Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 1 ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL ESM Table 1. Variety-quality correlations (p-value) by attribute and sex of option set. Attribute Female options Male options .12 .32 (.11) (.01) .07 .03 (.49) (.82) .05 .05 (.58) (.66) .03 .01 (.88) (.93) .06 .07 (.48) (.44) .06 .01 (.62) (.87) .03 -.07 (.76) (.35) Average correlation (s.d.) .06 (.57) .06 (.57) Average correlation (s.d.) excluding “age” .02 (.66) .05 (.65) Age Education Occupation Smoking Religion Height BMI Note: We performed the supplemental analyses shown in this table in order to determine whether the finding of a negative relationship between variety (variance) and number of proposals might be explained, instead, by an a priori relationship between variety and optionset quality; i.e., perhaps additional variety brings with it lower-quality options and it is for this reason that people make fewer proposals in events with more variety. For this argument to work, one has to assume some consensus on the part of choosers as to what constitutes “quality” for each attribute. We therefore had to do the same in our analysis. To that end, we used prior speed-dating findings to help us code the “quality” of the attribute levels. For example, with respect to height (a continuous attribute), “taller is better”. Thus, the average height of the option sets (male, female) was obtained for each of the 84 speed- Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 2 dating events. The event with the tallest average height was ranked 1, and the event with the shortest average height was ranked 84 (or N, as ties permitted). With respect to occupation (a categorical attribute), we distinguished amongst the three categories described in the primary analysis: professional/managerial occupations, skilled non-manual occupations, and all of the other occupations. For each event and by sex, we then computed the proportion of options in professional/managerial occupations vs. all the other occupations (p1), the proportion of skilled non-manual occupations vs. all other occupations (p2) and the proportion of other occupations vs. professional/managerial and skilled nonmanual occupations (p3=1-p1-p2). For the initial rankings, we treated each attribute level separately. That is, the event with the highest p1 was ranked 1 and the event with the lowest was ranked 84 (or N, as ties permitted). Based on this ranking we computed the variety-p1ranking correlation (r1). We repeated the same exercise for skilled non-manual occupations, for which the event with the highest p2 was ranked 1 and the event with the lowest p2 was ranked 84 (or N). (This is based on previous speed-dating research, which established that more professional and more skilled, non-manual is better).The same was repeated for “other occupations”, but this time the event with the lowest p3 was ranked 1 and the event with the highest p3 was ranked 84 (or N). We finally computed the overall variety-occupation correlation as the weighted average of r1, r2 and r3, where the weights are given by the sample proportions p1, p2 and p3, respectively. These two approaches to operationalizing quality were replicated for the remaining continuous and categorical attributes, respectively. The full set of event codings were as follows: Age: youngest= 1, oldest= N; Height: tallest=1, shortest=N; Occupation (3 categories; see text for explanation): most professional/skilled, non-manual =1, least professional/unskilled manual =N; Education: most educated=1, least educated = N; BMI: Women: lightest = 1, heaviest = N; Men (a non-linear preference, where normal is preferred to heaviest is preferred to lightest): normal weight = 1, lightest = N; Smoking: nonsmoker = 1; smoker = N; Religion: nonreligious = 1, religious = N. As readers will note, nearly all of the correlations are very weak and nonsignificant. The significant correlation for age in the case of male options likely had something to do with the design of speed-dating events: the company organises events according to age bands (e.g., “participants for this event should be men and women between the ages of 18 and 24”).Variety in age distribution within events appears to come from the attendance of participants who are at the older side of each age band. If we exclude age as an attribute, the average variety-quality correlations reduce to negligible values. Overall, the evidence suggests that there is not a prior association between variety and option quality across attributes and, as such, this does not seem to us a viable alternative explanation for the results. Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 3 ESM Table 2. Partial correlations (p-values) between chooser attribute variety (variance) and option attribute variety (variance), controlling for number of speed-daters attending the event. Attribute Age Partial correlation .29 (.04) Education .06 (.59) Occupation .07 (.70) Smoking .15 (.18) Religion .10 (.38) Height -.08 (.52) BMI .14 (.18) Average partial correlation (s.d.) .26 (.05) Average partial correlation (s.d.) excluding “age” .18 (.25) Note: We performed the supplemental analyses shown in this table in order to determine whether chooser variety correlated with option variety, i.e., whether events in which the women were diverse were also those in which the men were diverse. This might be an alternative explanation for the result that choosers made fewer proposals at events in which the array of options had greater variety. That is, if one assumes that choosers’ preferences are determined, at least in part, by their own attributes, then attribute variability amongst the choosers would be indicative of preference variability. And, further, if there is a correlation between chooser variability and option variability, then the reduced number of proposals made by choosers facing higher variability option sets could be explained by choosers in this context encountering fewer options that meet their more diverse preferences, the result of which is that, as a set, they make fewer offers . As can be seen above, age was the only attribute for which there was a reliable relationship between the variability in the chooser set and the variability in the option set: the more diverse the choosers were in age, the more diverse were the options in age. Again, the speed- Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 4 dating company organises events according to age bands (e.g., “participants for this event should be men and women between the ages of 18 and 24”). This is the most likely explanation for age being the only variable for which we observe a statistically reliable association between degree of variety amongst choosers and degree of variety amongst the to-be-chosen. Excluding this outlying attribute, there is little evidence to support the idea that an a priori relationship between chooser preferences and option variety explains our findings. Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 5 ESM Table 3. Regression results: proportion of choosers making 0 proposals. Predictor Constant coefficient p- effect size (s.e.) value (ηp2)1 alternative coefficient2 (s.e.) 1.08 .001 -- .44 (.04) Variety (Variance) .04 (.10) .001 .04 (.01) Number of options -.01 (.01) .23 .01 (.01) Sex of chooser set .26 -.00 .001 .10 .01 .95 .00 .00 .79 .00 .04 .004 (.02) .82 .00 (.02) Variety × Number of options × Chooser sex -.001 (.01) (.02) Number of options × Chooser sex .26 (.04) (.01) Variety × Chooser sex -.03 (.02) (.06) Variety × Number of options .03 .01 (.08) .37 .02 (.04) .04 (.02) Note: for all t-tests, degrees of freedom = 167. 1 Partial eta-squared indicates the proportion of variability attributable to a given predictor, over and above the other predictors. 2 This column reports coefficients (s.e.) from a regression with raw (unstandardized, uncentred) predictors. Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 6 ESM Table 4. Regression results: proportion of choosers selecting the top-ranked oppositesex speed dater. Predictor coefficient p-value (s.e.) Constant .50 .001 effect size (ηp2)1 alternative coefficient2 (s.e.) -- .54 (.01) Variety (Variance) -.03 (.14) .03 .03 (.01) Number of options .01 (.01) .02 .03 (.003) Sex of chooser set .18 .001 .001 .25 .02 .67 .00 .01 .37 .00 .001 (.01) .02 (.03) .42 .00 (.01) Variety × Number of options × Chooser sex .001 (.004) (.02) Number of options × Chooser sex .19 (.03) (.002) Variety × Chooser sex .01 (.003) (.02) Variety × Number of options -.03 .01 (.01) .91 .00 .001 (.01) Notes: for all t-tests, degrees of freedom = 167. Because the probability of choosers making 0 proposals was moderately correlated with the probability of choosers selecting the top-ranked opposite sex speed dater (r = -.35), it is conceivable that the reason variety appears to yield a Variety in mate choice / Lenton & Francesconi 7 negative effect on the likelihood of choosing the top-ranked opposite sex speed dater is because variety makes it more likely that choosers will choose no one at all. To substantiate the independence of these effects, we re-ran the above analysis, but where the sample was restricted only to those participants who made at least one proposal. That is, speed daters making 0 proposals were excluded. If the significant (negative) relationship between variety and the likelihood of choosing the top-ranked speed dater was due to the significant (positive) effect of variety on the likelihood of choosing no one at all, then in this new analysis we should have found that the relationship between variety and the dependent variable was nonsignificant. This is not, however, what the analysis showed: the effect of variety on the likelihood of choosing the top-ranked speed dater remained significantly negative (b = -.03, se = .014, p = .025, ηp2 = .04). The size of the effect even increased (albeit slightly). Thus, we are confident that ESM Tables 3 and 4 are reporting unique effects of variety on chooser behaviour. 1 Partial eta-squared indicates the proportion of variability attributable to a given predictor, over and above the other predictors. 2 This column reports coefficients (s.e.) from a regression with raw (unstandardized, uncentred) predictors.