Kim and Leung - week 5 – forming and reacting to overall fairness: a cross cultural comparison This study compared the cross-cultural formation and reactions toward overall fairness perception of employees from the US, China, Korea, and Japan. Distributive justice was related to overall fairness less strongly for Americans and Japanese than for Chinese and Koreans. In contrast, interactional justice was related to overall fairness more strongly for Americans and Japanese than for Chinese and Koreans. As expected, materialism seems to provide a coherent account of these cultural diVerences. In addition, overall fairness showed a stronger eVect on turnover intention for Americans than for Chinese and Koreans. For job satisfaction, the eVect of overall fairness was stronger for Americans than for Chinese, Koreans, and Japanese. Power distance seems to provide an adequate account of these cultural diVerences. There are 3 main kind of justice : distributive, procedural, and interactional. All of them generate overall fairness judgements Once an overall fairness judgment is formed, as the theory suggests, it will exert influence on other attitudes and behaviors. Perceived overall fairness has been found to influence various outcomes that are beneWcial to employees and organizations, such as satisfaction, commitment, trust, and organization citizenship behaviours. One important area of organizational justice research concerns the role of cross-cultural differences in outcomes of justice. Cross-cultural perspectives on organizational justice are important, since they contribute to assessing the generalizability of organizational justice beyond north America. Several researches examined how country of origin affects employees’reactions to perceived injustice but 2 questions remain unaddressed: little attention has been paid to identify the contributions pf various facets of justice perception to overall fairness in various cultures ( one culture can weighted it more than others) so the first goal of this study is to assess how procedural, distributive and interactional justice have differential impacts on overall fairness perceptions across countries. Contrary to the implicit or explicit assumption made in previous cross-cultural studies that all East Asians are alike, probably because of their characterization as high in collectivism and power distance (, East Asian countries actually differ substantially in their attitudinal and behavioral patterns To summarize, it is important to examine how distributive, procedural, and interactional justice are diVerently weighted in the formation of overall fairness, and how reactions to overall fairness diVer across East Asia and the US. To achieve these ends, the present paper examined how distributive, procedural, and interactional justice are related to overall fairness and how overall fairness aVects job satisfaction and intention to leave their organization in the US, China, Japan, and Korea. Lam et al.tested the eVects of distributive justice on job performance,absenteeism, and job satisfaction, and found that distributive justice significantly aVected all the outcomes. However, they found that the latter relationships were not signiWcantly diVerent between American and Hong Kong Chinese samples. On the other hand, Pillai et al. found that distributive justice significantly affected trust and job satisfaction in the Chinese sample, whereas distributive justice significantly affected trust but not job satisfaction in the American sample. With regard to procedural and interactional justice, a few studies have investigated whether the influence of procedural and interactional justice perceptions on employee outcomes vary across countries, but the results of these studies are also mixed. On the one hand, country does not affect people’s reactions to perceived procedural and interactional justice. On the other hand, there are several studies that show significant cross-cultural differences in the effects of procedural and interactional justice on employee outcomes. Taking stock of current cross-cultural justice research, several important issues have to be addressed. For example, there are inconsistent findings regarding US–East Asian differences in the effects of justice perceptions on employee outcomes. In addition, current justice measures are restrictive for cross-cultural justice research for several reasons. First, current justice measures assess the respondents’ perceptions of the treatment they receive rather than the fairness of the treatment. For instance, interactional justice is measured by the extent to which supervisors treat employees with dignity and respect. There may be cross-cultural differences in the extent to which a given personal treatment is perceived to be fair to employees. Second, current justice measures may not appropriately capture cross-cultural differences in defining justice perceptions. For example, distributive justice is typically assessed by the extent to which rewards are fairly allocated based on the equity rule. One of the factors that may explain cross-cultural differences in how distributive, procedural, and interactional justice perceptions impact on overall fairness judgments is materialism (a devotion to material needs and desires, to the neglect of spiritual matters Hypothesis 1. Countries with higher materialism weigh distributive justice more highly in forming overall justice perceptions. Specifically, Chinese and Koreans weigh distributive justice perception more highly in forming overall fairness perceptions than do Japanese and Americans. Hypothesis 2. Countries with lower materialism weigh procedural justice more highly in forming overall justice perceptions. Specifically, Japanese and Americans weigh procedural justice perceptions more highly in forming overall fairness perceptions than do Chinese and Koreans. Hypothesis 3. Countries with lower materialism weigh interactive justice more highly in forming overall justice perceptions. Specifically, Japanese and Americans weigh interactional justice perceptions more highly in forming overall fairness perceptions than do Chinese and Koreans. Country differences in reactions to overall fairness Based on the power distance frameworks, East Asians may differ from Americans in their reactions to perceived justice. In East Asia, where people show a stronger power distance, they tend to be tolerant of unfair treatment from the organization. According to Hofstede, power distance refers to “the extent to which a society accepts the fact that power in institutions and organizations is distributed unequally.” The norms of high power distance cultures legitimize inequality between those in high power positions and those in low power positions. As a result, for individuals with higher power distance, “inequality and injustice are expected and taken for granted, while they are not expected or seen as acceptable in lower power distance individuals”. In contrast, in a low power distance society, individuals tend to minimize status diVerentials and subordinates have higher expectations about respectful treatment from authorities. Subordinates with a low power distance also expect organizations to have fair practices that can constrain managers from arbitrarily exercising their authority Hypothesis 4a. Overall fairness is more strongly related to intention to leave an organization in the US, compared to the other three countries. Hypothesis 4b. Overall fairness is more strongly related to job satisfaction in the US, compared to the other countries. In summary, the predictions based on materialism group Japan with the US, whereas the predictions based on power distance pitch the US against the three East Asian countries. Discussion These findings support and extend Abramson and Inglehart’s (1995) conclusion that materialists emphasize material well-being such as pay and promotion, whereas post-materialists put a greater emphasis on ecological issues, human and animal rights, and the quality of life. According to Abramson and Inglehart, Chinese and Koreans are relatively more materialistic than Americans and Japanese, and the cultural differences we found are consistent with their findings. An unexpected finding is that procedural justice shows no significant cross-cultural difference, and the reason may be that it has both instrumental and non instrumental connotations Cross-cultural researchers need to pay more attention to what types of facet fairness are particularly important to people of different countries. An unexpected finding is that procedural justice shows no significant cross-cultural difference, and the reason may be that it has both instrumental and noninstrumental Connotations Another important result from this study is concerned with how Americans differ from Chinese, Japanese, and Koreans in reacting to injustice. As expected, job satisfaction was related to organizational injustice more strongly for Americans than for Chinese, Koreans, and Japanese. The results also suggest that Americans, compared with Chinese and Koreans, are more likely to leave their organization when they perceive organizational injustice, and the same is true for Japanese as well. With the exception of this unexpected finding, the general cultural argument that people who value collectivism and power distance react less negatively toward an authority that treats them unfairly is supported. To explain the lack of significant difference between Americans and Japanese in the effect of overall fairness on turnover intention, we speculate that in Japan, because companies emphasize the managerial principle of “wa,” which emphasize group (organizational) loyalty, harmony, and consensus We should point out, however, that the results need caution to interpret because the reliabilities for both materialism and power distance are low. One reason why materialism and power distance did not explain all the country differences may be due to their low reliability Limitations They used just the common method variance so it would be used to compare the results using other methods of measurement.