1 Behavioral Competence and Academic Functioning among Early Elementary Children with Disruptive Behaviors Kyongboon Kwon PhD, Susan M. Sheridan PhD, and Elizabeth A. Moorman PhD Nebraska Center for Research on Children, Youth, Families and Schools University of Nebraska - Lincoln Introduction Externalizing problems are related to a host of developmental risks in children (Schofield et al., 2008), and prognostic of academic underachievement and underattainment (Bub et al., 2007; Fergusson & Horwood, 1998; Hinshaw, 1992). Significant emphasis has been placed on the negative effects of behavioral problems, which has guided the development of numerous intervention programs to address these issues (see Farmer et al., 2002). Our understanding of and interest in the role of behavioral competence in academic functioning among children with externalizing problems appears to be relatively limited. This is a notable gap, given growing evidence that the positive impact of competent behaviors on academic functioning may outweigh the negative impact of externalizing problems (Caprara, 2000). Children’s behavioral competence may also serve as a protective factor in the face of disadvantages, such as parents’ limited education. Parental education has been found to be the strongest predictor of children’s cognitive and academic development (Mercy & Steelman, 1982; Suizzo & Stapleton, 2007), and parents’ limited educational attainment is associated underachievement (Magnuson, 2007). A shift of attention from the deficits of children with externalizing problems to the competencies they possess may be fruitful. A focus on the early elementary years is further beneficial given the importance of this developmental period for acquiring important skills to adapt to social and learning environments (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2009). Behavioral competence, as defined in this study, includes behaviors that allow children to function effectively at school, including prosocial skills, interpersonal skills, self-regulation, social responsibility, study skills, and cooperation (Caprara, 2000; DiPerna et al., 2002; Howse et al., 2003; McClelland et al., 2006; Wentzel, 1991). Understanding the role of behavioral competence in children’s learning during this period may play an important role in informing the direction of intervention efforts for children with externalizing problems. Purpose, Study Questions and Hypotheses The purpose of this research was to investigate how behavioral competence is related to academic functioning among a sample of early elementary school children who display externalizing problems. Specific questions were: 2 1. Is behavioral competence uniquely related to children’s academic functioning? Hypothesis: Behavioral competence will be negatively related to academic problems and positively related to reading and math achievement after taking into account children’s background characteristics and behavior problems. 2. Does behavioral competence buffer children’s achievement against the effects of risk, as manifested in parents’ limited educational attainment? Hypothesis: Children’s behavioral competence will moderate the relation between parents’ education and children’s achievement such that the adverse impact of parents’ limited education on children’s achievement will be attenuated when children display heightened levels of behavioral competence. Methods Participants Participants were recruited from 21 schools in a moderately-sized Midwestern city and surrounding communities. Participating children were selected via a multiple-gate screening procedure wherein teachers identified children who displayed externalizing behaviors (e.g., aggression, non-compliance). Participants’ characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Measures Behavior Assessment Scale for Children, Second Edition (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) – Teacher Reports (T-scores of M = 50, SD = 10) Externalizing problems composite: hyperactivity, aggression, and conduct problems School problems composite: attention problems and learning problems Adaptive skills composite: adaptability, social skills, study skills, leadership, and functional communication Woodcock-Johnson III Test of Achievement (WJ–III; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001; M = 100; SD = 15) Broad Reading: Letter-word identification, reading fluency, passage comprehension Math Calculation: Calculation, math fluency Outcome, Predictor, and Control Variables Outcomes: Academic problems as measured by School Problems composite on the BASC-2, reading achievement (Broad Reading) and math achievement (Math Calculation) as measured by the WJ-III. Predictors: Behavioral competence as measured by the Adaptive Skills composite of the BASC2. Control variables: Gender, age, ethnicity (White/non-Hispanic = 1; minority = 0), disability status (diagnosed disability and/or special education placement = 1, other = 0), externalizing problems, parents’ level of education (college and/or beyond = 1; less than college = 0) 3 Results Descriptive statistics and correlations of the central variables are provided in Table 2. Is Behavioral Competence Related to Children’s Academic Functioning? A series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted for each dimension of children’s academic functioning. In the first step, child demographic variables (i.e., gender, age, ethnicity, and disability status) were entered. In the second step, parental education was entered followed by children’s externalizing problems in the third step. Finally, adaptive skills were entered in the last step. The results are described below for each outcome and also presented in Table 3. Does Behavioral Competence Buffer Against the Risk of Parents’ Limited Education on Children’s Achievement? Given that parental education was related to reading achievement only, the second research question was addressed with regard to reading achievement. A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted. In the first step, child gender and parent education variables were entered. Child gender was controlled for given its significant effect on reading achievement as described in the previous analysis. In the second step, child adaptive skills were entered. Finally, the interaction between parent education and adaptive skills was entered. In testing an interaction effect, the adaptive skills variable was centered (Aiken & West, 1991) at 40 (At-Risk), which is the approximate average of the sample. The results are presented in Table 4 and Figure 1. Discussion Summary of Findings This research is unique in that it examined the contribution of behavioral competence to children’s academic functioning among a sample of children who display externalizing problems. The findings support and strengthen the evidence of the powerful role of behavioral competence in children’s academic functioning. Across three indices of children’s academic functioning (i.e., academic problems, reading and math achievement) children’s behavioral competence was a significant predictor over and above a variety of child background characteristics. Behavioral competence appeared to have relatively stronger effects over externalizing problems on academic functioning. Externalizing problems were related to teacher ratings of academic problems only but were not related to reading and math achievement. Moreover, after behavioral competence was taken into account, externalizing problems were not related to teacher ratings of academic problems. Children’s behavioral competence buffered the effects of limited parental education on children’s reading achievement. Among children of parents with less than a college education, children’s reading achievement was higher when displaying average levels of behavioral competence as opposed to displaying At-Risk behavioral competence. Limitations 4 Because the current research is based on a cross-sectional research design, causal effects between behavioral competence and academic functioning are not clear. The sample was relatively homogeneous ethnically. Approximately 75% of children were White as were the participating teachers (100%). Also, the sample is characterized by disproportionately more male children than female children. Thus, generalizability of the findings may be limited. Implications and Future Directions When designing an intervention to promote academic functioning for children with behavioral concerns, it appears important to consider children’s behavioral strengths in addition to their problems. Whereas the traditional assessment protocols have focused on problems and deficits, the findings appear to support the usefulness of strength-based assessment when working with children with behavior problems. Research supported by IES Grants #R305F050284 and #R305B080010 (Postdoctoral training), awarded to Susan M. Sheridan, PhD., Todd A. Glover, PhD, and James Bovaird, PhD. Poster presented at the 5th annual Institute of Education Sciences Research Conference, Washington, DC., June 2010. 5 Table 1. Participants’ Demographic Characteristics Child (N = 207) Gender Female: 25% Parent (N = 207) Age M = 34.73 (SD = 7.83) Male: 75% Grade Teacher (N = 82) Gender Female: 97% Male: 3% Ethnicity Kindergarten: 26% White/non-Hispanic: 87% Grade 1: 35% African American: 4% Grade 2: 26% Other: 9% Ethnicity White/non-Hispanic: 100% Grade 3: 13% Ethnicity Education White/non-Hispanic: 75% Less than high school: 5% African-American: 9% High school/some college: 50% Other: 16% College: 37% Advanced graduate degree: 8% Free/reduced lunch Eligibility: 49% Years in current position M = 9.38 (SD = 10) 6 Table 2. Descriptive Statistics for and Correlations Between Central Variables Child gendera Child Parent Child age ethnicityb Disabilityc educationd Externalizing Adaptive Academic problemse skillse problemse Reading Math achievementf achievementf M or % 74.39 % 6.52 75.13 % 43.48% 44.79% 68.03 41.36 60.28 100.38 103.70 SD -- 1.11 -- -- -- 10.92 6.49 8.14 16.06 14.01 Range 0, 1 5-9 0, 1 0, 1 0, 1 42 – 98 29 – 62 40 – 81 54 – 148 62 – 148 Externalizing .04 -.06 -.12 .13 -.12 Adaptive skills -.15 .05 .03 -.20* .19* -.39** Academic .23** .00 .13 .16 -.01 .34** -.54** -.16 -.07 -.07 -.09 .13 .02 .28** -.44** -.06 -.21* .04 -.29** .18* -.01 .27** -.28** problems problems Reading achievement Math .52** achievement *p < .05; ** p < .01. Note. a Male = 1. b White/non-Hispanic = 1. c Diagnosed disability and/or special education placement = 1. d College and beyond = 1. e BASC-2 composite. f WJ-III subtests. 7 Table 3. Hierarchical Multiple Regression Predicting Child Functioning from Behavioral Competence, Controlling for Background Characteristics Academic Problemse Reading Achievementf Math Achievementf Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 β β β β β β β β β β β β Gendera .20* .21** .20* .11 -.14 -.16* -.16* -.12 .03 .02 .01 ..04 Age -.04 -.04 -.01 .03 -.01 -.01 -.01 .01 -.20** -.19** -.19** -.17* .11 .12 .15 .13 -.01 -.02 -.02 -.02 .05 .04 .05 .05 .12 .11 .08 .04 -.09 -.07 -.07 -.03 -.29** -.28** -.28** -.24** -.08 -.05 .04 .17* .17* .12 .11 .11 .08 .29** .13 .02 .11 .05 .12 Ethnicityb Disability status c Parent educationd Externalizing problemse Adaptive skillse -.50** .28** *p < .05; ** p < .01. Note. a Male = 1. b European American = 1. c Diagnosed disability and/or special education placement = 1. d College and beyond = 1. e BASC-2 composite. f WJ-III subtests. .21* 8 Table 4. Interaction Between Parent Level of Education and Child Behavior Competence in Predicting Child Reading Achievement Reading Achievementd Independent variables Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 B B B Gendera -.6.66* -4.86 -4.62 Parent educationb 5.44* 3.70 5.06* .62** 1.10** Adaptive skillsc Parent education x Adaptive skills -.94* * p < .05. ** p < .01. Note. a Male = 1. b College and beyond = 1. c BASC-2 composite. d WJ-III subtests. Reading achievement 115 110 105 At-risk adaptive skills 100 Average adaptive skills 95 90 Low High Parent education Figure 1. Children’s reading achievement as a function of parents’ education and children’s adaptive skills. Children’s at-risk adaptive skills were centered at 40 and average adaptive skills were centered at 50. Parents’ low education was defined as having less than a college degree; high was defined as having a college degree or greater.