BRAIN BASIS OF SOCIAL INTERACTION

advertisement
BRAIN BASIS OF SOCIAL INTERACTION
Riitta Hari
Brain Research Unit, Low Temperature Laboratory, and
Advanced Magnetic Imaging Centre
Helsinki University of Technology
hari@neuro.hut.fi
We human beings are social creatures and crave for interaction with each other; consequently
our most important “stimuli” are other humans. From the neuroscientific point of view, this
leads in extremely complex situations: the stimuli are highly variable, depending in an
unpredictable manner on the context, as well as on the emotional state and reactions of the
subjects themselves. Think, for example, the fleeting facial expressions and other non-salient
non-verbal cues that drastically affect the social interaction with your spouse!
Are the brain mechanisms of social cognition and interaction just combinations of processes
that are used for other types of behavior as well? Or could the human brain have some special
mechanisms for dealing with the continuously time-varying social interactions?
Some of these questions can now be addressed with new brain imaging methods that have
revolutionized human neuroscience during the last decade: We can study, totally
noninvasively—accurately in space and time—brain functions of behaving humans.
Information on brain circuitries subserving social interaction has thus started to emerge. For
example, amygdala lightens up in many conditions that require social evaluation and
judgment.
One emerging new view in brain science emphasizes shared neuronal representations of
action and perception. Importantly, such a similarity in perception–action coding within one
individual also implicates shared representations between several individuals, i.e. between the
self and the others. Humans thus do not share only the contents of their minds, but also of
their brains! Shared sensory representations allow people to “perceive” what other persons
feel (e.g. disgust).
Also of interest to brain basis of social interaction is the now recognized central role of motor
functions in cognition. A good example is the mirror-neuron system (MNS) which is activated
both when the subject views another person’s goal-directed motor acts and when he himself
performs similar movements. The human MNS comprises at least Broca’s region, the primary
motor cortex, the inferior parietal cortex, and the superior temporal sulcus (STS); these
structures are activated within less than 200 ms in a well-defined temporal order. The MNS
likely supports understanding other persons’ motor acts, as well as reading their intentions on
the basis of the observed movements, gestures, hand and body postures, etc. The MNS is also
a good candidate for the neuronal locus of disorders resulting in difficulties of smooth and
reciprocal social interaction. Interestingly, highly-functioning autistic (Asperger) subjects,
who have difficulties both in action understanding and in imitation, show delayed brain
activation in Broca’s area.
In the world of increasing specialization it is reassuring to see that the research on the brain
basis of social interaction converges information from several disciplines, such social
psychology, cognitive neuroscience, clinical medicine, and advanced brain imaging.
References:
Hari R and Nishitani N: From viewing of movement to imitation and understanding of other
persons’ acts: MEG studies of the human mirror-neuron system. In N Kanwisher and J
Duncan (Eds) Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Cognition. Attention and Performance XX.
Oxford University Press, Oxford 2004, pp 463–479.
Nishitani N, Avikainen S and Hari R: Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation
sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Ann Neurol 2004, 55: 558–562.
Download