Using the Online Psychology Laboratory (OPL) to illustrate the Concept of Implicit Attitudes by Pamela Marek, Kennesaw State University This third article in the OPL (http://opl.apa.org/) series focuses on involving students in a procedure for assessing attitudes that was designed by Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz (1998) to overcome the bias present in self-report techniques. Because it involves associations that are activated outside of our conscious awareness, the Implicit Association Test is an appropriate activity to introduce when discussing consciousness. However, the demonstration is directly linked to prejudice; thus, introductory psychology textbooks typically include information about such implicit measurement in the social psychology chapter. Attitudes, evaluative judgments, are an important focus of social psychology coverage in most introductory textbooks. Frequently, attitudes are examined in an ABC framework, involving affective, behavioral, and cognitive correlates and consequences (Olson & Zanna, 1993). Insofar as the attitude of prejudice, an evaluative judgment of a person or object based solely on group membership, the affective manifestation of negative affect might include hostility, anxiety, or even fear. The cognitive correlate might be a negative stereotype, with people believing that the stigmatized group is lazy, aggressive, or unmotivated. The behavioral manifestation might involve a predisposition to discriminate against, or behave negatively toward, the stigmatized group. Knowledge of the distinction between prejudice and discrimination may help students better understand the meaning of their IAT results. Students who are typically willing to express their attitudes on a wide variety of topics may not readily realize that at times they may be unaware of their attitudes. In fact, explicit attitudes, those that students are able and willing to express, may conflict with implicit attitudes, those that students are unaware of or are unwilling to express. Prejudice is a prototypical area in which implicit and explicit judgments may conflict. Explicit measurement of prejudice, whether related to age, sex, ethnicity, or sexuality, is clearly prone to social desirability effects. Few people, with the possible exception of members of hate groups, would publicly admit to holding prejudiced attitudes against or negative stereotypes of older people, women, minority groups, or homosexuals. Instructors might introduce students to early techniques for circumventing social desirability by explaining an experiment involving a bogus pipeline, perhaps linking an experimental description to coverage of research methods. For example, Sigall and Page (1971) used a bogus pipeline to assess stereotypes, randomly assigning Caucasian men to one of two groups. One group rated “Americans” and “Negroes” on a series of traits using a standard pencil and paper test, whereas the other group were led to believe that a wheel that they turned to indicate their ratings could detect if they were untruthful. This bogus pipeline group held more negative stereotypes of Negroes and more positive stereotypes of Americans than did the pencil and paper control. More recently, Greenwald et al. (1998) developed an implicit association test that has been used to measure implicit attitudes, defined as “introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past experience that mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling, thought or action toward social objects” (Greenwald & Benaji, p. 8). The test is based on the principle that people will respond more readily to pairs of concepts that they associate with each other, even if they are not consciously aware of the association. The on-line psychology lab includes such a test to measure implicit or automatic prejudice toward African Americans and Euro Americans. When introducing this activity to students, instructors might position it as a demonstration of an implicit measure of attitudes, rather than as a “test”. To some extent, this might assuage potential student discomfort in participating in what they might view as a sensitive activity. Prior to distributing the assignment to students, instructors are encouraged to visit the OPL website to obtain a class ID. This will enable instructors (or students) to download class data for further examination after the activity is completed. When students begin the demonstration, they are presented with two optional questions about explicit attitudes (preference for Whites vs. Blacks using a Likert-type scale and a “thermometer” measure of warmth-coldness toward Blacks and Whites). Next, to prepare for the IAT, students place one finger on each of two keys on opposite sides of the keyboard. For responses to be meaningful, students are instructed to proceed quickly. The actual demonstration contains five blocks of trials, summarized in the table below. Block 1 2 3 4 5 Task Initial target concept discrimination Associated attribute discrimination Initial combined task (Stereotypecongruent) Reversed target concept discrimination Reversed combined task (Stereotype noncongruent) Stimulus Key Stimulus African-American Left European face American face Key # of trials Right 10 Bad words (e.g. “agony”) Right 10 Left Good words (e.g. “joy”) African-American Left Europeanor bad words American of good words Right EuropeanAmerican face Left AfricanAmerican face Right EuropeanAmerican face or bad words Left AfricanAmerican face or good words Right For analysis, the key blocks of interest are the two combined tasks (Blocks 3 and 5). The IAT is scored by calculating the difference in average reaction time for Block 3 (the stereotype congruent condition) and the average reaction time for Block 5 (the stereotype non-congruent condition). A positive score is interpreting as suggesting an automatic association between African-Americans and negative terms, or as a preference for European-Americans. At the end of the demonstration, students are shown their IAT score and given the opportunity to save their data. Instructors are strongly encouraged to allot class time to discuss the results of the demonstration and to respond to potential student skepticism about the meaningfulness of this research technique. First, it is important to reassure students that a positive score does not mean that they display either prejudice or discrimination. Rather, Devine (1989) has demonstrated that whereas most people are aware of negative stereotypes about AfricanAmericans that may be activated automatically, people’s personal beliefs may conflict with the negative stereotype. Given time to consciously access their personal beliefs, lowprejudiced people may invest cognitive effort to inhibit the negative stereotype, thereby avoiding unpleasant affect or discriminatory behavior. In contrast, high-prejudiced people would not attempt to consciously inhibit prejudice or discrimination because their personal beliefs coincide with the negative stereotype. On the IAT, the rapid response requirement is purposely designed to eliminate the possibility of conscious inhibition of stereotypes, thereby measuring only the automatic component of the attitude of prejudice. Second, students may complain that, despite the Block 4 practice at reversed target concept discrimination, they had difficulty making accurate rapid classifications in the combined Block 5 task because the designated keys for European American and African American faces had been reversed from Block 3. Instructors might acknowledge that this is, indeed, a valid point, consistent with research findings that have revealed small order effects, with faster response latencies for whichever combined task is presented first. Instructors might take this opportunity to make a connection between proactive interference in memory and the manner in which interference may influence performance on the second target concept discrimination task. Instructors might also mention that some on-line versions of the IAT (e.g., Project Implicit at https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/ ) randomly select the ordering of the combined tasks to reduce the influence of order effects on aggregate scores for groups of participants. Another task alteration that reduces the order effect involves increasing the number of trials in Block 4, the reversed target-concept discrimination task (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005). For further discussion, instructors (or students) may opt to download the class data as an Excel file, in which the mean response latencies (in seconds?) for the stereotype congruent and stereotype noncongruent conditions may be computed and compared. It is expected that times will be faster for the stereotype congruent condition, suggesting the presence of an automatic association. Supporting this hypothesis, in a fascinating fMRI study in which response times on the IAT were linked to activation of the amygdala, Phelps et al. (2000, p. 731) included the following graph to illustrate the IAT results (in milliseconds rather than seconds). Does class data resemble this pattern? Note that Phelps et al. might serve as a link to coverage in biological psychology chapter relating the amygdala to emotional responses. RT (ms.) 1000 750 500 250 Stereotype noncongruent Stereotype congruent Condition An excellent follow up activity to the IAT involves directing students to the Southern Poverty Law Center’s Tolerance website, http://www.tolerance.org/hidden_bias/). By clicking on the link “Track U. S. hate groups” in the Tolerance Watch box on the left, students can engage in an eye-opening experience as they determine the variety of currently active hate groups in their home state and the beliefs of each of these groups. On a more positive note, clicking on links in the Do Something box can lead to the discovery of “10 ways to fight hate” or “10 ways to fight hate on campus.” Students might discuss the proposed solutions in class or by writing a brief paper relating the material to their textbook reading. Clearly, this site provides a wide range of opportunities for creative class-based on community-based assignments. Overall, the IAT activity provides students with an introduction to a cutting-edge, albeit controversial, research technique that has been used to study bias in a wide range of personally relevant areas. It is a stimulating tool with applications that extend well beyond introductory psychology to more advanced courses (e.g., social psychology, cognitive psychology, and research methods.) An abundance of material concerning IAT development, validity, applications, and controversies is available via links on Dr. Anthony Greenwald’s homepage (http://faculty.washington.edu/agg/iat_materials.htm). References Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-19. Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 835-848. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166-180. Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M. (1993). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 117-144. Phelps, O’Connor, Cunningham, Funayama, Gatenby, Gore, & Banaji (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdale activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729-738. Sigall, H., & Page, R. (1971). Current stereotypes: A little fading, a little faking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 247-255.