Matt Johnson Lecture Notes ORNITHOLOGY (Humboldt State Univ. WILDLIFE 365) LECTURE 16 – VOCAL COMMUNICATION I. Introduction. Last lecture we talked about visual communication. While it is true birds are wellknown for spectacular plumage and visual displays, Bird vocalizations are nothing short of phenomenal. Indeed, birds have the greatest sound producing capabilities of all vertebrates. II. The physics of bird voice. A. Bird vocalizations are arbitrarily categorized into songs and calls. 1. Songs are typically long vocalizations with specific repeated patterns. They are most often performed, but not always, by males during the breeding season. Ex: Indigo Bunting (TAPE AND SLIDE). 2. Calls are typically short simple vocalizations, usually given by both sexes. There are many varieties of calls including contact calls, distress calls, flight calls, etc. TAPE 3. But this distinction is arbitrary; there is no clear distinction between songs and calls in their structure or function – when considering all birds, there is a gradient from call to song. Nonetheless, within a single species, it is often easy to distinguish between short non-breeding calls, and longer breeding songs. B. Physical attributes of vocalizations. 1. One major, and real, distinction in vocalizations exists in the acoustical structure of bird sounds: a. Whistled songs. These are pure or nearly pure sound waves. The higher the pitch, the higher the frequency of wave oscillation (measured in Hz, or cycles per second). They may or may not sound like true whistles. Ex: Blackpoll Warbler, Sage Grouse TAPE. Whistled songs lack what are called “harmonics,” which are multiple sound waves of different frequencies stacked on top of one another. b. That brings us to the second type of song -- Harmonic songs. Harmonic songs are comprised of several frequencies that are multiples of each other, which in musical terminology means they are in different octaves. Usually, one frequency is louder (higher amplitude) than the others – it is the dominant tone, the other frequencies are overtones. In the Red-winged Blackbird, the overtones give the call a nasally quality. TAPE In the Catharus thrushes, like the Swainson’s thrush, overtones are at odd multiples, giving the song an ethereal, flute-like quality. TAPE 2. What these differences mean to a listener. III. IV. a. The physical structure of a call affects the ease with which a listener, such as predator or a potential mate, can locate its source. b. Short notes with broad frequency ranges are easy to locate – good for contact calls. c. Long, high, thin calls are more difficult to locate – good for alarm calls. (long difficult to locate due to bouncing) d. Low frequency songs are best for long distance communication, and in high interference habitats such as forests – good for species like Great Horned Owls. TAPE e. In contrast, higher, buzz-like calls are effective for open habitats, such as grasslands (Ex: Grasshopper Sparrow TAPE). In fact, closely related birds in forest and field have very different song structures. The syrinx. A. All songs and calls come from the syrinx, formed at the junction of the two bronchi and the trachea. B. Air from the main anterior air sacs is forced thru the syrinx, where it vibrates tympaniform membranes. OVERHEAD C. The efficiency of sound production is extraordinary nearly 100% of air vibrates to make sound; only about 2% is so used in humans. D. Syringeal muscles control the details of the syrinx action during song production, changing the pressure provided by the interclavicular air sac, the tension of the membranes, and shape of entire structure. Birds also produce syllables by rapid contraction of the chest muscles. E. Range is truly phenomenal. The Brown-headed cowbird has largest range of any single bird. 700 to 11,000 Hz. TAPE F. Some birds, it has recently been learned, produce two independent sounds thru the syrinx – OVERHEAD and TAPE (save tape until end) Woodthrush. Avian vocabularies. A. Just like birds have repertoires of visual displays, so too do they have a repertoire of songs and calls used for various functions. B. Steller’s Jays – Contact Calls, Alarm Calls, etc. TAPE C. Many birds have rare calls used for very specific purposes: precopulatory calls, juvenile begging calls, etc. D. The typical “songs” of most songbirds have two primary functions: to (1) signal potential rivals that a site is occupied and/or defended, and (2) advertise to unmated females. 1. When territorial males are removed from their territories, they are usually replaced very quickly by other males “floating” in the background waiting for an opportunity. But when the removed male is replaced by a speaker broadcasting the species’ song, replacement takes significantly longer. 2. Once a male forms a pair bond with a female, he sings less. Take her away and he resumes singing. 3. Many birds vary their song for these two functions. Many warblers have what are called accented ending songs to ward off rival males TAPE….and serial song to attract a mate. TAPE All of this sounds so honest, so faithful. Oh, I’m sorry, I’m already mated, so I’ll sing a song V. VI. that says I am (just like a ring). Ah, but recent studies are documenting that in forest songbirds long thought to be mainly monogamous, both male and females are occasionally sneaking off for what are called EPCs: extrapair copulations. Males will often sneak away form their territory, then start singing serial songs! Recognizing species and individuals. Another primary function of bird song is for identification of species or individuals. A. Species ID. 1. Pairs of species that look very similar often have different songs: Alder vs. Willow Flycatchers TAPE. 2. Species that inhabit dense foliage may distinguish each other call’s by variation in phrasing. Gray Catbird vs. Brown Thrasher. TAPE. B. Individual recognition. 1. Subtle differences in pitch, syntax, and timing enable birds to distinguish individuals within a species. 2. This is well developed in the recognition of colonial nesting seabirds, which need to pick their calling offspring out of a “sea” of others. 3. Also well developed in secretive but highly territorial species, such as March Wrens and other songbirds inhabiting especially dense habitats. a. A series of territorial males quickly learn to recognize each others’ songs, so that they avoid wasting energy defending a territorial boundary with an individual with whom the boundary has already been established. b. Experiments have shown than Marsh Wrens respond much less strongly to taped playbacks of their neighbors than to “fresh meat.” But if their “neighbor” sings form the wrong side of their territory, they very strongly react. 4. Some birds sing distinctive duets to recognize and strengthen pair bonds. These are best developed in long-lived monogamous species, including many tropical birds. TAPE of chachalaca or King Rail. Song repertoires. Many species have large song repertoires – the question then is why? A. Function of large repertoires. Winter Wrens have varied song repertoires, each song lasting up to 8 seconds and comprised 50 notes delivered in various patterns form a pool of over 100 possible notes. TAPE So, why? 3 hypotheses: 1. Sexual selection: A large repertoire may indicate age and experience, which females may preferentially select. Indeed, studies have shown that female Canaries respond to large repertoires by building faster nests and laying more eggs. And in Great Tits, males with the largest repertoires produced the heaviest young. 2. A large repertoire may also thwart a neighbor's ability to track a bird’s movement, which could confer advantages for feeding or for obtaining EPCs. 3. Lastly, most birds’ response intensity to songs declines as that song is repeated. Thus, a large repertoire may help prevent declining interest – VII. which could maximize the efficacy of a variety of song functions. Indeed, the bird species that tend to sing the most continuously are the ones with the largest repertoires. Ex, Mockingbird. B. Vocal mimicry. 1. This brings us to vocal mimicry. The Mockingbirds is most famous for it here, but there are many other species that mimic. In fact 15-20% of birds in most regions practice vocal mimicry. 2. Some Mockingbirds know up to 150 songs, often performed surprisingly like their original composers. TAPE? 3. In fact, some Mockingbirds sing songs from distant bird species – whether these result from vagrants, or from the gradual passing of songs among Mockingbirds past the original composer’s range is unclear. 4. Migratory mimics can have international repertoires. Marsh Warblers in Europe sing African songs. 5. The leading hypothesis for vocal mimicry is that it is simply a function to easily increase vocal repertoire, the evolutionary significance of which we just went over. This idea is supported by the fact that Mockingbirds clearly sing toward their mates or potential mates, and NOT toward the species they are mimicking. Song Learning. A. The age old question – nature or nurture. The age-old answer: both. B. Brood parasites have totally innate songs – they are raised not by their own species so this makes perfect sense. C. Other birds appear to develop vocalizations primarily via learning, especially parrots. D. But most birds have a combination, with perhaps a heavy influence of inheritance. 1. The first time baby song sparrows hear their species’ song, their heart rate increases. They have a “template;” sounds that do not match it illicit no response. 2. Yet they cannot deliver “true” songs raised in captivity, they must hear it to form a model from which to develop their song. 3. Studies on White-crowned Sparrows have revealed that young birds pass thru 4 periods of song development. a. Critical learning period. – When they first hear their song and store it in memory. Lasts usually much less than a year. b. Silent period. Usually winter. No practice, no hearing. c. Subsong period. Analogous to infants’ babbling. Exploratory syllables, quiet songs for practice, not communication. Late winter early spring. d. Song crystallization. – finalization of the song in environment of conspecific singing. Usually a bird’s first spring and summer. E. Heavy learned components to songs create regional variation in song forms – dialects, just like human accents. White-crowned Sparrows living in Coastal California have been well studied in this regard. Areas as small as a few square kilometers can have their own dialects, and they can be persistent. The Berkeley dialect has persisted for decades. Lastly, we talk a lot about how bird songs are structured, how they evolved, and how they influence birds. But that’s not all they influence. They influence the human spirit as well. Perhaps not intentionally, but undoubtedly profoundly. I’ll close with a quote from Henry David Thoreau: SLIDE