined to disagree with the racist statements on the survey than those who did not read Tim's story until after they completed the survey. Therefore, the story had a positive effect on racial attitudes. Am I Racist 9 Figure 2: Extent of agreement with the question “Do you agree with the professor's conclusion that he is racist?” for students who completed the Tim story first compared with students who completed the Tim story second 0 Cell Mean for Is prof racist? -.2 -.4 -.6 -.8 -1 -1.2 -1.4 -1.6 -1.8 -2 1st 2nd Unpaired t-test for Is prof racist? Grouping Variable: Story 1st/2nd Hypothesized Difference = 0 Mean Dif f . DF t-Value 1st, 2nd 1.909 66 2.481 P-Value .0156 As Figure 2 shows, the students who did the Tim story activity before completing the Modern Racism Scale had a mean score of 0.00 in response to the question asking them if they thought Tim's professor was racist, while the students who did the Tim story activity after completing the Modern Racism Scale had a mean score of – 1.91 in response to that question. These results indicate that the students who read Tim's story prior to completing the survey were more likely to agree that the professor was racist in comparison to those who did not read Tim's story until after they completed the survey. This may be because the derogatory statements in the survey primed students to view African Americans more negatively. Am I Racist 10 Discussion and Recommendations Our results demonstrate that students who complete the Tim story activity show significantly less negativity in their attitudes toward African Americans than those who have not participated in the activity. Therefore, we believe that by promoting racial awareness and familiarity with the concept of aversive, or subtle, racism, this type of activity can make racism more meaningful to students and encourage them to examine their own thoughts and feelings about race and racial prejudice. Because of this potential, we believe that the Tim story or a similar activity should be used as a tool to foster increased racial understanding and selfreflection among the Fairfield University community. The activity should be incorporated into FYE classes or a freshman orientation activity or retreat devoted to diversity. It could also be used in diversity-related floor programs within the dorms or in a classroom setting for a course that focuses on racial issues. New diversity classes should be implemented and strongly promoted within the university's curriculum, and more extracurricular programs that encourage discussion about the topic of race should be initiated. Fairfield University encourages its students to be committed to social justice and caring for other people. The Jesuit ideals that this school is founded upon emphasize human rights, dignity, and respect for all people, as well as a commitment toward helping and improving the lives of subjugated, marginalized, and disadvantaged groups. We believe that the reduction of racial biases and an increase in attitudes of tolerance and egalitarianism among Fairfield University students is an important step toward achieving these aforementioned goals and promoting an environment of humanity, compassion, and acceptance that celebrates diversity and allows members of all racial and ethnic groups to learn and grow from their experiences with one another. By emphasizing the Jesuit principle of “cura personalis”, or care for the whole person, Fairfield University has made a commitment to fostering not only the academic development of its students, but also their personal, moral, and spiritual development. In regard to issues of racial prejudice and intolerance, it is not enough to simply teach students about such problems in a way that makes them seem far removed and irrelevant from the students’ own lives; we want students to internalize ideals of equality and acceptance and strive to make a personal contribution toward bettering the lives of other people, including racial/ethnic minorities. Students must be encouraged to put themselves in the shoes of people who are discriminated against and hindered as a result of their race in order to better understand the challenges and obstacles that such groups face in their everyday lives. Self-reflection, social awareness, and empathy are critical tools that must be cultivated and developed in each and every student on this campus, and we hope that our project has served as one of many future steps that will bring us closer to achieving this goal. References Brown, E.M. (1995). The tower of babel: Bridging the divide between critical race theory and “mainstream” civil rights scholarship. Yale Law Journal, 105(2), 513-547. Dietz-Uhler, B.L., Dovidio, J.F., Drout, C., Gaertner, S.L., & Murrell, A.J. (1994). Aversive racism and resistance to affirmative action: Perceptions of justice are not necessarily color blind. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 15(2), 71-86. Dovidio, J.F., & Gaertner, S.L. (2005). Understanding and addressing contemporary racism: From aversive racism to the common ingroup identity model. Journal of Social Issues, 61(3), 615-639. Dovidio, J.F., Gaertner, S.L., Hodson, G., & Kawakami, K. (2002). Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal biases and interracial distrust. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8(2), 88-102. Ford, T.E., Grossman, R.W., & Jordan, E.A. (1997). Faculty forum: Teaching about unintentional racism in introductory psychology. Teaching of Psychology, 24(3), 186-187. McConahay, J. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 91–125). San Diego: Academic Press. Spencer, S. J., & Wolfe, C.T. (1996). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their overt and subtle influence in the classroom. American Behavioral Scientist, 40(2), 176-185.