1 DOES A CONNECTION EXIST BETWEEN AUDITORY PERCEPTION ACCURACY ABILITIES AND EMOTIONAL EXITATATION? Kari Suoniemi Tampere University Department of Anthropology Finland Abstract This empirical research used statistical (SPSS) methods to determine whether people have significant differences to perceive, discriminate and memorize the basic elements of musical structures and whether these cognitive abilities were linked to emotional sensitivity. Two experiments were conducted: a musical ability test by A. Bentley, and a questionnaire containing rating scales to make self-reports how strongly students (N=201), choir-singers (N=120) and control group (N=70) were emotionally affected by their daily listened favorite music. The two questions asked were: 1) Are there significant musical test score differences between three groups: students, coir-singers and control group measured by Bentley’s musical ability test? 2) Does a significant connection exist between perceptual accuracy and memory abilities and primary emotional sensitivity intensities? Results displayed significant test score difference between choir-singer and control groups. There were five significant emotional intensity differences between high accurate (48-60 scores) and low accurate (1-37 scores) perception and memory ability category groups. Keywords: perception accuracy, musical schema, amygdala, emotion, cued recall, self-report, priming Introduction Emotional experiences are subjective perceptual and cognitive processes that may include overt behaviours and psychophysical reactions. Investigators agree that emotions are in general conscious and thus possible to remember and estimate (see Ekman and Davidson 1994, 411-430; Sloboda and O’Neill 2001, 71-98; Gabrielsson 1991). The present empirical investigation used self-reports and free recall to evaluate emotional affects of people’s daily favourite music. The aspects of music structures most obviously associated with emotion are melodic contour and modality (Gerardi and Gerken 1995; Gabrielsson and Linström 2001, 223). Even in 1930’s was established that ascending vs. descending melodic contours, major vs. minor modalities or fast vs. slow tempos evoke consistent emotional responses in adult listeners (Hevner1937). A musical ability test devised by Bentley (1966) is able to measure perceptual abilities directly related to musical structure, which include how well one can discriminate pitches of sounds, and remember and discriminate details in melodic contour, chords or rhythmic differences. Such discrimination abilities can be seen as a fundamental base for mastering the more complicated abstract hierarchy and syntactic rules of the musical memory structures. There are lot of electrophysiological research that can demonstrate cognitive perceptual sensory differences between musicians and non musicians (see Besson 1994; Tervaniemi 1993, 150; Tervaniemi, Iivonen, Karma, Alho and Näätänen; Hodges 1996, 240) but only a few research that has made an effort to display differences between emotional affections and cognitive perceptual sensory abilities (see Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez and Evans 1999; Koelsh 2005; Krumhansl 2002; Halpern 2001; Kilpatrick, Zald, Pardo and Cahill 2006). 2 Emotional intensities are linked to imagery abilities prior to auditory sound images based on thinking processes and activities in the autonomous nervous systems. The cognitive schema theory is used to describe the listener’s syntactic memory knowledge of the musical structures. Internal schemas serve to interpret music as well as to produce expectations and violations about the probable progressions of melodies. When a cued schema from declarative memory differs from the musical expectations an evaluation process with emotional affect follow (see Gawer & Mandler 1987; Meyer 1956). Hormones and neurotransmitters inhibits or facilitates the though or motor process associations and plays an important role in processing of emotional excitation (Guyton and Hall 2000, 679). The amygdala is supposed to be an important nucleus connecting emotional intensity to the long-term memory images and perceptual experiences (Le Doux 2002; Koelsh 2005). Ii is likely that emotional excitation of the hierargical schema memory system is associated with better memory performance of music in a perception task. Carl Seashore has proposed that the sense of pitch determines not only what we hear, but fundamentally what we remember, imagine, and think, and most important of all, it determines in large part what emotional reaction we have to the tone (Seashore 1967, 63). The mechanisms underlying musical experiences have not well studied. However, it is likely that emotional intensity is an automatic counterpart of the perceptual and discrimination mechanisms and thus a psychophysical factor in musicality (see Suoniemi 2008, 54-64). The present investigation is an attempt to discover weather there exist significant emotional intensity differences between high accurate and non accurate perception and memory abilities. Materials and Method Subjects A self-evaluation questionnaire was given to secondary school students (N=201) aged 15 to16 years, choir-singers (N=120) aged 30 to 55 and control-group (N=70) aged 30 to 50 years. Students were not involved in music-oriented school education. They formed a good random sample of young Finnish people who were mostly listened to techno-, rap- and rock/pop-music. Control-group’s people have not musical activities and they can not name C-major scale notes. They listened mostly to rock/pop, classical music and popular hits. Choir-singers listened to many different musical styles probably due to their vast choir repertoire. They listened for example classical music, rock/pop-music, popular hits, operamusic, folk-music and jazz-music. Test materials The two tests were demonstrated 1) Bentley’s (1966) musical ability test which measured perceptual and memory abilities such as: pitch discrimination ability, tonal perception and memory ability, chord perception (analysis) ability and rhythmic perception and memory ability, and 2) Self-reports of the emotional intensities, which were based on listening experiences of one’s daily favorite music. The selected primary emotions for the investigation can be seen in table 2. Procedure 3 The students were tested in classroom settings during a school hour. The students’ desks were far enough apart to prevent answers being copied from each other. Choir-singers were tested in their practicing places and control-group in the auditory of library Metso. The music ability test by Bentley involved listening tasks from a cassette tape with English language instructions lasting some 35 minutes. The Finnish language version of the instructions was displayed on the blackboard. Next came the questionnaire, which asked the people to make subjective intensity evaluations based on their daily listened favorite music scoring their answers from 1 to 5. Statistical Method The original test groups (Table 1) were redefined into three categorical test score groups based on success in the musical ability test by Bentley. The three categories were formed as follows: high accurate perception and memory ability (48-60 scores), medium accurate perception and memory ability (38-47 scores), and non accurate perception and memory ability (0-37 scores). In order to simplify the emotional grading system, subject’s emotional intensity values (original scale 1-5) were combined and named as follows: 1-2 = low emotional intensity, 3 = medium emotional intensity and 4-5 = high emotional intensity. SPSS for Windows 10.1 was used for the data analysis that involved the use of descriptive statistic, ONE-WAY ANOVA, MANOVA variance analysis and BIVARIATE correlation analysis. Results Choir-singers have better test scores in all test parts especially pitch discrimination test scores and tonal memory test scores were much higher and their standard deviation lower than by the students group or control group (Table 1 about here). For example there was only 2.3 test scores difference of the mean between students and control-group but the mean differed 10.9 between control-group and choir-singers that was significant (p<.001). Table 1. The descriptive statistics of the three test groups measured by Bentley’s musical ability test (Suoniemi 2008, 88) Test groups Students Choirsingers Controlgroup Total. Pitch discrimination test (max. 20) Tonal memory test (max. 10) Chord analysis test (max. 20) Total score (max. 60) 201 11,3 3,1 Rhythmic memory test (max. 10) 201 7,6 2,2 N Mean Std. Deviation 201 14,1 4,1 201 6,6 2,1 N Mean Std. Deviation 120 17,0 2,4 120 8,5 1,2 120 13,6 3,2 120 8,7 1,4 120 47,8 6,4 N Mean Std. Deviation 70 13,1 4,3 70 6,0 2,5 70 10,6 2,9 70 7,2 2,1 70 36,9 8,2 N Mean Std. Deviation 391 14,8 4,0 391 7,1 2,2 391 11,9 3,3 391 7,9 2,0 391 41,6 8,9 201 39,6 8,5 4 Musical ability test scores were recalculated and divided into three perception accuracy and memory categories based on the test score distributions. The percentage distribution categories were formed as follows: A. High accurate perception and memory ability (48 – 60 scores), 27.9% of people B. Medium accurate perception and memory ability (38 – 47 scores), 45.3% of people C. Low accurate perception and memory ability (1 – 37 scores), 26.9% of people The descriptive statistic and variance analysis demonstrates systematic sensitivity differences between musical test score categories (Table 2 about here) and primary emotional intensities. The intensity differences indicated that people with high accurate perception and memory abilities experienced five primary emotions significantly more strongly than people with non accurate perception and memory abilities. Table 2. Descriptive statistic and variance analysis (ANOVA) between self-reported primary emotional intensities and perception and memory category abilities (Suoniemi 2008, 92). Primary emotions Joy Sorrow Disgust Aggression Amazement Fear Interest Perception and memory category Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Low accurate (1–37) Medium accurate (38–47) High accurate (48–60) Emotional intensity (max. 5) Mean Std. N Deviation 4.01 1.10 100 4.32 0.9 154 4.71 0.6 93 3.14 1.3 100 3.68 1.2 154 3.97 1.2 93 2.43 1.36 100 2.62 1.40 154 2.61 1.30 93 2.30 1.28 100 2.50 1.26 154 2.65 1.28 93 2.11 1.07 100 2.33 1.03 154 2.51 1.20 93 1.93 1.11 100 2.05 1.02 154 2.18 1.07 93 3.25 1.30 100 3.65 1.02 154 3.91 1.10 93 ANOVA df F (p) 2 13.12 .001*** 2 11.33 .001*** 2 1.215 .298 2 2.97 .052** 2 3.100 .046* 2 1.191 .305 2 9.34 .001*** * = p < .05 ** = p < .01 *** = p < .001 Results display that disgust and fear do not have significant link between perceptual accuracy and memory abilities. But it seems likely that there are a strong cognitive link between emotional intensities such as joy (F(2, 391)=13,12, p<.001) sorrow (F(2, 391)=11,33, p<.001) interest (F(2, 391)=9,34, p<.001) and high and low perceptual accuracy and memory abilities. The same systematic intensity differences between accuracy abilities and emotional intensities of the three test groups can be seen in figures 1 and 2. Notice that emotional intensity lines of the different perception ability categories by the students, coir-singers and control group do not across and there were no interaction (all figures see Suoniemi 2008, 93). 5 Joy Sorrow 5,0 5,0 4,8 4,8 4,6 4,4 4,6 4,2 4,0 4,4 Perception category Emotional intensity 4,2 4,0 3,8 3,6 Students Choir-singers 3,8 Perception category 3,6 High accurate 48-60 Medium accurate 3,4 High accurate 48-60 3,2 Medium accurate 3,0 38 -47 38-47 2,8 Low accurate 1-37 Control group 2,6 Low accurate 1-37 Students Choir-singers Control group Testgoups Testgoups Fig 1. Intensity of joy Fig 2. Intensity of sorrow The subtest correlations between the four different perception accuracy and memory abilities and primary emotional intensities can be seen in table 3. Table 3. Bivariate correlations between the perception accuracy and memory abilities of the four subtest parts and emotional intensities (Suoniemi 2008, 95) Subject (N=391) Pitch discrimination and memory ability (max. 20 scores) r Emotional intensity Joy (1-5) Sorrow (1-5) Disgust (1-5) Aggression (1-5) Amazement (1-5) Fear (1-5) Interest (1-5) * = Significant at < .05 level ** = Significant at< .01level .213** .209** .096 .200** .111* .069 .193** Tonal perception and memory ability (max.10 scores) r .258** .185** .094 .087 .158** .140** .237** Chord discrimination ability (max. 20 scores) r .214** .171** .001 .025 .087 .009 .159** Rhythmic perception and memory ability (max.10 scores) r .223** .140** .073 .008 .050 .017 .184** The correlations are not high but they are likely to display which one of the four subtest parts of the perception and memory abilities were stronger related to the distinct emotional intensities. For example disgust and fear have only low correlations to all perception abilities. But emotions such as joy, sorrow and interest have significant correlation to all perception and memory abilities. Discussion Investigation suggests that people in general have extensive ability differences to perceive, discriminate and memorize the details of the musical test items. It is difficult to estimate how much genetic heritage or musical exercises declare the significant difference between 6 choir-singers group and control group. Surely both the genetic heritage and musical exercises are important factors. Results demonstrate also that five of the primary emotional intensities are significantly linked to cognitive perceptual abilities and that they differ systematically between people who have high accurate and low accurate perception and memory abilities. In addition results suggest that pitch, tonal, chord, and rhythmic perception accuracy abilities can have different emotional intensity affects. What is the inner mechanism of emotional affections and how they could be linked to cognitive musical perception abilities? A complicated answer could be that emotional intensities are linked to the hierarchy and quality of the acquired structural details of musical memory schemas and their working memory processes of the frontal and auditory sensory cortex (STG), which are linked to the psychophysical mechanism of the limbic and paralimbic cerebral system circuitry (Koelsch 2005; Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez and Evans 1999). A theory of the musical perception abilities linked to emotional values may contribute to neurohormonal and neurotransmitters substances that change the emotional intensity values based on violation of the auditory sensory expectancies. Perception accuracy can in general lead to perceived tension (Krumhansl 2002) of the musical schemas but there may be a more exact connection between sensory perception accuracy and emotional excitation. First, sensory perception accuracy and memory ability differences effect on dissimilar extend of expected violation (Besson 1994; Tervaniemi, Iivonen, Karma and Näätänen 1997) that may be a factor to inhibit or increase emotional excitation between recall from long-term memory associations and assimilations in the working memory processes. Emotional excitation means that neurotransmitter responses permits the slow or rapid generation of multiple images so that the associative process is richer and associations are made to the large variety of cues available (Damasio 1994, 163). The rapid generation is contributed by the brain steams endings that release the neurotransmitter substance called acetylcholine, which serves as an excitatory signal, lasting only a few milliseconds (Gyton and Hall 2000, 678). Second, high sensory perception accuracy can be an automatic booster of the rules and acquired syntaxes and thus the base on detailed cues and priming that coordinates assimilations of the musical schemas in the working memory process. Joy, sorrow and interest are quite plausible to express by acquired details of musical structures such as minor vs. major chords or fast vs. slow rhythms. The intensity of the primary emotions such as joy, sorrow and interest are surely based on reasoning and perception accuracy abilities because we like to listen to music which has a distinct style of the syntactic rules and perception accuracy is an important quality to acquire and memorize the details of one’s favorite music structures. Fear and disgust were not connected to perception accuracy abilities of musical structures. A hypotheses could be that fear and disgust do not need to cognitive perception accuracy abilities and reasoning processes of musical structures but are processed direct from medial geniculate to amygdala (see Le Doux 1996, 152). Results suggest that emotional intensities such as joy, sorrow and interest and perception accuracy abilities of the musical structures seem to link together and differ markedly even significantly between ordinary people. 7 References Bentley, A. (1966). Musical Ability in Children and its Measurement. George G. Harrap & Co. Ltd. Besson, M., Faita, F. & Requin, J. (1994). Brain waves associated with musical incongruities differ for musicians and non-musicians, Neuroscience Letters 168, pp. 101–105. Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P. & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimpic brain regions. Nature neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 382–387. Damasio, A. R. (1996). Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: Putman. Ekman, D. & Davidson, R, J. (1994) Affective Science: A Research Agenda. (eds. P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson) The Nature of Emotion. Fundamental Questions. New York: Oxfort University Press. Gabrielsson, A. (1990). The Study of music experience in music psychology. Proceedings of the international Seminar of Researchers and Lectures in the Psychology of Music. Poland: Radziejowice, pp. 85– 90. Guyton, A.C. & J.E. Hall. (2000). Textbook of Medical Physiology, W.B. Saunders Company. New York. Halpern, A, R. (2001). Cerepral substrates of musical imagery. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, pp. 179–192. Harrer, G. & Harrer, H. (1977). Music, Emotion and Autonomic Function. Teoksessa Music and The Brain (eds. M. Critchley & R. A. Henson) The Camelot Press Ltd, pp. 202–217. Hevner, K. (1937). The Affective Value of Pitch and Tempo in Music. American Journal of Psychology 49, pp. 621–630. Hodges, D. A. (1996). Handbook of Music Psychology. IMR Press San Antonio. Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A. (2001). Music and emotion: Theory and Research. Oxford University Press. Kilpatric,L.A.,Zald;D.H.,Pardo,J.V. & Cahill,L.F. Sex-related differences in amygdale functional connectivity during resting conditions. Neuroimage 30, pp. 452 – 461. Koelsch, S. (2005). Investigating Emotion with Music Neuroscientific Approaches. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1060, pp. 412–418. Krumhansl,C.L. (2002). Music: A Link Between Cognition and Emotion. Current Direction In Psychological Science. Blackwell Publishing Inc,Volume 11, 45-50. Le Doux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster Paperbacks. Le Doux, J. (2002). Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. Published by the Penquin Group. Meerum Terwogt, M. & van Grinsven, F. (1991): Musical expression of moodstates. Psychology of music, 19, pp. 99–109. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and Meaning in Music. The University of Chicago Press. Seashore, Carl E. (1967). Psychology of Music. New York: Dover Publications Inc. Sloboda, J. A. (1991). Music Structure and Emotional Response: Some Empirical Findings. The Psychology of Music, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 110–120. Sloboda, J. A. & O’Neill, S. A. (2001). Emotion in everyday listening to music. Teoksessa P. N. Juslin ja J. A. Sloboda (toim.) Music and Emotion: theory and research. Oxfort Unversity Press. Suoniemi, K. (2008). Havaintokyky, musikaalisuus ja musiikinkuuntelukokemukset. Dissertation. Tampere University Press. Tervaniemi, M., Iivonen, T., Karma, K., Alho, K. & Näätänen, R. (1997). The Musical Brain: brain waves reveal the neurophysiological basis of musicality in human subjects. Neuroscience letters 226, pp. 1–4. Terwogt, M. M. & Van Grinsven, F. (1991). Musical Expression of Moodstates. The Psychology of Music, 19, pp. 99–109.