Media Stereotyping Running Head: MEDIA STEREOTYPING Media Stereotyping: The Effect of Cultural Videos in College Classroom Kashmira Thakkar Illinois State University COM 472 – Seminar in Intercultural/Interethnic Communication Dr. John R. Baldwin May 10, 2005 1 Media Stereotyping 2 Abstract Utilizing a random sample of 121 participants, this particular research analyzes the undergraduate students’ perception of the East Indian Culture that is formed and influenced by media. A survey of stereotypical perceptions is used to inquire about views that individuals hold of the East Indian Culture. Furthermore, using a one-way ANOVA test a cross analysis is done between three groups to see if media has an impact on student perceptions of the East Indian Culture. The results reveal that media does have a significant influence in shaping one’s stereotypes. These findings validate the Cultivation theory and have implications for educators who choose films as a tool for teaching about a culture. What one chooses to watch and show is of importance because it leaves behind impressions that eventually lead to stereotypical views about a particular culture. Media Stereotyping 3 Media Stereotyping: The Effect of Cultural Videos in College Classroom Introduction It is said that on an average a person watches up to four hours of TV per day. Aside from the four hours of TV that one watches at home or at any other place, we are constantly surrounded by other forms of media such as newspapers, magazines, internet, etc. All forms of media bring across information and messages that we analyze and interpret, whether it is conscious or unconscious. We rarely stop to think about the impact that media have on us. Implicitly or explicitly media influence us and begin to shape our ideas, our perceptions, and our stereotypes. While we all know that stereotypes are not one hundred percent true, they do have a kernel of truth in them, which is the reason why they have been formed in the first place. In a fast moving, global world media has become the prime source for information exchange for many of us. Therefore, it is quiet common for individuals to learn about different aspects of life through TV, the internet, and other forms of media. Among these things are perceptions about other cultures. Forms of media such as TV shows and movies depict what other cultures are like, how they live, what they eat, how they communicate, and so on. The depiction of these aspects of culture in movies shapes our view of that particular culture. Keeping this in mind, this particular study analyzes undergraduate students’ perception of the East Indian Culture through a stereotype survey. The objective of the study is to not only find out what stereotypes students hold of the East Indian Culture, but to also inquire whether media affects these stereotypes. There are three groups involved in this study. The first group watched a Hollywood movie, second group watched a Bollywood (Indian film industry) movie, and the third group (control group) watched no movie. All three groups filled out a brief stereotype survey afterwards. Results of the study Media Stereotyping 4 were used to determine if perceptions of students differ depending on which movie clip they saw. As mentioned before, the objective is to see what kind of impact media has on one’s perception of the culture. Apart from this, our objective is to also analyze what stereotypes students hold in general about the East Indian Culture. Literature Review Stereotypes According to the Merriam-Webster’s dictionary (2002), a stereotype may be defined as an oversimplified or biased mental picture held to characterize the typical individual of a group. The Color of Words: An Encyclopedic Dictionary of Ethnic Bias in the United States defines stereotype as “a generalization of what people are like; an exaggerated image of their characteristics without regard to individual attributes” (Herbst, 2001, p. 212). Though the term is commonly applied to human beings, it is quite possible to stereotype objects as well (Nachbar & Lause, 1992). However, for the purpose of this research study we will treat stereotypes within the context of popular beliefs and images that people hold about other groups of people. Social Beliefs and Stereotypes According to Lippmann (1922) stereotypes are essentially “pictures in our heads.” (p. 9). These mental pictures are formed not only by one’s own personal experiences, but also through continuous social interaction. Later, Boulding (1956) further discussed the concept of “image” which we all keep in our thoughts about the world. According to Boulding (1956), very few of these images are based on personal experiences. Both Lippmann (1922) and Boulding (1956) realized that what we know about the world – the images and pictures in our heads are byproducts of our interaction with the people and the society as a whole. Consequently, these thoughts or “facts” which we hold about the world result from agreeing with people during the Media Stereotyping 5 course of social interactions that a particular “fact” is true. Therefore, the thoughts or “facts” are likely to represent the biases of these social interactions. Recent research in the field of social psychology points to the fact that stereotypical beliefs are implicit in the sense that they are based on ordinary biases rooted in the memory and happen at an unconscious level (Banaji & Bhaskar, 2000). According to Allport (1979), man has an inclination to prejudice. This inclination lies in his natural and normal tendency to form generalizations, concepts, and categories. Often these concepts represent an oversimplification of his world of experiences. Allport (1979) indicates that these concepts and categories sometimes even lack the “kernel of truth” and can be formed based on hearsay evidence, emotional projections and fantasies. Allport (1979) defined stereotype as an exaggerated belief associated with a category. Its function is to justify our conduct in relation to the category. He points out that some stereotypes are totally unsupported by facts; while other stereotypes develop from a sharpening and overgeneralization of facts which in few cases have a “kernel of truth” associated with them. Social Beliefs and Mass Media Different forms of mass communication include newspapers, magazines, television, popular cinema, music, literature, internet, textbooks, etc. According to Newspaper Association of America (NAA), in the year 2004, 1456 newspapers have been published in the morning and evening editions with a total circulation of 55,185,351 newspapers per day with a readership of 57% of men and 52% of the entire population (NAA, 2004). According to A.C. Nielsen Co. report (1998), the average American watches 3 hours and 46 minutes of TV each day. By age 65, the average American will have spent roughly 9 years watching TV. Almost 98% of American homes have at least one television. The report also indicates that 6 million videos and Media Stereotyping 3 million library items are checked out everyday. According to the National Association of Theater Owners (2004), there are a total of 36,652 movie screens, with a total of 1.53 billion admissions in the year 2004 alone. Few sociologists would refute the fact that this huge exposure to different forms of media could play a big role in creating the so called “images” (Boulding, 1956) and “pictures in our heads” (Lippmann, 1922) which result in the formation of stereotypes. Media Stereotyping In popular culture, people are often stereotyped around characteristics of age, gender, race, ethnicity, religion, vocation, and nationality (Nachbar & Lause, 1992). The following section reviews relevant research literature available on general media stereotyping around the characteristics of age and gender. General Media Stereotyping According to Rutherford (1994), women in television advertisements are depicted as feminine, showing their beauty, grace and sexual attractiveness, while men on the other hand were associated with outdoors, sports, cars, driving, and relaxing. Rutherford (1994) further emphasized the fact that women are often associated with the images of homemaker whereas roles within the home and work environments that demand expertise and authority are often associated with men. A content analysis of the New York Times coverage of 1995 Women’s NCAA Basketball tournament indicated that there were three times as many stories, photos and graphics on the men’s tournament when compared to the women’s tournament (Silverstein, 1996). The analysis revealed that the New York Times coverage perpetuated the “women are not aggressive” stereotype (Silverstein, 1996) and trivialized the women’s tournament. A study which investigated a random sample of 91 MTV music videos which were broadcasted in 1993, 6 Media Stereotyping 7 depicted men as more adventuresome, violent and aggressive when compared to women. More than nine out ten stereotypically male occupational roles were portrayed by males on MTV and all stereotypically female occupational roles were portrayed by females (Seidman, 1999). Comic strips, which are one of the most widely read forms of humor in American popular culture, appearing in the Washington Post between 1970s and 1990s were analyzed comparing humor and ageing. Findings indicated that older adults are stereotypically represented as unattractive, out-of-touch and not so intelligent people (Hanlon, Farnsworth, & Murray, 1997). For the purpose of this research study, we will be concentrating on the racial and ethnic stereotyping in media. The following sections review the relevant research literature available on media stereotyping of various ethnic minorities. Media Stereotyping of Ethnic Minorities Cultivation theory sometimes also referred to as cultivation hypothesis or cultivation analysis was an approach developed by George Gerbner (Chandler, 2000), to study whether and how television watching may influence viewers’ ideas of what the world is like. According to cultivation theorists television has long-term effects which are small, gradual, indirect but cumulative and significant. In essence cultivation theory states that continuous exposure to mass media namely television, creates and cultivates attitudes that are more consistent with the media version of reality than with the actual reality (Chandler, 2000). Television in this case acts as a socializing agent that educates viewers on a separate version of reality. Cultivation theorists argue that heavy viewers tend to have more homogenized or convergent opininons than light viewers. This cultivation effect is one of “level” or “homogenizing” opinion and is called “main streaming effect” (Chandler, 2000). According to Gerbner, this “main streaming effect” results in heavy viewers of violence on television coming Media Stereotyping 8 to believe that incidents of violence in everyday world are higher than light viewers with similar backgrounds. As a consequence, heavy viewers tend to believe that the “world is a nastier place” than do light viewers. This is a direct effect of misjudging the amount of violence in the society and is called the “mean world syndrome.” Gerbner, further points out that congruence between the viewers’ everyday experiences and the depictions on television will result in a double-dose effect, boosting the cultivation process. This is referred to as “resonance” (Chandler, 2000). Cultivation theory has often been criticized by researchers for being over-simplified, not considering several other forms of media which can contribute to cultivation and not dealing with several factors such as demographics, age, gender, ethnicity, and socio-economic background that can affect a person’s experience while watching television (Chandler, 2000). In spite of its limitations, cultivation theory continues to hold an important place in today’s society. The following section reviews the literature based on media stereotyping of different minorities which inherently “cultivate” cultural perceptions of ethnic minorities. African Americans Research literature reviewed indicates that popular media including television, newspapers, websites and films often portray African Americans in stereotypical representations. A content analysis of 1,699 television commercials (Coltrane & Messineo, 2000) aired on popular television programs with high ratings between 1992 and 1994 indicated that African American men are often stereotypically portrayed as hyper masculine or menacing and aggressive in nature. The findings also indicated that African American men are less likely to be at home and are less likely to be passive/emotional when compared to their white counterparts, which are common stereotypical beliefs held against African Americans (Coltrane & Messineo, 2000). Media Stereotyping 9 A content analysis of 813 television commercials aired on children’s television programming conducted by Bang and Reece (2003) also indicated that African Americans were less likely to be shown in home environments and more likely to be shown in food commercials when compared to Caucasians. Cultivation theory suggests that these kinds of portrayals can unduly result in stereotypical beliefs about African American children. Findings from a content analysis of fifty popular films in 1996 (Eschholz, Bufkin, & Long, 2002) indicate that African Americans are still underrepresented in leading roles in Hollywood and are in general portrayed stereotypically. African Americans are more likely to be portrayed in less serious and comic roles whereas their white counterparts are more likely to be portrayed in drama and action/adventure genres. Furthermore, the occupational prestige of African American actors was less than the occupational prestige of their white counterparts. Ever since the movie The Birth of a Nation was released, African American men have often been portrayed in Hollywood as violent in nature. This is indicated by the ingrained association of African American males with violence in movies such as Boyz N the Hood, Menace II Society, Fresh, Jason’s Lyric, Juice, and many others (Pierson, 1997). Another source (Cripps, 1978) in R. Miller’s edited book, also discusses the movie, The Birth of a Nation in which Griffith derives black images from southern mythology and common racial stereotypes that have long been circulating in the popular cinema. The images in this movie depict negative stereotypes of African Americans during the Civil War. According to Cripps (1978), African Americans understood the implications of Griffith’s images and as result the NAACP launched a campaign to boycott such films. To reverse the negative stereotypes created by the movie, the NAACP and Booker T. Washington sought to develop a black motion Media Stereotyping 10 picture company in which they scaled their efforts down to a movie titled, The Birth of a Race (Cripps, 1978). Furthermore, a source by Pines (1975) states that Blacks are exploited in movies, which is known as “blaxploitation.” According to Pines (1975), the trend of “blaxploitation” in films has been popular through the gangster idiom that started in the movie Shaft and carried on through others movies such as Cool Breeze, Slaughter, and Black Gunn. All of these movies are obsessively geared towards violence (Pines, 1975). Native Americans A study conducted by Merskin (2001) on four popular national products: Land O’ Lakes Butter, Sue Bee Honey, Big Chief [Monitor] Sugar, and Crazy Horse Malt Liquor indicates the use of Native American stereotypes in advertising. According to Vrasidas (1996), film industry in US has played a significant role in misrepresentation and creation of stereotyping the image of Native Americans. Vrasidas (1996) discussed “The Western” as a film genre and as a type of American mythology which perpetuated the myths and stereotypes of Native Americans. Another source by Walker (1998) in Rollins and O’Connor’s edited book states that a movie titled, The Last of the Mohicans is a prime example of how Native Americans are negatively portrayed in popular cinema. The central theme that is depicted in this particular movie is that of barbarism. According to Walker (1998), Hollywood’s depiction of Native Americans unfortunately contributed to the history of repeatedly telling the “wrong tale of The Last of the Mohicans” (p. 177). According to Hilger (1986), there is no doubt that the “wild and woolly westerns” have put in place negative images of Native Americans in the minds of the audience. Although such negative stereotypes exist, some films such as Little Big Man, attempt to be sympathetic to the Media Stereotyping 11 Native American characters. However, despite such sympathy that is portrayed, yet it still uses Native American characters as only political symbols to criticize contemporary values (Hilger, 1986). Latinos A content analysis of newspaper coverage on Hispanics in nine emerging communities (Paulin, 2004) indicates that Hispanics are still stereotypically portrayed as victims who are not in control of their circumstances. A story-line content analysis of six textbooks that are extensively used in grades 7-12 across US indicates that subtle and not-so-subtle adjectives and parenthetical comments were used to reinforce negative stereotypes about Latin Americans as lazy, passive, irresponsible, and somewhat paradoxically, lustful, animalistic and violent (Cruz, 1994). According to Pettit (1980), a large number of movies depicting Hispanics, portrays three major stereotypes: the dark lady, the bandito, and the clown. Furthermore, Pettit (1980) points out that most Mexican women do not hold key moral positions in popular cinema. In addition, a source by Williams (1985) in Keller’s edited book sheds light on the fact that Hispanic women’s bodies are often reduced to the status of sex objects. This kind of portrayal is for the delight of male subjects who are no less stereotyped than the “lazy Mexicans” who serve as foils in countless “Westerns to flatter the intelligence and energy of the Anglo cowboy” (Williams, 1985, p. 59). Furthermore, according to Williams (1985), films such as Let Katie Do It or Martyrs of the Alamo depict Hispanics as “lazy greasers,” banditos, and sex and dope addicts. Arabs In his book, Reel Bad Arabs: How Hollywood Vilifies Arabs, Shaheen (2001), discussed more than 900 films, which portrayed a distorted image of Arabs. Fifteen to twenty movies that Media Stereotyping 12 have been appearing on TV screen since the mid 1980’s projects Arabs as dehumanized. These movies are mostly recycled versions of: The Sheik (1921), The Mummy (1932), Cairo (1942), The Steel Lady (1953), Exodus (1960), The Black Stallion (1979), Protocol (1984), The Delta Force (1986), Ernest in the Army (1997), and Rules of Engagement (2000). Though, there have been few movies where Arabs have been projected as heroes, majority of the portrayals are negative stereotypes commonly held against Arabs. According to Shaheen (2001): “The moviemakers’ distorted lenses have shown Arabs as heartless, brutal, uncivilized, religious fanatics through common depictions of Arabs kidnapping or raping a fair maiden; expressing hatred against the Jews and Christians; and demonstrating a love for wealth and power” (p. 171). Shaheen (2001) also draws an interesting parallel between how negative stereotypes of Arabs are portrayed in movies with the propaganda of the images of Jews in Nazi-inspired movies such as Robert and Bertram (1939), Die Rothschilds Aktien von Waterloo (1940), Der Ewige Jude (1940), and Jud Süss (1940). According to Woll and Miller (1987), the Arab stereotype derives from cultural traditions dating back to the Middle Ages. Furthermore, movies regarding Arab stereotypes have depicted views of sinister figure of crime and corruption. International events, especially the continuing Arab-Israeli conflict and Middle Eastern political instability have repeatedly resulted in negative reporting about the Arab world (Woll & Miller, 1987). Asians Similar to other ethnic groups mentioned above, Asians also have been stereotyped and misrepresented in the media. According to Choy (1978) although superficially one might say that the media images of the typical Asian has undergone changes, however in actuality, “all we Media Stereotyping 13 see is a repackaging of the various stereotypes developed in the 1800s” (p. 145). For example, according to Sardar (2000), conventionally the Chinese have been portrayed as: untrustworthy, stupid, greedy, superstitious, lustful towards white women, irrationally attached to rather conservative notions of honor and family loyalty, and prone to narcotic addictions. However, some movies such as Broken Blossoms were seen as sympathetic portrayal of the Chinese. But the intended sympathy only reconfirmed the prejudice it seeks to expose (Sardar, 2000). Furthermore, movies such as Keys of the Kingdom (1944) and Inn of the Sixth Happiness (1958) showed China as cruel, despotic, wrapped in tradition, chaotic, a corrupt “ruin on the edge of the world,” and perennially resistant to change and to the west (Sardar, 2000). Additionally, a source by Kobayashi (1999) states that before the 1960s Asians were generally stereotyped in “devious, inscrutable, unassimilable, and in other overtly negative ways” (p. 3). Furthermore, before 1960s Asian women were generally portrayed as exotic and erotic (Kobayashi, 1999). All these images in films show misrepresentation of Asians. East Indians According to Rosser (1995), Hinduism in American textbooks is referred to as one of the world’s five greatest religions. However, Hindu beliefs and traditions are represented superstitiously. A survey conducted in University of Texas at Austin in the fall semester of 1995 indicated that, wars, disease, population, Gandhi, Mother Theresa, female infanticide, flooding, and starvation were the most emphasized topics in the study about India in American history textbooks. The survey also indicated that India is economically backward with emphasis on the superstitious and polytheistic nature of Hinduism as one of the primary reasons. A large number of students also indicated the portrayal of Indians as those eating monkey brains by many teachers, as depicted in the Spielberg film Indiana Jones and the Temple of Doom (1984). Media Stereotyping 14 According to Woll and Miller (1987), India became a backdrop in Hollywood films in the 1930’s. Often, these films presented the British view of the events and personalities of the Indian colony. This was clearly portrayed in the movie Gunga Din (1939), which depicted the supremacy of three British officers over thousands of evil Indians obsessed by warlike religion. More recent films by the British like Gandhi (1982), A Passage to India (1984), Granada Television’s The Jewel in the Crown, heed back to the American versions of life presented in the 1930’s. Later, the stereotyped and hackneyed representations of 1920’s and 1930’s were used by screen writers as evident in Spielberg’s film Indiana Jones and the Temple of Doom (1984). According to Woll and Miller (1987), this film borrowed its conception from the depiction of Indian life from the tribesmen of Gunga Din (1939). In addition, Bernardi (1996) discusses the birth of whiteness in D.W. Griffith’s narrative system followed in such films as The Birth of a Nation (1915) and Broken Blossoms (1919), which cast non-whites as metonymic and metaphoric threat to superiority of whiteness. These threats to whites from non-whites is also evident in films such as Leather Stockings (1909), A Mohawk’s Way (1910), and The Thread of Destiny (1910), in which Indians attack and brutalize white families (Bernardi, 1996). According to Bernardi (1996), Griffith’s stories are ultimately about white masculinity and their divine and natural right to judge and punish. The emphasis in Griffith’s stories on non-whites being inferior and brutal and the dominance on non-whites are required to maintain the “privilege” of white men. Furthermore, Hearn and Demille (1996) in Hagashi’s edited book, note the demonstration of white supremacy and white privilege which marked its birth in Griffith’s narrative system (Bernardi, 1996) in movies such as Indiana Jones and Temple of Doom (1984) that portrays people around the globe as exotic, mysterious, pagan, culturally backward, and racially inferior. Media Stereotyping 15 Similarly, Shome (1996), in his essay examines City of Joy (1992), a movie about India and notes the representation of white supremacy and white privilege. According to Shome (1996), the essential theme in this movie and many others such as Lawrence of Arabia (1962), Indiana Jones and the Temple of Doom (1984), or even more recent films such as Rambo III (1988), Indiana Jones and the Last Crusade (1989), The Medicine Man (1992), and On Deadly Ground (1994) is one of “White man’s burden.” This genre of films use the colonialistic narrative of the white man playing the savior of so called “third world” countries and the imperialist idea that white person has to arrive and save the humanity, incapable of governing themseleves from miserable conditions. According to Srivastava (2005), an irritating fact is of the western world’s portrayal of India in few defined parameters such as poverty, maharajas, snake charmers and elephants. Srivastava (2005) notes that though the west is talking of India as the emerging economic powerhouse, the creative outputs still seem to be caught in a time warp. According to Srivastava (2005), a recent Hollywood film Alexander (2003) came in particular criticism in India for its stereotypical treatment of Indians heeding back to Indiana Jones and the Temple of Doom (1984). Few economists would refute the fact that India is on the path of becoming an economic powerhouse for the last decade and a half. A recent report from Goldman Sachs (Nahan, 2004) indicates this fact and notes that even if the Indian economy continues to grow for the next 20 years at just the same pace as it has over the past decade, it will become the world’s third largest economy on the planet after US and China. Rodrigo de Rato, Managing Director of International Monetary Fund, in his remarks about the growing Indian economy (Rato, 2005) emphasizes the fact of dramatic declining poverty from 41% in 1992-93 to less than 29% percent in 2000. Media Stereotyping 16 According to Rato (2005), international investors are eagerly focusing to take part in a new India. Rato (2005) also notes about the booming IT (Information Technology) sector along with many other sectors including tourism, steel production, biotechnology and pharmaceuticals. Research literature reviewed indicates that India is changing. However, the representation and portrayal of Indians in Hollywood films continue to be based on stereotypical themes such as poverty, squalor, backwardness, paganism and mysteriousness. The purpose of this study is to identify some stereotypical beliefs held by Americans’ about East Indian Culture and also study the effect of popular cinema on Americans’ perceptions of East Indian Culture. Therefore, the researchers pose the following questions: RQ1: What are Americans’ perceptions/stereotypes of the East Indian Culture? RQ2: Does media impact Americans’ perceptions/stereotypes of the East Indian Culture? Method Participants This particular research study included a random sample of 121 participants. These participants were recruited by one of the principal investigators at a large Midwestern university. The participants were all undergraduate students in a COM 110 class at the university. For the purpose of this study no specific demographic information was necessary. Procedures The 121 participants were broken up into three groups. The first group in the study, which was 37 participants, was shown a movie clip about India titled, City of Joy, which is made and produced by Hollywood. The movie clip was approximately 7-8 minutes long, in which the participants mostly saw the poor, overpopulated, polluted, old slums of Calcutta (a city in India). In addition to this, participants also saw a poor Indian man struggling to find a job in this big Media Stereotyping 17 city. Following the movie, students were asked to fill out the stereotype survey. The second group in the study, which was 39 participants, was shown a movie clip about India titled, Hum Tum, which is made and produced by Bollywood (the Indian film industry). This movie clip was also approximately 7-8 minutes long, in which the participants saw a young, modernized girl and a guy (who are strangers for one another) saying goodbye to their family at the airport before leaving for their trip to America. Strangely enough the two strangers (the boy and the girl) end up sitting next to one anther on the airplane. Following the movie, participants were asked to fill out the same stereotype survey. The third group was the control group with 45 participants. Since they were the control group, they were not asked to see any movie clips, but to simply fill out the same stereotype survey based on their past experiences and knowledge of the culture. All subjects were given a consent form prior to their participation, which explained the purpose of the study and the fact that their participation is entirely voluntary. No demographic information was asked on the survey, since this was not relevant to the study. The surveys collected were entirely anonymous. One of the principal investigators involved in this research study is from India and is of the East Indian Culture and therefore, she herself did not conduct the surveys because it would create a social desirability bias. In order to eliminate any such biases, the investigators appointed a confederate who was trained to conduct the research study. The confederate was also given an introduction to read to the participants prior to showing the movie clips so that they would have some framework to understand the movie with. Instrument The stereotype survey that the participants were given included 20 adjectives that they had to asses on a Likert scale of 1 to 5 (strongly disagree to strongly agree). The survey was Media Stereotyping 18 constructed using Leonard & Locke’s (1993) model of stereotype adjectives/survey (see the attached Appendix C for the survey). Most of the stereotype adjectives in the survey are extracted from the above mentioned source; however a few were modified and changed in order to match the purpose of this particular research study. The survey instrument contained the following 20 adjectives: traditional, modern, submissive, dominant, individualistic, collectivistic, liberal, conservative, intelligent, knowledgeable, direct, indirect, noisy, calm, rich, poor, aggressive, passive, emotional, and practical. Data Collection/Analysis All the data was collected within two weeks of each other. The surveys were conducted on pieces of paper. The appointed confederate went into the different undergraduate COM 110 classes to conduct the surveys. Only the principal investigators and the confederate were able to see the surveys. After the data collection was completed, only the two principal investigators involved in the study had access to the data which was securely stored. Once the data was entered into SPSS software and analyzed, the surveys were destroyed in order to maintain anonymity. Results This particular research study employed a quantitative analysis technique. Since this study is intended to be a pilot study, significance of 0.10 or lower is acceptable. Its objective was to compare perceptions of students about the East Indian Culture. Since there were a total of three groups involved in the study, a One-way ANOVA test was utilized to analyze the data. The means and standard deviations of the three groups of students for each stereotype adjective who watched a Hollywood movie, a Bollywood movie or no movie are shown below in Table 1. Media Stereotyping 19 Table 1 Mean and Standard Deviations of Students Seeing (or Not Seeing) Films on India Stereotype Conditional Variable Adjectives No Movie Hollywood Bollywood Traditional 4.09 (0.83) a 3.73 (0.83) 3.62 (0.87) a Modern 2.60 (0.79) a 2.30 (0.79) b 3.31 (0.91) a b Submissive 3.09 (0.61) 2.97 (0.61) 3.05 (0.60) Dominant 2.75 (0.97) 3.03 (0.97) 2.92 (1.04) Individualistic 3.09 (1.06) 2.86 (1.06) 3.13 (1.11) Collectivistic 3.48 (0.67) 3.36 (0.67) 3.41 (0.76) Liberal 2.56 (0.98) 2.65 (0.98) 2.54 (0.72) Conservative 3.58 (0.96) 3.30 (0.96) 3.51 (0.74) Intelligent 3.95 (0.69) a 3.22 (0.69) a b 3.90 (0.75) b Knowledgeable 3.91 (0.80) a 3.38 (0.80) a b 3.92 (0.79) b Direct 3.33 (0.75) 3.46 (0.75) 3.41 (0.93) Indirect 2.79 (0.67) 2.70 (0.67) 2.87 (0.94) Noisy 2.93 (0.94) a 4.27 (0.94) a b 2.79 (0.73) b Calm 3.23 (0.99) a 2.22 (0.99) a b 3.36 (0.98) b Rich 2.64 (0.87) a 1.95 (0.87) a b 2.87 (0.74) b Poor 3.36 (0.84) a 3.97 (0.84) a b 3.03 (0.93) b Aggressive 2.58 (0.70) a 3.11 (0.70) a b 2.72 (0.77) b Passive 3.33 (0.68) a 2.86 (0.68) a 3.18 (0.75) Emotional 3.26 (0.69) 3.30 (0.69) 3.44 (0.62) Media Stereotyping 20 (Table 1 Continued) Practical 3.33 (0.57) 3.49 (0.57) a 3.10 (0.93) a NOTE: Standard deviations appear in parenthesis. Means are calculated on a 5-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree). Common superscripts on each line represent significant differences (p < 0.10). The ANOVA results indicate that there was a significant effect of media (watching a Hollywood movie, a Bollywood movie or no movie) on the following stereotype adjectives: traditional (F=3.28, p<0.10), modern (F=14.89, p<0.10), intelligent (F=11.08, p<0.10), knowledgeable, (F=5.76, p<0.10), noisy (F=33.99, p<0.10), calm (F=16.62, p<0.10), rich (F=15.02, p<0.10), poor (F=13.07, p<0.10), aggressive (F=5.11, p<0.10), passive (F=4.23, p<0.10), practical (F=2.64, p<0.10). See Appendix D for detailed ANOVA results. Post hoc Scheffe’s tests indicate that students who did not watch any movie perceived Indians to be more traditional than the students who watched a Bollywood movie. Students who watched Hum Tum, (the Bollywood movie) reported that Indians are more modern when compared to the students who watched the Hollywood movie or no movie at all. According to the post hoc tests, students who watched the Bollywood movie, Hum Tum or no movie perceived Indians to be more intelligent and knowledgeable than the students who watched the Hollywood movie. Students who watched City of Joy (the Hollywood movie) perceived Indians to be noisier and less calm when compared to students who did not watch any movie. Also students who watched City of Joy considered Indians to be poorer when compared to students who watched no movie or the Bollywood movie. On the other hand, students who watched Bollywood movie or no movie perceived that Indians are richer when compared to students who watched the Media Stereotyping 21 Hollywood movie. The post hoc tests also indicate that students who watched Hollywood movie considered Indians to be more aggressive than students who watched the Bollywood movie or no movie. Students who watched the Hollywood movie reported that Indians are less passive when compared to students who did not watch any movie. Furthermore, students who watched Hollywood movie perceived Indians to be more practical when compared to students who watched the Bollywood movie. Discussion Interpretations/Implications The study indicates that movie watching did have a significant effect on students’ perceptions of East Indians and their culture. Specifically, movie watching had an effect on the following stereotypical traits: traditional, modern, intelligent, knowledgeable, noisy, calm, rich, poor, aggressive, passive, and practical. These effects of movie watching on cultural perceptions can be related to cultivation theory, which in essence says that continuous media exposure cultivates attitudes which are more consistent with the media than with the actual reality. In accordance with the cultivation theory, students who watched the Bollywood movie Hum Tum perceived East Indians to be modern, intelligent, knowledgeable, and rich. On the other hand, students who watched the Hollywood movie, City of Joy perceived East Indians to be noisy, poor, and aggressive. The findings of this research study have implications for various individuals: educators, communication scholars, intercultural/interethnic trainers and consultants, movie makers (producers and directors), and the average public in general. Specifically instructors who use movies as a teaching tool or an activity to inform their students’ about other cultures will find value in the results of this particular study. Knowing if there are any benefits or harms in Media Stereotyping 22 showing movies made in Hollywood to teach about different cultures as opposed to showing a movie made in the actual culture itself, has great value. Overall implications of the findings are the fact that these results are informing us of how media can influence us and shape our perceptions and stereotypes of other cultures. The results also are implying that one should not solely rely on media as its only source of information, especially when they can create negative stereotypes about other cultures. Information gained through media should be analyzed for its validity, reliability, and bias otherwise such stereotypes result in ignorance about a particular culture. Strengths/Limitations Similar to most studies, this particular study also contains a few strengths and limitations that researchers admit to. One strength of this study is that it involves 121 participants, which is a large enough number to give statistical value to the study. Furthermore, with a large random sample such as this, the results of the study can be generalized. Another strength of the study is that in order to eliminate the social desirability bias, the principal investigators appointed a confederate to conduct the survey. Lastly, the researchers did a cross analysis and comparison of three groups in which they were able to not only understand what views are held about the East Indian Culture, but also see if media impacts those views. In addition to this, most cross cultural studies done thus far in relation to Asia have mostly included Japanese and Chinese, but this study attempts to look at a different group of Asians, specifically the East Indians. Lastly, another strength of the study is that it utilized a very short, brief one-page survey with only 20 adjectives, which eliminates the survey-taking fatigue. There are few limitations in this study as well. First obvious limitation is that it utilized undergraduate students as the participants of the study, which is not representative of the general Media Stereotyping 23 population. Second limitation of the study is that while some might consider a random sample of 121 to be sufficient, others might suggest using a larger sample of participants. Therefore, this could be viewed as both, a strength and a limitation of the study. In addition, this research being an exploratory study (pilot study), results with significance of 0.10 or lower was accepted. This is a limitation of the study as well. Suggestions for Future Research Researchers suggest that for future study, if given more time and resources, one should include a larger random sample of participants that is not limited to undergraduate students only, but one that is representative of the general population. Another suggestion of value for future research would be to conduct a longitudinal study/analysis that examines the stereotypes and perceptions of individuals over many years. Lastly, in the future with a larger sample, results with significance value of 0.05 (95%) should be accepted as compared to 0.10 (90%) significance value. Conclusion Very rarely we think about what we watch on TV or in movies. Very rarely we stop to wonder if what is being shown is even true. Very rarely we stop to analyze the impact that TV has on us. Very rarely we speculate how media might shape our perceptions and possibly build the stereotypes that we come to believe as the truth. This particular study sheds light on this phenomenon. Its objective is to make individuals understand that what we see and what we show has a significant impact on others and therefore, we should be selective in our choices. Media Stereotyping 24 References Allport, G. W. (1979). The nature of prejudice. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, Inc. Banaji, M. R., & Bhaskar, R. (2000). Implicit stereotypes and memory: The bounded rationality of social beliefs. In D. L. Schacter & E. Scarry (Eds.), Memory, brain, and belief (pp.139-175). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bang, H., & Reece, B. B. (2003). Minorities in children's television commercials: New, improved, and stereotyped. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 37(1), 42-67. Boulding, K. (1956). The image. New York: Ann Arbor. Bernardi, D. (1996). The voice of whiteness: D.W. Griffith’s biograph films (1908-1913). In D. Bernardi (Ed.), The birth of whiteness: Race and the emergence of U.S. cinema (pp. 103-128). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Chandler, D. (2000). Cultivation theory. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.aber.ac.uk/media/Documents/short/cultiv.html Choy, C. (1978). Images of Asian Americans in films and television. In R. Miller (Eds.), Ethnic images in Americans film and television. (pp. 145-155). Ephrata, Pennsylvania: Science Press. Coltrane, S., & Messineo, M. (2000). The perpetuation of subtle prejudice: Race and gender imagery in 1990s television advertising. Sex Roles, 42(5), 363-389. Cripps, T. (1978). Black stereotypes on film. In R. Miller (Ed.), Ethnic images in American film and television (pp. 5-10). Ephrata, Pennsylvania: Science Press. Cruz, B. (1994). Stereotypes of Latin Americans perpetuated in secondary school history textbooks. Latino Studies Journal, 1(1), 51-67. Media Stereotyping 25 Eschholz, S., Bufkin, J., & Long, J. (2002). Symbolic reality bites: Women and racial/ethnic minorities in modern film. Sociological Spectrum, 22, 299–334. Hanlon, H., Farnsworth, J., & Murray, J. (1997). Ageing in American comic strips: 19721992. Ageing and Society, 17, 293-304. Hearn, L., & Demille, C. B. (1996). Touring the orient with Lafcadio Hearn and Cecil B. Demille. In S. Hagashi (Ed.), Birth of whiteness: Race and the emergence of U.S. cinema (pp. 329-353). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Herbst, P. H. (2001). The color of words: An encyclopedic dictionary of ethnic bias in the United States. Yarmouth, Maine: Intercultural Press, Inc. Hilger, M. (1986). The American Indian in film. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow, Inc. Kobayashi, F. (1999). Model minority stereotype reconsidered. Unpublished manuscript, The University of Texas at Austin. Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Macmillan. Leonard, R., & Locke, D. C. (1993). Communication stereotypes: Is interracial communication possible? Journal of Black Studies, 23(3), 332-343. Merskin, D. (2001). Winnebagos, Cherokees, Apaches, and Dakotas: The persistence of stereotyping of American Indians in American advertising brands. Howard Journal of Communications, 12, 159-169. Nachbar, J., & Lause, K. (1992). Popular culture: An introductory text. Bowling Green, Oh: Bowling Green University Popular Press. Nahan, M. (2004). India: A new economic powerhouse without government red tape. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.onlineopinion.com.au/view.asp?article=2053 Media Stereotyping 26 National Association of Theater Owners. (2004). Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.natoonline.org/statistics.htm Newspaper Association of America. (2004). Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.naa.org/info/facts04/readership-demographics.html Nielsen, A. C. (1998). The ubiquity of modern TV and other facts to ponder in a mediated world. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.pcusa.org/ega/more/stats.htm Paulin, L. M. (2004). Fitting the pieces together: The puzzle of newspaper coverage of Hispanics in emerging immigrant communities. Paper presented at the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication in Toronto, Canada. Pettit, A. G. (1980). Images of the Mexican American in fiction and film. Texas A&M University Press. Pierson, E. (1997). Black on black crime: Hollywood's construction of the hood. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois. Pines, J. (1975). Blacks in films. London: The Chaucer Press. Rato, R. (2005). Prospering in a globalized economy. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.imf.org/external/np/speeches/2005/031705.htm Rosser, Y. C. (1995). Stereotypes in schooling: Negative pressures in the American educational system. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.infinityfoundation.com/mandala/s_es/s_es_rosse_school.htm Rutherford, P. (1994). The new icons? The art of television advertising. Buffalo, NY: University of Toronto Press. Sardar, Z. (2000, May 22). China syndrome stereotype. New Statesman, 129, 46-48. Media Stereotyping 27 Seidman, S. A. (1999). Revisiting sex-role stereotyping in MTV videos. International Journal of Instructional Media, 26(1), 11-22. Shaheen, J. G. (2001). Reel bad Arabs: How Hollywood vilifies Arabs. New York: Olive Branch Press. Shome, R. (1996). Race and popular cinema: The rhetorical strategies of whiteness in City of Joy. Communication Quarterly, 44(4), 502-518. Silverstein, L. (1996). Full court press? The New York Times’ coverage of the 1995 women’s NCAA basketball tournament. Paper presented at Annual Meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication in Anaheim, CA. Srivastava, S. (2005). The Oscar that has brought cheer in India. Retrieved April 29, 2005, from http://www.dayafterindia.com/apr105/national1.htm Vrasidas, C. (1996). Visionquest: Journeys toward visual literacy. In R. Griffin, J. Hunter, C. Schiffman, & W. Gibbs (Eds.), The white man's Indian: Stereotypes in film and beyond (pp. 63-70). Walker, J. (1998). Deconstructing an American myth. In P. Rollins & E. O'Connor (Eds.), Hollywood's Indians (pp. 170-186). Lexington, KY: The University Press of Kentucky. Williams, L. (1985). Type and stereotype: Chicano images in film. In G. Keller (Ed.), Chicano cinema: Research, reviews, and resources (pp. 59-63). New York: Bilingual Review/Press. Woll, A. L., & Miller, R. M. (1987). Ethnic and racial images in American film and television: Historical essays and bibliography. New York: Garland Publishing, Inc. Media Stereotyping 28 Appendix A: Consent Letter Forms Informed Consent Cover Letter (for movie and survey) You are being asked to participate in a research study that explores undergraduate students’ perceptions of the East Indian culture. Participants in this study will watch a short movie clip and then fill out a brief survey following it. This should take approximately 10-15 minutes to complete. Your participation is voluntary and there is no penalty for non-participation. Furthermore, you may withdraw from the study at any time without any penalty. There is no risk involved in participating in this study. Your responses will remain strictly anonymous. Only the investigators of this study will have access to this information. By completing and submitting this survey, you are giving us permission to use your answers in our research report. Please know that your participation is greatly appreciated. Finally, you must be at least 18 or older to participate in this study. If you have any questions about this study, please contact the following individuals at the phone numbers provided below: John R. Baldwin Dept. of Communication (309) 438-7969 jrbaldw@ilstu.edu Bruce Deason Research Ethics& Compliance (309) 438-8451 Kashmira Thakkar Dept. of Communication (309) 310-6939 kashmirat@gmail.com Please remove this page form the research survey and keep it for your records. Thank you for your assistance. Informed Consent Cover Letter (for survey only) You are being asked to participate in a research study that explores undergraduate students’ perceptions of the East Indian culture. Participants in this study will fill out a brief, one-page survey based on their past knowledge and experiences of the culture. This should take approximately 3-5 minutes to complete. Your participation is voluntary and there is no penalty for non-participation. Furthermore, you may withdraw from the study at any time without any penalty. There is no risk involved in participating in this study. Your responses will remain strictly anonymous. Only the investigators of this study will have access to this information. By completing and submitting this survey, you are giving us permission to use your answers in our research report. Please know that your participation is greatly appreciated. Finally, you must be at least 18 or older to participate in this study. If you have any questions about this study, please contact the following individuals at the phone numbers provided below: John R. Baldwin Dept. of Communication (309) 438-7969 jrbaldw@ilstu.edu Bruce Deason Research Ethics & Compliance (309) 438-8451 Kashmira Thakkar Dept. of Communication (309) 310-6939 kashmirat@gmail.com Please remove this page form the research survey and keep it for your records. Thank you for your assistance. Media Stereotyping 29 Appendix B: Introductions to the Movie Clips Directions: Read the following, respective introductions prior to showing the movie clip. Intro for City of Joy: You will watch a short movie clip. Following the movie clip, you will fill out a brief, one-page survey on your perceptions of the East Indian Culture. The movie clip you are about to watch comes from the movie, “City of Joy.” In this clip, you will see the main character, Hazzari, a poor Indian man who moves from a small village in India to a big city in India (Calcutta) with his family in search of a job. As he makes his way through the poor, overpopulated, polluted, and dirty slums of Calcutta, he ends up coming in contact with an American doctor who is visiting India at that time. Over the course of their encounter (which is not shown in this movie clip), the American doctor ends up being a big savior for Hazzari. Intro for Hum Tum: You will watch a short movie clip. Following the movie clip, you will fill out a brief, one-page survey on your perceptions of the East Indian Culture. The movie clip you are about to watch comes from the movie, “Hum Tum.” In this clip, you will see two main characters, Karan (the boy) and Rhea (the girl), who are smart, educated, good-looking, modernized, youngsters in India. Initially both are strangers to one another, who coincidentally happen to be at the New Delhi airport in India, saying goodbyes to their family as they both leave for their flight to New York to study abroad. Later, in the clip you will see that both of them (Karan and Rhea) end up sitting next to one another on the airplane and go from being strangers to acquaintances in which Karan tries to act like a “playboy” at the sight of a beautiful girl (Rhea) sitting next to him. Media Stereotyping 30 Appendix C: Research Study Survey East Indian Culture Survey Directions: On a scale of 1 to 5, circle the appropriate number for each of the following items about your perceptions of the East Indian culture. Strongly disagree Disagree Neither agree nor disagree Agree 1 2 3 4 1) Traditional 1 2 3 4 5 2) Modern 1 2 3 4 5 3) Submissive 1 2 3 4 5 4) Dominant 1 2 3 4 5 5) Individualistic 1 2 3 4 5 6) Collectivistic 1 2 3 4 5 7) Liberal 1 2 3 4 5 8) Conservative 1 2 3 4 5 9) Intelligent 1 2 3 4 5 10) Knowledgeable 1 2 3 4 5 11) Direct 1 2 3 4 5 12) Indirect 1 2 3 4 5 13) Noisy 1 2 3 4 5 14) Calm 1 2 3 4 5 15) Rich 1 2 3 4 5 16) Poor 1 2 3 4 5 17) Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 18) Passive 1 2 3 4 5 19) Emotional 1 2 3 4 5 20) Practical 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly agree 5 Media Stereotyping 31 Appendix D: ANOVA Results Table Anova Results Table Sum of Squares Traditional Modern Submissive Dominant Individualistic Collectivistic Liberal df Mean Square Between Groups 5.161 2 2.580 Within Groups 92.164 117 .788 Total 97.325 119 Between Groups 20.608 2 10.304 Within Groups 80.316 116 .692 Total 100.924 118 .292 2 .146 Within Groups 46.498 116 .401 Total 46.790 118 Between Groups 1.599 2 .800 Within Groups 95.992 117 .820 Total 97.592 119 Between Groups 1.554 2 .777 Within Groups 134.311 116 1.158 Total 135.866 118 .261 2 .130 Within Groups 66.218 114 .581 Total 66.479 116 .262 2 Between Groups Between Groups Between Groups .131 F Sig 3.276 .041 14.882 .000 .364 .696 .975 .380 .671 .513 .225 .799 .154 .857 Media Stereotyping 32 Conservative Intelligent Knowledgeable Direct Indirect Noisy Calm Within Groups 98.729 116 Total 98.992 118 Between Groups 1.709 2 .854 Within Groups 83.938 116 .724 Total 85.647 118 Between Groups 12.939 2 6.469 Within Groups 67.767 116 .584 Total 80.706 118 Between Groups 7.392 2 3.696 Within Groups 75.108 117 .642 Total 82.500 119 .370 2 .185 Within Groups 82.067 116 .707 Total 82.437 118 .543 2 .271 Within Groups 69.205 116 .597 Total 69.748 118 Between Groups 50.662 2 25.331 Within Groups 86.447 116 .745 Total 137.109 118 Between Groups 29.820 2 14.910 Within Groups 104.972 117 .897 Total 134.792 119 Between Groups Between Groups .851 1.181 .311 11.074 .000 5.757 .004 .262 .770 .455 .636 33.991 .000 16.618 .000 Media Stereotyping 33 Rich Poor Aggressive Passive Emotional Practical Between Groups 17.567 2 8.784 Within Groups 68.433 117 .585 Total 86.000 119 Between Groups 17.463 2 8.731 Within Groups 78.129 117 .668 Total 95.592 119 Between Groups 5.818 2 2.909 Within Groups 65.930 116 .568 Total 71.748 118 Between Groups 4.339 2 2.169 Within Groups 59.510 116 .513 Total 63.849 118 .713 2 .356 Within Groups 63.506 116 .547 Total 64.218 118 Between Groups 2.834 2 1.417 Within Groups 62.275 116 .537 Total 65.109 118 Between Groups 15.018 .000 13.075 .000 5.118 .007 4.229 .017 .651 .523 2.640 .076