Applied Linguistics Lecture 4: Theories of Second Language Acquisition 2 4.1 Current psychological theories: The cognitive/developmental perspective Cognitive/developmental psychologists hypothesize that language has no specific module (i.e. UG) in the brain; rather it is part of the general theories of learning. The cognitive/developmental perspective includes four models: information processing model, connectionism, the competition model, and second language applications. 4.1.1 Information processing ‘pay attention’ → automaticity In information-processing model, cognitive psychologists believe that learners at first ‘pay attention’ using cognitive resources to process information. Gradually, through experience and practice, information that was new becomes easier to process quickly. This frees the learners to pay attention to aspects of language that become automatic. ‘individual words’ → overall meaning Learners use their attention on processing attention of individual words. On the other hand, proficient language users give attention to the overall meaning where proficient speakers and listeners of language perform automatic responses in using or understanding the language without using the resources to process new information. ‘Practice’ = production + exposure + comprehension ‘Practice’ is not limited to the production of language. It is also the exposure and comprehension of language in which cognitive effort is made by the learner without being fully aware of that. ‘Declarative knowledge’ → ‘Procedural knowledge’ Language learning starts with the ‘declarative knowledge’ (or knowledge ‘that’), which becomes ‘procedural knowledge’ (or knowledge ‘how’) through practice. LANE 423 –2014/15 1 Restructuring ‘Restructuring’ is a qualitative changes in the learner’s knowledge where learners ‘put it all together’ incorporating too much of systematic aspect. (e.g. after saying the past tense ‘saw’, the learner may say ‘seed’ or ‘sawed’) ‘Transfer appropriate processing’ Information is best retrieved in situations that are similar to those in which they are acquired. This is because when we learn something our memories also record something about the context in which it was learned and even about the way we learned it (e.g. reading or hearing) 4.1.2 Connectionism The learner is exposed to thousands of linguistic features. After hearing the language features in specific situational contexts over and over again, the learner develops a stronger and stronger network of ‘connections’ in his mind between these elements that have frequently occurred in the learner’s input. For example, hearing phrases like ‘I say’ and ‘he says’, the learner will make connections between the subject and the verb. So, each subject pronoun activates the correct verb form. The presence of one feature activates the others in the same network. 4.1.3 The competition model The competition model takes into account the language form + language meaning + language use. Through exposure of thousands of language forms associated with particular meanings, learners come to understand how to use the ‘cues’ with which a language signals a specific function. The competition model requires the learners learn the relationship of the different cues appropriate to the language they are learning. The relationship between words may be signaled by word order, grammatical markers, and the animacy of the noun in the sentence. ▫ the SVO Word order in English signals the relationship between sentence components. Box push boy ‘A box pushes a boy’ is interpreted by a four-year-old child as ▫ grammatical markers (for example, the subject agreement on the verb, case marking on pronouns) in Italian and Spanish signal the relationship between sentence components. LANE 423 –2014/15 2 ▫ animacy of nouns signals the relationship of the elements in the sentence in Italian. Il giocattolo guard ail bambino The toy is looking at the boy ‘The boy is looking at the toy’ (MacWhinney 1997) 4.1.4 Second language applications: Interacting, noticing, and processing: There are four hypotheses that are inspired by the cognitive /developmental perspective 1. The interaction hypothesis ▫ Researches prove that speakers modify of their speech and their interaction patterns help learners participate in a conversation. ▫ Modified interaction makes language more comprehensible to learners (Krashen’s comprehensible input). ▫ The relationship between interactional modification and language acquisition is described by Long (1983) as follows: 1. Interactional modification makes input comprehensible 2. Comprehensible input promotes acquisition Therefore, 3. Interactional modification promotes acquisition ▫ Interactional modification during interaction leads to a better understanding than linguistic simplification or modification planned in advance. ▫ Examples of conversational modification are: 1. Comprehension checks by the native speaker (e.g. ‘the bus leaves at 6:30. Do you understand?’ 2. Clarification requests by the learner (e.g. ‘Could you repeat please?’) 3. Self-repetition or paraphrase by the native speaker (e.g. ‘She got lost on her way home from school. She was walking from school. She got lost’) ▫ Interactional modification yields to language development. When communication is difficult → the interlocutors ‘negotiate for meaning’ → the learners’ ‘comprehensible output hypothesis’ pushes them to produce output that is comprehensible to the interlocutor → learners’ language development. LANE 423 –2014/15 3 2. The noticing hypothesis ▫ The noticing hypothesis suggests that nothing is learned unless it has been noticed, noticing something that is heard or seen in the second language to fill a gap in the knowledge of the language. ▫ The question of whether the learner must be aware that he is ‘noticing’ is debatable? - According to information processing theories, anything that uses up our ‘mental processing space’ even if we are not aware of it on purpose can contribute to learning. 3. - According to the connectionist perspective, the likelihood of acquisition is best predicted by the frequency with which something is available for processing, not by the learner’s awareness of something in the input. Input processing ▫ In order to interpret the input, the learner should focus on the details of the language form. ▫ As discussed in the competition model, the learner is required to understand the form of the language they are learning ▫ For example, to interpret the following Spanish sentence La singue el señor She follows the man ‘The man follows her’ (Spanish) (Bill VanPatten, 2004) the learner should not rely on word order alone, he should pay attention to whether the pronoun is a subject or an object. ▫ Two reasons are responsible for the input misinterpretation: a) learners have limited processing capacity and cannot pay attention to form and meaning at the same time. b) they tend to focus priority to meaning than the form. LANE 423 –2014/15 4 4. Processability theory ▫ Ease of processing features of syntax and morphology affects the sequence of development. ▫ In other words, the easier the features are to process, the first to be developed. ▫ Features that are occurred at the beginning or end of a sentence are easier to process than those in the middle. ▫ Some features are ‘variational’ used by learners of different developmental stages. They are not affected by the same beginning-middle-end constraints ▫ According to Piennemann (1999,2003), ‘Learners do not simply transfer features from their first language at early stages of acquisition. Instead they have to develop a certain level of processing capacity in the second language before they can use their knowledge – in their first language’ Reading for this lecture: Lightbown & Spada (3th ed.): 38-46 LANE 423 –2014/15 5