CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION 1.1 Background to the study Parent-child relationship is critical for psychosocial development because of diverse ways that parents guide the life and experience of their offsprings. From big decisionssuch as what neighbourhood the family lives in, whether or not the child attends preschool, and so on- to small ones- such as how to respond to a child’s request for more playtime, more information, or more dessert, - parents’ child-rearing choices affect the emotional well-being, intellectual growth, and social competence of their children .Mead (1935) emphasized the significance of childhood experiences in the family. To her, children take the roles of their parents by pretending to be their mothers and fathers while they play. Oyideran (1981) also emphasized that modes of child rearing practices greatly influence the intellectual attitude or abilities of the students. In families, children receive their initial socialization and also develop their psychological traits, behavioural patterns and cognitive habits that will reflect on their academic performance in schools. In contrast, because of inappropriate modes of parenting styles and lack of appropriate school involvement of many homes in the study area, children’s academic performance is adversely affected. According to Epstein (2001) types of home climate either positively or negatively affects emotional functioning and academic performance of children. 1 It is widely recognized that if children are to realize their potential from schooling, they will need the full support of their parents. Attempts to enhance parental involvement in school or education should be in the hearts of governments, educators, and parents. It is anticipated that parents should play a role not only in the promotion of their own children’s achievements but more broadly in the school improvement and the democratization of school governance. According to Howell (2002), the European Commission, for example, holds that the degree of parental participation is a significant indicator of the quality of schooling. When parents participate in their children’s schooling, they may experience more academic and social success. Epstein (2001) suggests that parents who are informed and involved in their children’s school can positively influence their children’s attitude and academic performance. Parental awareness and interest in their children’s learning and school activities is important for children of school age, which may lead to positive behaviours. Importantly, Epstein’s (2001) research shows that parental involvement can have a positive impact on student’s academic work at all grade levels. Epstein’s (2001) research offers a comprehensive parent involvement model and perhaps the most frequently used and current model. Epstein’s typology suggests effective parent involvement model on; (1) parenting (2)communication (3) learning at home (4) decision-making (5) volunteering and (6) collaborating with the community. The researcher will not consider the first, fifth and the sixth because the first is the researcher’s independent variable (IV).The fifth is directly associated with the Parents 2 Teacher Association (eg.decision-making) whiles the sixth is also more associated with the community involvement. Maintaining a warm and supportive home, showing interest in children’s progress at school, helping with homework, discussing the value of a good education and possible career options staying in touch with teachers and the school staff are reflections of parents’ expectations and dreams for their children’s academic achievement which are the strongest factors influencing students’ school performance (Philips,1998). According to Stegelin (2001), parental involvement can influence students’ academic achievement regardless of the student’s age or subject. We believed we could succeed only with dedication and hard work but for many in this country, the Ghanaian dream is fading as the gap between the rich and poor continues to widen. Children are told that if they studied hard they could ``make great men or women out of themselves’’. While this is true, it seems as if the rules have changed and the stakes are higher. Numerous studies have been done to confirm the assumption that students do better when their parents are involved in their education (Philips, 1998). According to Philips (1998), students succeed in school when their parents are involved in their children’s education; the results would be higher grades in test, and examination scores as well as more self confidence, better attendance and regularly completed assignments, fewer placement in remedial classes, more positive attitudes and behaviour in school, higher graduation rates, and grater enrolment in secondary schools. 3 Henderson, (cited in Philips, 1998) found that parents are involved in school in four ways. The parents serve as teachers of their children at home and serve as volunteers and supporters at school. The next two include parents becoming advocates for their children and decision-makers in school in such areas as school policy and budget. This study focuses on three types of parents’ school involvement namely; learning at home, communication, and decision-making, and to determine which type of the parents’ school involvement yields higher academic achievement. Without research to indicate the effect of parental involvement in students’ academic performance, many parents may feel it is too late to become involved in their children’s lives. When we consider how complicated our children’s lives have become as they travel through their teen years, parents owe them, as responsible parents and educators, our involvement in their school lives. The kind of parenting that helps children develop a positive sense of self, to interact positively with others, and to be competent at school has no simple and universal answer because there is no guaranteed cause-and-effect relationship between how a parent rears a child and how a child turns out. Indeed, parents adopt many reflective styles ranging from quite strict to very permissive, from intensely involved to rather relaxed involvement. A child reared in one style may not markedly be different from a child reared in another (Baumrind, 1991) According to Baumrind (1967), home climate has been identified as one of these three types of parenting: authoritarian, authoritative, permissive type. The authoritarian parents have been found to rely more heavily on power- assertive and coercive discipline. 4 The parents' word is law, not to be questioned, and misconduct brings strict punishment. According to Spinetta & Rigler (1972), authoritarian parents are less warm, affectionate and sympathetic with their children. Maturity demands are high and parent-child communication is rather low. Authoritarian parents are found spanking their children frequently, enforcing rules rigidly and not explaining them, and show rage toward the children. Children of authoritarian parents are typically less skilled, have lower selfesteem and show high aggressiveness. According to McIntyre (1990), children of authoritarian parents are often unhappy, fearful, anxious about comparing themselves with others, fail to initiate activity, and have weak communication skills because it is only their physiological needs that are provided. Such children find it difficult to ask questions in class and are uncooperative in-group work, hardly express their thoughts in class hence have stagnant knowledge. According to Baumrind (1967), such children do less well in school. Children reared in authoritative homes are usually disciplined not because of the absence of punishment, but because of the authoritative parents’ show commitment to maintaining a reciprocally cooperative interaction with the children. Children in this home show higher self-esteem, outgoing, independent but at the same time are more likely to comply with parental requests and show more altruistic behaviour as well. They are also selfconfident and achievement-oriented. Such students express their thought in class; ask questions where they do not understand, and are normally group leaders. Hyman (1988) emphasized that children of authoritative parents get better grades in school. 5 Paulson (1994) also emphasized that permissive parents are tolerant and warm but exercise little authority. Such children usually share bed with their parents even during their early adolescence. (Maccoby &Martin, 1983). They are not properly monitored and therefore do what they want to learn to control their behaviours. The parenting style that will make such children have self-respect, independence and competence is lacking (i.e. unprovided esteem needs). Such children cannot do independent work in class and cannot think critically. Paulson (1994) emphasized that such children do slightly less well in school. ‘’There is no way in which parents can evade having a determining effect upon their children’s personality, character, and competence” (Baumrind, 1978). The functions of parenting and parents’ involvement greatly influence how children develop (Arendell, 1997). Therefore, as a trainee in counselling, the researcher is concerned with effective parenting styles and parental involvement by most parents in upbringing their children. 1.2 Statement of the problem The researcher has observed certain features of the family's culture in the study area. First, parents differed in their warmth, or nurturance toward their offsprings. Secondly, parents varied in their strategies to control their children’s actions through explanation, persuasion, and /or punishment. Third, parents also differed in the quality of communication, decision making, and in helping their children to learn at home. These differences in the family culture affect the children's academic performance (Steinberg & Dornbusch, 1991). 6 The researcher has observed that parents spend a lot of time during P.T.A meetings discussing the poor academic performance of their children and blame the teachers. The teachers on the other hand put the blame squarely on the parents. Considering the parents viewpoint regarding the poor academic performance, the question is posed, how many of these parents are involved in their children’s schooling? What child-rearing strategies do they use and how effective are these strategies? Despite the government’s effort to improve the infrastructure and to provide free school uniforms, the researcher has observed that the parent- teacher communication that can motivate students to learn; strategies such as monitoring students to learn at home, and creating a book-friendly environment to contribute to the development of the children’s literacy skills and positive attitudes toward learning seem to be lacking. The study therefore is necessitated by the desire to investigate the effect of parenting styles (i.e. authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive) in conjunction with parental involvement (communication, learning at home and decision-making) on student’s academic performance. 1.3 Purpose of the study The purpose of this study is twofold. The main purpose was to investigate the effects of parenting styles and parental involvement on the students’ academic performance. The second was to investigate whether differences in parenting styles and parental involvement depend on the demographic variables of the students’ parents of educational background, marital status, and sex. 7 1.4. Research hypotheses The following were the research hypothesis for the study. i. There will be statistically significance difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ parenting styles ii. There will be statistically significance difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ school involvement iii. There will be a statistically significant interaction effect between parenting styles and school involvement on academic performance. iv. There will be a significant contribution of parenting styles and parental involvement on the students academic performance v. There will be statistically significant differences in parenting styles and selected parents demographic variables of educational background, marital status, and sex. vi. There will be statistically significant differences in parents’ school involvement and selected parents’ demographic variables of educational background, marital status, and sex 1.5. Assumptions of the study The following were the basic assumptions of the study. It is assumed that the: i. Study area will be accessible for the researcher to carry out his work. ii. Sample of the study is a true representative of the population of the study iii. Instrument will measure exactly what it is purported to measure iv. Respondents will be able to appropriately give answers to the items in the questionnaire. 8 1.6. Significance of the study The parents-students relationship has a significant impact on the students’ experiences during their course of study. On the other hand, poor quality of the relationship between students and their parents can cause the students to terminate their schooling prematurely. This study provides a framework for further research into factors that promote or impede students’ academic performance. One of the major priorities of parents is to identify those determinants on their children academic performance. It is only after specifying the determinants of academic performance, that effort for improvement would be meaningful. Moreover, the study can provide the necessary basis for a systematic method of parental evaluation. Evaluation of relationships in terms of students academic performance and help determine the most effective type of parenting and parents school involvement that help improve the quality of students education, and potentially decrease the number of students dropping out and leaving the school unfinished. Accordingly, this study presents an effort to assist teachers to understand some possible causes of students’ poor academic performance and device ways of assisting them. In addition, the study will aid parents to realize the influence of their school involvement and parenting style as a contextual element in shaping their children academic outcomes. Finally, the study will help students to develop their potential as they discover the role of their parents’ school involvement and parenting styles in adapting to diverse learning situations. It is therefore hoped that this investigation will serve as a guidepost to parents and other stakeholders of education to be conscious that both parenting styles and parents school involvement play an important role on the students’ academic performance. As a result, 9 Ghana Education Service and the stakeholders will be encourage to organize public lectures on potentials of parenting style and parents school involvement on academic performance of students. 1.7 Delimitation of the Study The study was limited to both third year students and their parents’ viewpoints on the child-parent relationship. The reason of this delimitation is that according to Baumrind (1966), parents and children have different perceptions of their relationship hence the mean of their viewpoint is the researcher’s interest. Another delimitation is that students within one municipality were recruited to participate in the study. Lastly, the parents’ assessment on the parenting styles and parental involvement were gathered based on the perspective of the students. 1.8: Definition of Terms and Abbreviation and Explanation of Acronyms The following terms and abbreviations used in the study are as follows: Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ)-A research instrument designed by Buri (1991) which is used to assess the three types of parenting styles: Authoritative Style, Authoritarian Style, and Permissive Style. Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ)-A research instrument designed by the researcher to assess the parents’ school involvements: Learning at home, Communication, Decision-Making Responsiveness/Acceptance- this is one of two constructs that functions in establishing typology of parenting styles. Responsiveness is synonymous with supportiveness, and 10 describes the parent’s process of using sensitivity to the child’s desires and needs when communicating with their children while simultaneously passing on a certain level of self-assertion and self-regulation traits. Demandingness/Control – This is the second constructs that functions if establishing typology of parenting style. Demandingness is synonymous with control, and is characterized by the range of parental supervision, disciplinary methods and inclination to confront when the child disobeys, which will ultimately integrate the child into the family system. Statistical Package of Social Sciences (SPSS) - A computer progamme usually employed by social scientists for statistical analysis. Senior High School (S.H.S) – A 4- year post Junior High School education that leads to tertiary education. The Junior High School is a 3 – year post primary education in Ghana that leads to Senior High School. Parent Teacher Association (PTA) - A formal organization composed of parents, teachers, and staff that are intended to facilitate parental participation in a school. 1.9: Organization of the Study The study comprised five chapters. The first chapter is the introduction that deals with background to the study, statement of the problem, purpose of the study, study hypotheses, significance of the study and delimitations. It also dealt with terms or acronyms which may not be familiar to readers. 11 Chapter two touches on the review of related literature and framework for the study while chapter three discussed the methodology for data collection. This covers; research design, rationale for adopting quantitative approach, population of the study, study area, instrumentation, pilot study, reliability and validity, procedure for data analysis, and data analysis. Chapter four covers the presentation and analysis of data collected as well as discussion of the results obtained. Finally chapter five was devoted to summary of findings, conclusions, limitations, implications and limitations for theoretical, educational and counseling for future research. 12 CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE 2.1. Introduction Whether it is within a high depth level of discourse among researchers in an academic institution, or a discussion between school educators, the topic of parental involvement and parenting style is likely to surface when discussing problems in students' academic achievements. This chapter will focus on the literature as it pertains specifically to parenting styles (i.e. authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive) and parental involvement (learning at home, communication, and decision- making,). The literature review will focus on the impact these variables have on the academic performance (dependent variables). 2.2. Theoretical Framework The theory and models that are used as the background of the study included; Baumrind’s (1966) model of parenting style Epstein’s (2001) typology of parental involvement 2.2.1 Baumrind’s (1966) Model of Parenting Styles The most influential proposal on child-rearing has come from Baumrind (1966) model of parenting style. Her proposal came about when she looked at combinations of the following dimensions: 13 1. Warmth or nurturance 2. Level of parents’ expectations, which she describes in terms of “maturity demands” 3. The clarity and consistency or rules and 4. Communication between parent and child. Baumrind (1966) saw the following three specific combinations of the characteristics: The permissive style is high in nurturance but low in maturity demands, control, and communication. The authoritarian style is high in control and maturity demands but low in nurturance and communication. The authoritative style is highest in all of the variable said Maccoby and Martin (1983) extended Baumrind’s category system, proposing a model on styles of child-rearing. They emphasized two dimensions: the degree of demand or control and the amount of acceptance/rejection or responsiveness. The intersection of these two dimensions created four types, three of which corresponded fairly closely to Baumrind’s authoritarian, authoritative and permissive types. Maccoby and Martin’s fourth type, the uninvolved neglecting style, was not identified by Baumrind. The figure below illustrates the Maccoby and Martin’s (1983) variation. 14 Level of control or demand Level of acceptance / responsiveness High High Low Low Authoritative reciprocal Authoritarian assertive power Permissive indulgent Neglecting uninvolved Figure 2.1 Maccoby and Martin expanded on Baumrind's categories in this twodimensional typology Based on the view of Baumrind (1966) and Maccoby and Martin (1983) the researcher can deduce that children’s academic performance can be improved or weakened by the type of home climate. A child growing up in a home that the parents are characterized by high control and maturity demands but low in nurturance and communication (Baumrind, 1966) and for that matter low level of acceptance/responsiveness and high level of control or demand, will make the child less skilled and have lower self-esteem, high aggressiveness or other indications of being out of control. According to Baumrind (1991) children with these attitudes do less in school because they are nonconformist to school rules and regulations and their frequently being punished makes the teachers hate them and also makes them unaccepted by the students and therefore they may experience lack of teachers monitoring. And this would invariably influence their academic performance. Parents who are high in control, warmth, communication, nurturance and 15 maturity demands set clear limits and of the same time respond to their children needs. (Maccoby and Martin, 1983). Authoritative parents are quite willing to discipline their child appropriately if the child misbehaves. These parents are less likely to use physical punishment but rather use “time out” or other mild punishment, but it is important to understand that these parents are not wishy-washy. Maccoby and Martin (1983) further emphasized that such children are cheerful, outgoing and eager. These behaviour of asking questions in class, participate fully in group activities and consult teachers and their mates on where they did not understand. To Maccoby and Martin (1983), such children usually get better grades in elementary schools, high school, or colleges. Children who are from high level of acceptance/responsiveness but with the parents who are low level of control or demand usually perform below expectation (Maccoby and Martin, 1983). Permissive parents do not restrict their children’s activities, decisions and desires. The children are allowed to do what they want to do and are not expected to obey rules and regulations of behaviour. They lack control over matters or are not serious in their studies and therefore achieve less in school 2.3. The concept of parenting style Practically every undergraduate and graduate textbook on child development will mention three major styles of parenting including authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive. The researcher Baumrind (1966) who defined each construct according to observable behaviours by parents and children first proposed these constructs. In her early work, Baumrind (1968) presented children from authoritative parents as more selfreliant and self-controlled who had parents that were controlling and demanding, but had 16 a degree of reciprocity regarding communication with child. These parents used firm and consistent control communicating with the child in a warm, reciprocal, and verbal “give and take” style. Authoritarian parents use a firm style too; however they are characterized as “strict” expecting absolute obedience to parental authority and reacting punitively to individuality that is expressed by child. Additionally, authoritarian parents are characterized by lack of warmth and detachment. Finally, permissive parents, the opposite of authoritarian parenting, are characterized by frequent expressions of warmth and affection by parents but discipline infrequently and do not enforce rules of household. Therefore, children of these households are more likely to regulate their own behaviour independently. How is a parenting typology determined? 2.4. Dimensions of parenting style Baumrind (1991) explains that two researchers, Schaefer, and later Becker, analyzed data from studies examining the child-parent relationships noting that “two orthogonal factors” emerged: demandingness and responsiveness. Parenting styles are then described in relation to these two terms. For ease of reading, each dimension is divided into sublevels, each of which can be measured and yield a total score that would designate a particular parenting style. The sublevels of responsiveness (synonymous with supportiveness), consist of warmth reciprocity, and attachment; and the sublevels of demandingness (synonymous with control), consist of monitoring and discipline. 17 Warmth refers to the parent’s emotional expression of love; however, a warm and loving parent may well be a firm disciplinarian too. Reciprocity is described as regarding the child’s wishes and feelings with sincere value. The final sublevel of responsiveness is attachment, which is the extent of emotional connection a child has with his/her parent(s). Demandingness embodies how well and efficient the parent supervises the child, and the sublevels of monitoring and discipline are likely self-explanatory. Regardless, monitoring involves how parents approach establishing rules of the household, then enforcing those rules and how well they supervise the child. Are all parents’ coercive, giving threats or promises without reasons, do the parents use firm direct confrontation, or is the confrontation in the form of a friendly conversation or demeaning manner? As noted earlier, discipline was a sublevel, which refers to the type of discipline the parents implement for infractions of misbehaviour. Is the discipline inconsistent, providing tenuous support to child, it is in a punitive form that is hostile, or is it consistent and fair? Loading each dimension into the parenting typologies yields a particular parenting style. Therefore, if each sublevel within responsiveness was measured and the results yield high in each dimension of warmth, reciprocity, and attachment and a high score was yielded across each dimension in demandingness, then that parent would have high responsiveness and high demandingness-characteristics of the parenting typology authoritative parenting. Briefly, authoritative parenting, as noted would consist of high responsiveness and high demandingness, authoritarian would consist of low responsiveness and high demandingness, and permissive or indulgent parenting type would be responsive but low demanding (Jackson et al., 1998; Steinberg et al., 1995; 18 Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, & Dornbusch, 1991). Extending Baumrind’s (1966) work, Maccoby and Martin (1983) found through analysis of demandingness and responsiveness another parenting style emerged which Baumrind (1991) termed it as neglectful. 2.4.1. Authoritative parenting Authoritative parents involve a demonstration of high levels of demandingness coupled with high levels of responsiveness. Authoritative parents expect conformity to parental standards from their children while at the same time encourage autonomy and self-will. Authoritative parents encourage discussion of their expectation and provide their children with the rationale behind them. They are demanding of their children in that they guide firmly and articulate their expectations clearly, yet are also responsive in that they provide encouragement, love, and understanding. Authoritative parents are accepting of their children’s qualities while communicating expectations for future standards of conduct. Unlike authoritarian parents, authoritative parents do not hide their imperfections, nor do they expect infallibility in their children viewed. Mistakes are viewed as learning experiences, rather than as punishable offences. Unlike any other pattern, authoritative parenting upbringing generates competence and deterred problem behaviour (Baumrind, 1971, 1991). Glasgow, Dornbusch, Troyer, Steinberg, and Ritter (1997) points out, in comparison to the other parenting styles that, authoritative parenting is the most successful in fostering personal and social responsibility in adolescents, without limiting their emerging autonomy. Jackson et al (1998) indicated that authoritative parenting was reflective of better conflict resolution, 19 and “significantly lower odds of reporting substance use and violence-related behaviours than peers whose parents were defined as permissive”. 2.4.1.1 Authoritative style and child qualities Children of authoritative parents were found to be purposeful, cooperative, assertive, selfreliant, and highly achievement oriented (Baumrind, 1971, (1991). Other qualities of children of authoritative homes are: Lively and happy disposition Self-confident about ability to master tasks Well developed emotion regulation Developed social skills Less rigid about gender-typed traits (especially: sensitivity in boys and independence in girls). 2.4.2 Authoritarian parenting The authoritarian parenting centre on control. Authoritarian parents tend to raise obedient youths who do not question authority (Baumrind, 1991; Jackson, et al., 1998). Authoritarian parents exhibit high demandingness and low responsiveness toward their children. They attempt to control their children in accordance to a set of exterior o standards that are typically in adherence to the views of some higher authority. In order words, authoritarian parents may look to their religious leaders for such standards, rather than creating their own. They value unquestioned obedience, and commonly use punishment for noncompliance or any type of demonstration of self- will from the child. 20 They value work, respect for authority, and preservation of tradition, attempt to instill such values in their children. Authoritarian parents do not value discussion with their children concerning parental expectations, but instead believe children should accept adult authority without question. According to Jackson et al (1998), children of authoritarian parents tend to have both low self-esteem and less social competence in school. Again, youths from authoritarian parenting are more likely to report positive school performance compared to permissive but not compared to authoritative parenting (Dornbusch et al,. 1987; Jackson et al,. 1998). However, Lamborn et al. (1991) point out that because youths of authoritarian parents are raised stricter “they score well on measures of obedience and conformity and also do well in school”. Lamborn et al. (1991) continue, suggesting that although they perform well in school, they “have paid a price where self-confidence is concerned” regarding the potential of their academic abilities. 2.4.2.1 Authoritarian style and child qualities Children reared under the authoritarian parenting pattern are found to be reasonably achievement oriented, yet hostile and uncooperative (boys), or dependent and submissive (girls) (Baumrind, 1991). Other qualities of children reared authoritarian home have the following qualities: Anxious, withdrawn, and unhappy disposition Poor reactions to frustration (girls are particularly likely to give up and boys especially hostile) Do well in school (studies may show ac hortative parenting is comparable). 21 Not likely to engage in antisocial activities (especially: drug and alcohol abuse, vandalism, gangs) 2.4.3. Permissive parenting Obviously, this parenting style would be associated with low levels of responsiveness and some high levels of demandingness. The permissive parent attempts to behave in a no punitive, acceptant and affirmative manner towards the child’s impulses, desires, and actions. She (the parent) consults with him (the child) about policy decisions and gives explanations for family rules. She makes few demands for household responsibility and orderly behaviour. She presents herself to the child as a resource for him to use as he wishes, neither as an ideal for him to emulate, nor as an active agent responsible for shaping or altering his ongoing or future behaviours. Permissive parents affirm their children’s desires and actions, and avoid the use of power and control. They are, in a sense, a friend to their children without the guidance exercised by authoritative parents. They allow the child to regulate his own activities as much as possible, and do not encourage him to obey externally defined standards. They also attempt to use reasons and manipulation, but not overt power to accomplish their ends (Baumrind, 1971, 1991). 2.4.3.1. Permissive style and child qualities Children of permissive parents tend to be similar to of authoritarian parents, in that they are often hostile and uncooperative. These children are also lacking in self-control, are purposeless, and are not very achievement oriented (Baumrind, 1991). Other qualities of children permissive parents are as follows: 22 Poor emotion regulation (under regulation) Rebellious and defiant when desires are challenged. Low persistence to challenging tasks Antisocial behaviours 2.5. Psychological Effects of Parenting Styles The literature exploring the psychological attributes associated with different parenting styles appears to support the contention that parenting style has an effect on development. In exploring developmental outcomes in adolescents, Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, and Dornbusch(1991) surveyed 4,100 adolescents to determine the type of parenting style used by their parents and the adolescents levels of psychosocial development and levels of distress. After grouping the adolescents into their parental types: authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive, they were given measures of psychosocial development and internalized distress. Components of psychosocial development that were measured included social competence, work orientation, and self-reliance. Internalized distress referred to health problems and psychological symptoms of anxiety, tension, and depression. In this sample, adolescent who came from authoritative homes scored highest in psychosocial development and lowest in internalized distress. They were more competent and confident than adolescents from other types. Adolescents from authoritarian backgrounds scored reasonably well in psychosocial development, but tended to have low self-perceptions and confidence, and higher levels 23 of internalized distress. Finally, adolescents from permissive backgrounds scored reasonably high in social competency and self-perception, but low in work-orientation. Similarly, Kurdek, and Fine (1994) conducted a two-sample study to determine if family acceptance (responsiveness) and family control (demandingness) influenced the adjustment of young adolescents in the areas of psychosocial competence and selfregulation. In the first sample, psychosocial competence was assessed by measuring levels of self-esteem and self-efficacy, and self-regulation was measured by self-reports of academic performance and aggressive behaviours. In the second sample, adjustment was assessed by peer ratings of likability, which the authors suggest are linked to psychosocial competence and self-regulation. The findings of this study were found to be consistent with previous literature showing acceptance and control to be influential on the adjustment of adolescents. Adolescents from families of high in both acceptance and control had the highest levels of self-esteem, self-efficacy, and self-regulation. 2.6. Parenting styles and academic performance Parenting style has been found to predict child well-being in the domains of social competence, academic performance, psychosocial development, and problem behaviour. Research based on parents interviews, child reports and parents observations consistently find: Children and adolescents whose parents are authoritative rate themselves and are rated by objective measures as more socially and instrumental competent 24 then those parents who are non-authoritative (Baumrind, 1991, Weiss & Schwarz; 1996; Miller et al, 1993) Children and adolescents from authoritarian families (high in demandingness but low in responsiveness) tend to perform moderately well in school and be uninvolved in problem behaviour, but they have poorer social skills, lower self-esteem, and higher levels of depression (Maccoby and Martin, 1983). In a survey of 7,836 adolescents in the San Francisco Bay area, Dornbusch et al.(1987) found that Asian American parents were more authoritarian than European American parents, the authoritarian parenting styles was associated with lower academic grades. Children and adolescents from permissive homes (high in responsiveness, low in demandingness) are more likely to be involved in problem behavior and perform less well in school, but they have higher self-esteem, better social skills, and lower levels of depression (Maccoby and Martin, 1983). In general, parental responsiveness predicts social competence and psychosocial functioning, while parental demandingness is associated with instructional competence and behavioural control (i.e., academic performance and deviance). In reviewing the literature on parenting style, one is stuck by the consistency with which authoritative upbringing is associated with both instrumental and social competence and lower levels of problem behaviour in both boys and girls at all developmental stages. The benefits of authoritative parenting and the detrimental effects of uninvolved parenting are evident as early as the preschool years and continue throughout adolescence and into 25 early adulthood. Although specific differences can be the found in the competence by each group, the largest differences are found between children whose parents are unengaged and their peers with more involved parents. Differences between children from authoritative homes and their peers are equally consistent, but somewhat smaller (Weiss & Schwarz, 1996). Just as authoritative parents appear to be able to balance their conformity demands with their respect for their children’s individuality, children from authoritative homes appear to be able to balance the claims of external conformity and achievement demands with their need for individuation and autonomy (Kozol, 1991). 2.7. Epstein’s (2001) typology of parental involvement Much contemporary research on parental involvement has been drawn on the work of Joyce Epstein. Epstein (2001) typology of parental involvement is illustrated in the figure below. 26 Types of parental 2001) has drawn typology Involvement Definition 1. parenting 1. providing housing,health,nutritions, safety; parenting skills in parents-child interactions; home conditions to support study. 2. Communication 2. school-home/home-school communication 3. volunteering 3. in school help in classrooms/events 4. Learning at home 4. Help with choices/options 5. Decision-Making homework, help with educational 5.Membership of Parent-Teacher-Association(PTA) or School-Management-Committee(SMC 6. Collaborating with the 6. Community contributions to school/school contributions community to community Figure 2.2 Epstein’s (2001) conceptual framework for family-school involvement The researcher did not discuss the first, fifth and the sixth constructs in the study because the first is the researcher major independent variable (IV), the fifth is directly associated with decision- making (e.g. through P.T.A.), while the sixth is also more associate with the community involvement which is usually done through the ParentTeacher-Association. 2.7.1. Type 1: communicating between school and home Research suggests that the association between school- home communication and students was relatively small (Sui-Chu & Willms, 1996) with varied outcomes and whether the desired outcomes were standardized scores or students’ grade. Grades are 27 slightly more impacted than achievement test scores (Desimone, 1999) which may be the result of parent(s) communicating with the school and/or teacher at the time were impacted. McNeal (1999) indicates that because school-home communication and levels parent involvement vary by income level with the type the involvement, this suggest that some groups may feel comfortable communicating than others. This implies that parent involvement programs should develop positive communication strategies unique to the context of their own community. Parent involvement programs that review and adapt effective strategies used by schools with similar family and community background characteristics might be beneficial. 2.7.2. Type 2: learning activities to involve parents with students at home Desimone (1998), referring to studies by Muller (1995), concluded that school-level involvement had less effect on academic performance than parent-child involvement. The findings show that parent-child discussion is significantly related to increased academic achievement for whites and African-Americans; however, the link was not significant for Hispanics or Asians (McNeal, (1999). Sui-Chu & Willms (1996) found that home discussion of school activities was one of the stronger predictors of students’ academic performance. Although the dynamics of parent-child discussion about school are not clearly understood, it reflects positively on students’ academic achievement (Balli et al, 1998). The employment status of mothers affects child supervision after school, the nature of parent-child activities during this time, and the degree to which the parent is able to become involved in after-school activities. Better availability of supervised after-school 28 activities for adolescents, flexible work schedules that permit parents to participate in school activities in school functions, and school policies that accommodate working parents are three areas for possible improvement (Muller, 1995). Parent(s) helping students with homework or checking homework had a negative relationship with academic achievement (Wang & Wildman, 1994). Some researches’ believe this is an intervention strategy or a negative outcome of parental monitoring of an adolescent seeking his or her own independence. The findings suggest that perhaps a more proactive parent stance might prevent problems before they occur. Providing alternative school-based strategies for assisting adolescents with their homework in ways they find acceptable might be considered. The negative relationship may simply be due to parents who are trying to help a student who needs help. Based upon initial findings from parent involvement programmes, students’ academic work and attitudes improve when students conduct interactive homework with family members (Epstein, 1984). 2.7.3. Type 3: Decision making This includes parents in decisions about school programs via the Parents Teachers Association (PTA) and school improvement committees (Desimore, 1999). Being involved in these organizations allows parents to learn about school programsmes, policies and curriculum (Epstein, 1984). Consequently, parents can suggest ideas for school improvements and voice opinions about the quality of the school and school 29 programmes. Desimore (1999) emphasized that such parents discuss their expectations with their children. 2.8. The concept of Parental Involvement The child’s first place of contact with the world is the family. The child, as a result, acquires initial education and socialization from parents and other significant persons in the family. The parents are, in short, the child’s first teacher. They are the first and primary source of social support for young children. In the Ghanaian setting, the responsibility for raising a child is a collective one. When parents are involved in the education of their children, the children tend to model their parents’ attitude and actions (Georgiou, 1997). Suffice it to say that parents have profound influence on every aspect of child’s life. Available and accessible research evidence have indicated that students with parents who are involved in their education tend to have fewer behavioural problems and better academic performance and less likely to drop out of school than students whose parents are not involved in their school ( Rich, Van Dlen&Mallox,1979) Corroborating the above, Reynolds (1994) demonstrated that a significant relationship existed between parental involvement and academic performance. According to Fantuzzo, Davis, and Ginsberg (1995), the term parental involvement refers to a variety of parental behaviours that directly or indirectly influences children’s cognitive development and school achievement. Illustrative examples of these parents’ behaviours presented in the empirical literature, are: attending parent-teacher conferences, being a 30 member of a Parent- Teacher-Association (PTA), helping with homework, discussing school activities with the child’s school progress, monitoring the child’s out-of-school activities, and talking regularly with the child. Although the dynamics of parent involvement and student academic performance at the senior high level are not clearly understood, current efforts of purposeful parent involvement strategies make a difference (Baker and Soden (1998). The leadership of every high school, regardless of school enrolment size or community socio-economic composition, has an obligation to engage in discussions about purposeful parent involvement. There is no evident research documenting a negative effect as a result of the implementation of a well – designed parent involvement strategy (Eccles et al., 1993). According to Leler (1983), the earlier in a child's educational process parent involvement begins, the more powerful the effect. He further emphasized that the most effective forms of parent involvement are those, which engage parents in working directly with their children on learning activities at home. It is very clear that parental involvement is beneficial. It can definitely benefit the student in question, but it can also be benefit the teachers, school, the parents themselves and the community, as well as other children in the family. Everything possible should be done by the school system to encourage the parents to become involved. This is especially true of the headmaster of the school, and it is his or her leadership that will guide the teachers in the direction of emphasizing the importance of parental involvement. 31 2.9. Major factors of Parental Involvement According to Leler (1983), there are three main factors of parental involvement in the education of their children; Parents' beliefs about what is important, necessary and on behalf of their children; The extent to which parents believe that they can have a positive influence on their children's education; and Parents' perceptions that their children and school want them to be involved. When schools encourage children to practice reading at home with parents, the children make significant gains in reading achievement compared to those who only practice at school. Parents who read to their children, have books available, guide T.V watching, and provide stimulating experiences contribute to students' academic performance (Clark, 1990). According to him when parents come to school regularly, it reinforces the view in the child's mind that school and home are connected and that school is an integral part of the whole family life. 2.10. Parental involvement as school success strategy Simon (2001) theorized that parents' involvement has made an impact on a child's learning and motivation. The study looked at various types of involvement including, learning at home (home instruction), decision-making, communication and participation in school governance. 32 Epstein et al (2002) discuss parental involvement as educational tool needed to achieve academic success. Simon (2001) specifies the importance of a quiet time and place for homework. There was also a discussion on the negative impact of television, but the positive impact of praise. Beck and Murphy (1999) state that communication is a vital component for school success. Parents who prepare their children talk about setbacks, possible stressors and coping skills. Individual differences in children's academic achievement were studied by Fan & Chen (2001) relative to differences in the parental involvement. They found the children of authoritative parenting and parental involvement to be positively related with academic achievement. Children with higher test scores come from more supportive homes. The parents of senior high schools were asked about conveying positive feeling, conversing with the student, answering student's questions and assisting them in learning at home. 2.11. Importance of parenting style and parental involvement D' Agostino et al (2001) show the role of the family and the specific interactions between a child and parent have been determined to be powerful indicators of development. Some specific interactions include family discussions, encouragement, assisting students in their homework, involve children in decision making, and limit setting, daily routine, praise and intellectual stimulation. These studies have shown all of these connections to produce an impact on academic achievement. 33 Children have an unbelievable thirst for knowledge. If parents do not tap into that drive in early childhood, it could be lost before they even enter the school system. The parents that do not foster learning are easily identified. It is truly amazing how little children mention their parents. Parents' encouragement to achieve and interest in school performance are significantly related to student motivation and achievement. (Crozier, 2000)).He found that: '' What might be called '' the curriculum the home'' predict academic learning twice as well as the socioeconomic status of families. This curriculum includes informed parent/child conversations about everyday events, encouragement and discussion of schoolwork (homework), monitoring and joint analysis of televiewing; deferral of immediate gratification is to accomplish long-term goals, expressions of even occasional doses of caprice and serendipity. In 29 controlled studies conducted during the past decade, 91% of the comparisons favoured children in programs designed to improve the learning environment of the home over children not participating in such programs. Although the average effect was twice that of socioeconomic status, some program had effect ten times as large''. Drake (2000) spoke about the importance of taking time for children, playing with them, taking decision with them and assisting them in doing their homework. The decision parents make to involve children in the family decision making, or allow the child to be unsupervised will make a profound impact on their academic performance. The most vulnerable and dangerous time for children is between 3:00PM and 8:00PM. They may not be perpetrators but they well could be the victims. Victims outnumber perpetrators (Drake, 2000) 34 Stegelin (2002) declare the importance for parents to communicate positively, take decision with their adolescents and assist them in learning at home. Reading materials should be abundant and discussed on regular intervals. The study states that parental involvement and parenting styles show a significant relationship to academic achievement. 2.12. Empirical Evidence A research demonstrating the effects of parenting styles is a study of nearly 11, 000 high school students in California and Wisconsin by Laurence Steinberg and Dornbush and their colleagues. Of this, 6,902 were followed over a two-year period, providing valuable longitudinal information (Dornbusch et al, 1987, Glasgow et al, 1997; Lamborn et al, 1991; Steinberg et al, 1989). The researchers measured parenting styles by asking the students themselves to respond to questions about both parental acceptance responsiveness and parental control or demand the dimensions define Maccoby and Martin (1983) category system. The students were asked to indicate the extent to which each of the following statements was true or not true: I can count on my parents to help me out if I have some kind of problem. When my parents want me to do something they explain why. My parents know exactly where I am most afternoons after school. On the basis of students’ answers to such questions, Steinberg and Dornbusch were able to classify most of the students’ families in the Maccoby/Martin category system and 35 could then look at the relationship between these parenting styles and a variety of behaviours and the students’ performance in schools. They also found that: Students from authoritative families showed a higher self-reliance, higher social competence better grades, fewer indications of psychological distress, and lower levels of schools misconduct, drug use, and delinquency. Students from authoritarian families had the lowest scores on the several measures of social competence and self reliance. Teenagers from permissive families had the least optimal scores on measures of problem behaviors and school achievement (Steinberg et al 1994). In another longitudinal analysis of the data for the early, 7000 students for whom they have two years information, these same researchers found that: Students who described their parents as most authoritative at the beginning of the study showed more improvement in academic competence and self-reliance and smallest increases in psychological symptoms and delinquent behaviour over the succeeding two years, suggesting that the family has a causal, continuing effect. Authoritative parents not only create good family climate and thereby support and motivate their child optimally; they also behave differently toward the child’s school. They are much more likely to be involved with the school, attending school functions or talking to teachers, as this involvement seems to play a crucial role. When an otherwise authoritative parent is not involved with the school, the outcomes for the students are not clearly positive. 36 A student whose parent is highly involved with the school but is not authoritative shows less optimal outcomes. It is a combination of an authoritativeness and school involvement that is associated with the best result. Another research that demonstrated the parental pattern and academic achievement was conducted by Dwairy et al (2006) in eight Arab societies based on the Baumrind (1966) prototypical descriptions of the parenting styles (permissiveness, authoritarianism and authoritativeness) and measured these parenting styles through interviews and observations with 2, 893 adolescents and their parents. Their results suggested the following: Authoritarian and permissive parentings were associated with mental health problems and had low academic performance whereas authoritative parenting was associated with improved mental health and well-being. They also associated authoritative parenting with non coercive and democratic parenting which encourages the child to express individually with the family. The adolescents from authoritative homes showed more improvement in academic competence. They found that authoritarian parenting was associated with harsh, restrictive and psychological methods of control that do not foster psychological autonomy. They concluded that, there are a lot of factors that contributed to students' academic achievement therefore deductions that can be drawn from parenting styles and academic achievement should be approached with caution 37 2.13. Theoretical framework for the proposed study The theoretical framework that underpins the proposed study is derived from family motivation theories. In particular, the theoretical models shown as figure 2 and 3 dwells upon the constructs proposed by Baumrind (1966, 1991) on parenting styles and the Epstein (2001) typology of parental involvement . Intelligent is not the only determinant of academic performance. High motivation and encouragement in learning have consistently been linked to reduced dropout rates and increased levels of students’ academic success (Kushman, Sieber & Harold, 2000). Development of academic intrinsic motivation in students is an important goal for educators because of its inherent importance for future motivation as well as students’ effective school functioning (Gottfriend, 1990). The family is the primary social system for children. Philips (1998) found that high parental control, support, were associated with high academic performance. Academic performance is accomplished by actual execution of class work in school setting. It is typically assessed by the use of teacher rating, and examinations. Research shows that student’s perceptions of academic competency decline as they advance in school (Eccles, Wigfield & Schiefele, 1998). Hammer (2003) attributed this decline to various factors that included the lack of parents’ school involvement and inefficient parenting practices. Academic performance gab is not only about what goes on once students get into the classroom. It is also, about what happens to them before and after school. Parents have a crucial role to play to make sure that every child becomes a high 38 achiever. Parents influence has been identified as an important factor affecting student academic performance (Wang, Wildman, & Calhoun, 1996). The model diagrammed in Figure 2.3, has three causal paths that feed into academic performance variable: The impact of the parenting style (main effect) on academic performance (Path 1) The impact of the parental involvement (main effect) on academic performance(Path 2) The interaction or product of Path (1) and Path (2), that is Path (3) on the academic performance- the moderator. The moderator hypothesis is supported if the interaction (Path 3) is significant. There may be significant main effects for the Path (1) and Path (2) respectively, but are not true effect or relevant that can use to describe the relationships on the academic performance. In statistics, moderation occurs when the relationship between two variables depends on a third variable (Howell, 2002). The third variable is referred to as the moderator variable or simply the moderator. The effect of a moderating variable is characterized statistically as an interaction, which affects the direction and/or strength of the relation between dependent and independent variables. Specifically within a correlation analysis framework, a moderator is a third variable that affects the zero-order correlation between two other variables. In analysis of variance (ANOVA) terms, a basic moderator effect can be represented as an interaction between a 39 focal independent variable and a factor that specifies the appropriate conditions for its operation (Baron and Kenny, 1986). Parenting Styles 1. Authoritarian 2. Permissive 3. Authoritative 1 Moderator (Interaction) Parental Involvement Academic 3 2 Decision-making Communication Learning at home Figure 2.3 Theoretical Framework for the proposed study 40 Performance CHAPTER THREE METHODOLOGY 3.1. Introduction This chapter deals with general research methodology and covers the following subheadings. Research design Rationale for Adopting Quantitative Approach Population of the study Study area Instrumentations Pilot Study Validity of the Instruments Reliability of Instruments Data Collection Procedure Procedure for Data Analysis Data analysis 3.2. Research design The study followed a cross-sectional design using the quantitative model. The crosssectional design is a type of design that will enable the researcher to collect data across a wide range in a relatively short time and at a point in time. Unlike the longitudinal and 41 retrospective designs, the data collected from cross-sectional design are `snap-shot` of respondents’ views at that moment of data collection. The study also followed a quantitative research model using an exploration and descriptive design. Quantitative research approach is a process in which numerical data is used to obtain information and consist of descriptive, correlation, experimental, and quasi-experimental research. Descriptive research is the exploration and description of phenomena in real situations. It allows the researcher to generate new knowledge of the subject by describing characteristics of persons, situations and the frequency with which certain phenomena occur (Schunk, 1991). The exploratory research allows the use of questionnaire to a large sample of the population and is therefore intent on finding facts that relate to the field of study (Schiefele, 1991). The exploratory research probes more by allowing for an in-depth exploration of dimensions of the phenomena, including manifestation and related factors (Rubin, 2005). A survey method of data collection through questionnaire was used. The survey is a nonexperimental, descriptive research method. There are two basic types of surveys: crosssectional surveys and longitudinal surveys (Rubin, 2005). The researcher employed the cross-sectional survey in this study. Cross-sectional surveys are used to gather information on a population at a single point in time. According to Rubin (2005) the advantage of this method is that it is less expensive, relatively easy to administer, efficient way of collecting information from a large number of respondents, permits anonymity and may result in more honest responses. 42 3.3. Rationale for Adopting Quantitative Approach The scientific method widely used in both the natural and social science is derived from a system of philosophy known as positivism but the one that has been most influential in this century is logical positivism (Bernard, 2000). Positivism is a system of philosophy that excludes everything from its consideration except natural phenomena and their interrelationships. According to Creswell (2002), one of the major principles of logical positivism is the verifiability principle, which states that something is meaningful unless they can be verified through direct observation of the world. For example, if I make the knowledge claim that elementary school teachers ask mostly factual questions, that claim is meaningful and valid (if I am a logical positivist),only if I have direct observation of the teachers behaviours'. Again, I must have defined the concept of “fact questions” in such a way that, I, or anyone else, can identify instances of fact questions using only observable characteristics of teacher behaviours. Positivism, then places premium on observation of the world ‘’out there’’. The researchers' values, interpretations, feelings, and musings have no place in the positivism’s view of scientific inquiry. The quantitative researcher must be objective. In short the quantitative research is noted on positivistic approach of scientific inquiry (Bernard, 2000). The researcher approached the study quantitatively because he is aiming to produce numerical results of data that can in some way be quantified. The researcher tends to use 43 large sample of respondents to gather findings that can be extrapolated to tell us about a population in general. Another reason the researcher approached the study quantitatively was that the researcher wished to keep himself from influencing the collecting of data. Statistical methods are used to analyze the data and draw conclusions. In other words, the researcher wanted to be objective, meaning, the researcher wished to develop an understanding of the world as it is ‘’out there’’, independent of his personal biases, values and idiosyncratic notions. Again, because of the nature of the investigation (i.e. looking for differences), the quantitative approach is suitable. 3.4. Study Area The study was conducted at Offinso, the headquarters of Offinso South Municipal Assembly in Ashanti Region of Ghana. Offinso is a small town of nearly 6, 500 people. The largest employees are teachers, hospital workers, police, and other civil servants etc. Farming is also an important industry to this community. The community has limited culture diversity. Offinso South community has two senior high schools consisting students across the entire regions of Ghana. The students were approximately between 15 and 18 years old. Offinso is about twenty nine (29) kilometers from Kumasi and on Kumasi-Techiman, road. Offinso is located on the bank of river Offin, hence the name Offinso. 44 3.5. Population of the Study The target population for this study comprised all Senior High School three (3) students in the study area. There are two Senior High Schools in the study area. These are; Dwamena Akenten Senior High School (D.A.S.H.S=534 students), and Namong Senior High School (N.A.S.H.S=279 students), with the total population of 813. 45 Table 3:1 The table below shows the distribution of the schools with their third year (S.H.S.3) population CLASS D.A.S.H.S. N.A.S.H.S. No. of Students 3A1 45 41 86 3A2 46 -- 46 3A3 51 -- 51 3B1 48 49 97 3B2 52 -- 52 3H1 46 52 98 3H2 42 -- 42 3S1 46 -- 46 3S2 44 -- 44 3AG 48 45 93 3TECHAP -- 48 48 3TECHM -- 44 44 TOTAL 534 279 813 Source: Field Data (2011) Note: 3A1 = This is third year Arts class that offers Elective Mathematics Geography and Economics 3A2 = This is third year Arts class that offers Geography, Economics, Christian Religious Studies, 46 3A3 = This is third year Arts class that offers Economics, English Literature, and Akan 3A4 = Third year Arts class that offers Economics, English Literature and Government 3B1 = Third year class that offer Accounting, Business Management, Elective Mathematics 3B2 = Accounting, Business Management, Costing 3H1 = Third year class that offers Home Economics. Their subjects include; Food and Nutrition, Management in Living, General Knowledge in Arts and Biology 3H2 = Third year class that offers Home Economics. Their subjects include; Clothing and Textiles, Management in Living, General Knowledge in Arts and Biology. 3S1 & 3S2 = Third year class that offers Biology, Chemistry, Physics, and Elective Mathematics 3AG = Third year class that offers Agriculture Studies. Their subjects include; General Agriculture, Animal Husbandry, Chemistry and Elective Mathematics 3TECHAP = Third year technical class that offers Elective Mathematics, Elective Physics, Technical Drawing and Applied Electricity. 3TECHM = Third year technical class that offers Elective Mathematics, Elective Physics, Technical Drawing and Metals 47 3.6. Sample This is a small proportion of the population randomly selected subsets of the whole, which is being used to represent the population (Alhasssan, 2007). By observing the character of the sample, inference can be made about the characteristics of the population from which it has been drawn. The sample size comprised two hundred and eight (208) as Table 3:2 shows, thirteen (13) students were randomly selected from each class from the schools stated above. Table 3:2 The table below illustrates the distribution of schools (S.H.S.3) and sample selected Classes D.A.S.H.S. N.A.S.H.S. No. of Students 3A1 13 13 26 3A2 13 -- 13 3A3 13 -- 13 3B1 13 13 26 3B2 13 -- 13 3H1 13 13 26 3H2 13 -- 13 3S1 13 -- 13 3S2 13 -- 13 3AG 13 13 26 3TECHAP -- 13 13 3TECHM -- 13 13 Total 130 78 208 48 In choosing the sample size, simple random sampling procedure was used. Simple random sampling is one of the most popular types of probability sampling. In this technique, each member of the population has an equal chance of being selected as subject. The entire process of sampling was done in a single step with each subject selected independently of the other members of the population. There are many methods to proceed with simple random sampling, but the lottery method was used in this study. With the simple random sampling, ‘’YES’’ or ‘’NO’’ were written on pieces of paper and was picked by the students. Those who picked ‘’YES'' were selected to represent the sample size. 3.7. Variables of the Study There are three main variables involved in this study, which are categorized as dependent and independent variables. There is only one dependent variable and two independent variables. Each of the independent variable has three levels. Table 3:3 presents all the characteristics of these variables. Table 3:3. Identification of the variables Type of variable Name of variable Type of variable Type of scale DV Academic performance Continuous Interval IV Parenting Styles Discrete Nominal IV Parental Involvement 49 Discrete Nominal 3.7.1. Dependent Variable The dependent variable of the study is the students’ academic performance. The students academic performance was assessed by using the Mathematics Achievement Test(MAT), English Achievement Teat(EAT), and Science Achievement Test(SAT). This consisted of 20 multiple choice of MAT, 20 of EAT, and 20 of SAT respectively. The average score was taken to represent the students’ academic performances. It is continuous variables and measured on interval scale. 3.7.2. Independent Variables The independent variables included in the study are parenting styles and parental involvement. As indicated on table 3:3, each of the independent variables had three levels. Both the parenting styles and parental involvement are considered to be discrete variables and measured on nominal scale. Authoritative parenting style was coded three (3), two (2) for permissive, and one (1) for authoritarian. On the other hand, learning at home was coded three (3), two (2) for communication, and one (1) for decision making. 3.8: Instrumentation The instruments that were used for the study were Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) and Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ). Data from these instruments provided the basis for all subsequent analysis used to test the defined research hypotheses. 3.8.1 Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) Buri (1991) developed a self-report measure asking students to respond to how their parents act toward them. The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) had co-efficient alpha of 0.81. The PAQ (Appendix A) was used to assess the level of parenting style with 50 respect to Baumrind’s (1966) primary parenting style typologies’ authoritarian (high control, low warm), authoritative (high control and high warm) and permissive (low control, high warm). The measure consist of 30 items,10 for each of the different styles of parenting in a 5- point likert format ranging from strongly disagree(1) to strongly agree(5). The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) was designed as a measure of Baumrind (1966) three parenting styles based on authority, disciplinary practices of warmth, demands, expectations and control. The three parenting styles questions are embedded in the questionnaire in a random order. Authoritative parents are flexible, use reason with their children, are rational, maintain firm and clear boundaries, while being consistent in the expectations of their children’s behaviour (items q4, q5, q8, q11, q15, q20, q22, q23, q27, and q30). Authoritarian parents attempt to maintain unquestioning obedience from their children and attempt to control their behaviour with punishment as a form of discipline (items q2, q3, q7, q9, q12, q16, q18, q25, q26, and q29). Permissive parents tend to be relatively warm as well as non-demanding and controlling of the child (items q1, q6, q10, q13, q14, q17, q19, q21, q24, and q28). To score the PAQ, the individual items for each parenting subtype were summed. The score on each subscale are from a minimum of 10 to a maximum of 50. 3.8.2. Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) The researcher constructed the Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) in 2010. It was utilized to gather information regarding parents school involvement related to the study: communication, learning at home and decision- making. It is a fithteen-item scale with response anchor based on likert 5-point format. Some of the items of the scale read as 51 follows: (1) My parents discuss with me on what I learnt at school, (2) My parents listen when I talk to them, etc 3.9. The pilot study A pilot study was embarked on at Apam Senior High School in Central Region of Ghana with 100 S.H.3 students. It was done to ascertain the factor structure, construct validity, and reliability of the Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) questionnaire used in the research. Fifteen (15) item questionnaire developed by the researcher was used to tap three construct namely; decision making, communication, and learning at home. These were factor analyzed to ascertain its structure. The questionnaire used a 5-point likert scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). The direction of the score was that, the higher your score, the higher the chance of experiencing that construct. 3.10. Validity of the Instrument Validity refers to the degree of which an instrument measures what it is supposed to measure. In other words (Garson, 2006) a valid instrument actually measures the concept it is supposed to measure. In this study, content and construct validity were used to assess the validity of the instrument by means of assessing the adequacy, appropriateness, inclusiveness and relevancy of the questions to the subject under study. 3.10.1. Content Validity of the Instrument Content validity of an instrument means validating the fact that an instrument designed does represent the factors under study (Garson, 2006). In order to establish the content validity of a measuring instrument, the researcher identified the overall content to be represented. Items were randomly chosen from the content that accurately represented the 52 information in all areas. By using this method, the researcher obtained a group of items that is representative of the content of the property to be measured. Again, those items were constructed based on the Epstein’s (2001) typology of parental involvement the researcher selected to investigate. 3.10.2. Construct validity of the Instrument Construct validity approach concerns the degree to which the test measures the construct it was designed to measure (Garson, 2006). The researcher assessed the construct validity by using factor analysis, since it is a method of selecting only the indicators or items that tap the concept the researcher is trying to measure. 3.11. Factor Analysis of Parental Authority Questionnaire Factor analysis is a type of analytical statistical technique used to develop questionnaire in order to make sure that instrument measure what it is supposed to measure. After collecting the data from the respondents, it was fed into SPSS version 16. The data was first screened to make sure that it was normally distributed, thus meeting one of the assumption of parametric test. This was done using the skewness of the distribution on each of the variables. Each distribution was judged using the z-statistic of -/+3.29. According to Ofori and Dampson (in prep), distribution with the resulting z-statistic score more than -/+3.29 after dividing its skewness value by its standard error (SE) of skewness indicates that the distribution is abnormally skewed. Using this criterion of-/+3.29, it was found that items 8, 10 and 15 were abnormally skewed. For example, item 8 had zstatistic of -4.75, item 10 had z-statistic of 21.8 and item 15 had the z-statistic of -4.01, hence those items were excluded from further analysis. 53 The remaining 27 items were subjected to Principal Component (PC) with Oblinim Rotation using SPSS version 16 to ascertain the appropriateness of the factors. Oblinim rotation was used because the items are related. These factors were confirmed using factor loadings based on the content of the items. Factor loading exceeding 0.3 was used because the greater the loading, the higher the variable is efficient to measure what it is suppose to measure and that a factor loading of 0.1 for instance is not strong enough to ascertain the pureness of the measure of the factor( Howell,2002) First, an abridged version of the Rotated Matrix(R-Matrix) was inspected with the top half of the table contains the Pearson Correlation Coefficient between all pairs of the questions whereas the bottom half contains the one-tailed significance of these coefficients. The researcher screened the significance values and looked for variables for which the majority of values were greater than 0.05. In addition the researcher checked if there were any correlation coefficients where the relationship exceeded 0.9 or -0.9. It was found that items 2, 11 and 26 had their significance value greater than 0.05 and as a result made the KMO statistic 0.449. Kaiser (1974) recommends accepting values greater than 0.5 (values below this should lead to collect more data). These items were eliminated and the researcher re-run the analysis for the remaining 24 items. After 21 iterations, rotation converged with the extraction of 3 factors with eigen values above 1.0. The items 17, 18, 21, 23, 25, and 28 were found to be cross loaded meaning that they were not meaningfully measuring a single construct and as a result eliminated from further analyses. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.63, 54 above the recommended value of .50, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (i.e. 2 (78) = 143.471, p = .000). The diagonals of the anti-image correlation matrix were all over .5, supporting the inclusion of each item in the factor analysis. Finally, the communalities were all above .3 (see Table 1), further confirming that each item shared some common variance with other items. According to the rotated component matrix presented above, all items except 27, 29, fit into their component correctly. Although, item 27 was included in factor one which tapped the construct of permissiveness, it belongs to authoritative. Item 29 was included in three (authoritative), it belong to factor two (authoritarian). The researcher decided to include it at where the rotation placed it at the present rotation, because it may be due to cultural differences. Seven items (6, 1, 13, 14, 24, 19, and 27) showed their highest loading on factor one. These tapped permissiveness construct that accounted for 18.2% of the total variance in the data rotated. Items 9, 3, 12, 5, 7, and 4 showed the second highest loading on factor two which tapped the construct of authoritarian. This factor accounted for 12.3% of the total variance in the data rotated. Items 29, 22, 16, and 20 also loaded on factor three accounting for 8.7% of the total variance in the data rotated. Item 30 did not load above 0.3 on any factor. The final 17 items with their loading and communalities values are presented in Table 3.4 below: 55 T able 3.4 Factor loadings and communalities based on a principle components analysis with oblimin rotation for 17 items Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) (N = 50) Factor loading Communality Factors 1 2 3 Factor 1 – Permissiveness (alpha =0.75) q6 0.76 0.59 q1 0.71 0.48 q13 0.70 0.56 q14 0.68 0.50 q24 0.64 0.50 q19 0.56 0.47 q27 0.55 0.41 Factor 2 – Authoritarian (alpha =0.77) q9 0.65 0.53 q3 0.58 0.58 q12 0.54 0.48 q5 0.50 0.47 q7 0.57 0.56 q4 0.48 0.49 Factor 3 – Authoritative (alpha = 0.78) q29 0.76 0.43 q22 0.57 0.41 q16 0.49 0.49 q20 0.42 0.40 56 The internal consistency for each of the subscales was examined using Cronbach alpha. The alpha value for items 6,1,13, 14, 24, 19, and 27 (permissiveness) had the alpha value of 0.75. Items 9, 3, 12, 5, 7, and 4 (authoritarian) had the alpha value of 0.77. Items 29, 22, 16, and 20 (authoritative) had the alpha value of 0.78. The skewness and kurtosis were well within a tolerable range for assuming a normal distribution and examination of the histograms that the distributions looked approximately normal. Thus the data were well suited for parametric statistical analyses. Therefore, the three types of parenting styles were rooted in the questionnaire (Appendix A). The permissiveness consists of the items 1, 3, 6, 9, 11, 12 and 14. The authoritarian also consists of the items 2, 5, 8, 13, 15, and 16. Lastly, authoritative consists the items of 4, 7, 10 and 17 respectively. 3.12. Factor Analysis on Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) The 15 item questionnaire designed by the researcher was to tap the construct of decision making, communication and learning at home. Responses were on a Likert – type scale, ranging from 1 ‘’ Strongly Disagree’’, 2 = ‘’Disagree’’, 3 = ‘’ Neither’’, 4= ‘’Agree’’, 5 = ‘’Strongly Agree’’. After the administration of the instrument, they were factor analyzed to ascertain its factor structure. Prior to the analysis, the data were screened for univariate outliers, and two of the items (13 and 15) were deleted for the fact that they failed to meet the assumption underlying parametric test. That is such items had a z-statistic of 10.46 and 13.32 for skewness. This 57 was beyond the p = 0.001 criterion of +/- 3.29 indicating that it is abnormally positively skewed. As a result, they were excluded from further analysis. Initially, the factorability of the 13 items was examined. All of the 13 items were found correlated at least 0.3 with at least one other item, suggesting reasonable factorability. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .54, above the recommended value of .50, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (2 (153) = 211.063, p < .001). The diagonals of the anti-image correlation matrix were all over .5, supporting the inclusion of each item in the factor analysis. Finally, the communalities were all above .3 (see Table 3.5), further confirming that each item shared some common variance with other items. Given these overall indicators, factor analysis was conducted with all 13 items. Principle components analysis using oblimin was used because the items really relate and also the primary purpose was to identify and compute composite parental involvement scores for the factors underlying the PIQ. In other words, to ascertain the appropriateness of the hypothesized three-factor model. The first three eigen values showed that the first factor explained 20.9% of the variance, the second factor 15% of the variance, and a third factor 11.6% of the variance. The three factor solution, which explained 46.5% of the variance. This is when the rotation converged in this run after 9 iterations with the extraction of four factors with the stated eigen values. Three items were eliminated, because Item 11 was found cross-loaded meaning it did not contribute to a simple factor structure and failed to meet measuring a particular construct. 58 Items 8 and 14 also did not load above 0.3 on any factor. The factor loading matrix for this final solution using the oblimin technique is shown in the table 3.8 below. Table 3.5 Factor loadings and communalities based on a principle components analysis with oblimin rotation for 13 items Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) (N = 50) Factor loading Communality Factors 1 2 3 Factor 1 – Decision Making (alpha =0.63) Q12 .773 .64 Q7 .768 .55 Q9 .683 . 56 q10 .675 .46 Factor 2 – Communication (alpha =0.70) Q5 -.763 .68 q3 .717 .59 q6 .709 .41 Factor 3 – Learning at Home (alpha = 0.60) q2 .748 .40 q1 -.714 .44 q4 .538 .42 59 The internal consistency for each of the subscales was examined using Cronbach alpha. The alpha value for items 12, 7, 9, and 10 (decision making) had the alpha value of 0.63. Items 5, 3, and 6 (communication) had the alpha value of 0.70. Items 2, 1, and 4(learning at home) had the alpha value of 0.60. The skewness and kurtosis were well within a tolerable range for assuming a normal distribution and examination of the histograms that the distributions looked approximately normal. Thus the data were well suited for parametric statistical analyses. Therefore, the three types of parents’ school involvement were rooted in the questionnaire (Appendix B). The learning at home consists of the items 1, 2, and 3. The communication also consists of the items 4, 5, and 6. Lastly, decision-making consists the items 7, 8, 9, and 10 respectively. 3.13. Reliability of instruments Reliability is important criteria for evaluating quantitative instrument. The reliability was measured using the Cronbach’s Alpha. According to Howell (2002), reliability is the consistency with which the instrument measures the target attribute. This means that administering the same instrument by various researchers will provide the same results under comparable conditions. According to Garson (2006), reliability can be estimated in one of the following four ways that is internal consistency, split-half reliability, test-retest reliability, and inter-rater reliability. In this study, reliability of the instrument (PIQ) was tested by means of Cronbach Alpha that is the most common means of testing internal consistency if the 60 items, using the SPSS package. Internal consistency reliability refers to the extent to which all the subparts of the instruments will measure the identified attributes. According to Garson (2006) reliability of 0.60 is common in exploratory research; the alpha should be at least 0.70 or higher to retain an item in an adequate scale. Factor analysis was done for the themes to identify the items that may not be consistent with the themes within each category. The table below indicates the co-efficient alpha of the two instruments. Table 3.6 Reliability Analysis for Students Responses Subscale Cronbach’ Alpha Number of Items Authoritative 0.78 4 Authoritarian 0.77 6 Permissive 0.75 7 Learning at Home 0.60 3 Communication 0.70 3 Decision Making 0.63 4 The table above shows that reliability statistics of the various constructs of the questionnaire. It shows the number of items and its cronbach alpha. The table shows that 61 the authoritative subscale has an alpha co-efficient of 0.78 on 4 items; authoritarian had the alpha co-efficient of 0.77 on 6 items. Permissive also had alpha co-efficient of 0.75 with 7 items. On the other hand Learning at Home has an alpha co-efficient of 0.60 on 3 items; Communication has an alpha co-efficient of 0.70 on 3 items; Decision Making has an alpha co-efficient of 0.63 on 4 items. Table 3.7 Overall Reliability Analysis Variables Number of items Cronbach Alpha PAQ 17 0.78 PIQ 10 0.66 3.14. Students Academic Performance ---- MAT, SAT, and EAT The students academic performance was assessed by using the Mathematics Achievement Test(MAT), English Achievement Test (EAT), and Science Achievement Test(SAT). These subjects are key and examinable subjects on the school curriculum and it is believed that assessing the students’ academic performance in these disciplines will be useful and appropriate for this research. The test was prepared by making use of the questions taken from the WASSCE between the years of 2006 – 2010 and consisted of 20 multiple choice of MAT, 20 of EAT, and 20 of SAT respectively. Questions were selected based on the syllabus that is the same in all schools due to the settings of Ministry of Education Science and Sports. Average score of each student was taken to 62 represent the student academic performance. The following procedure was followed by the researcher while developing the achievement tests: The content of the syllabus up to third year first term was examined. All related questions were collected and a multiple pool was formed. The questions were given to three member panel on each subject area for selection of test items Items that had low corrected item – total correlations were deleted, distractors evaluated were not considered since the test items were standardized test items. 3.14.1. Try – Out the Test Items – Item Analysis The test items were tried – out to 50 students in the same school (Apam Senior High School). The purpose of this (item analysis) was to identify any test items that are not working well, too easy, too difficult, failing to show a difference between skilled and unskilled examinees. According to Gronlund (1985), there are two main approaches to item analysis; qualitative and quantitative analysis. The qualitative analysis includes the consideration of content validity (content and form of items) as well as the evaluation of items in terms of effective item-writing procedures. The quantitative item analysis on the other hand includes principally the measurement of item difficulty and item discrimination. Two most common quantitative item analysis reported in this study were item difficulty, which is a measure of the proportion of examinees who responded to an item correctly, and item discrimination, which is a measure of how well the item discriminates between examinees who were knowledgeable in the content area and those were are not. 63 3.14.2. Index of Difficulty A good test is a test that contains items which are not too difficult and too easy. This is the index of item difficulty. It is also referred to as p – value. Nunnaly (1964) states that the index of difficulty of an item illustrates how easy or difficult certain item is established in the test. Item difficulty is simply the percentage of students who answer the item correctly. It ranges from 0.0 to 1.0; the higher the value, the easier the question. The following formula was used to calculate the index of difficulty of an item. P = Np/N, where Np = the number of correct answers, and N = the number of students taking the test. In this try-out study, 27 out of 50 examinees answered the Mathematics Achievement Test (MAT) test items correctly. Using the formula above, Item Difficulty = 27/50, which is 0.54, which means that the test item was moderate. Regarding the Science Achievement Test (SAT) test items, 24 out of the 50 test-takers answered them correctly. Using the same formula, the item difficulty = 25/50 = 0.50, and 23 scored the English Achievement Test (EAT) test correctly. Using the same formula, the item difficulty for the English test was 0.46. Using the criteria of index of difficulty in table 3.6 below, the items of the three subjects were good. 64 Table 3.8 Criteria of Difficulty Index (P) P= Interpretation 0.00 – 0.30 Difficult Items 0.31 – 0.70 Moderate Items 0.71 – 1.00 Easy Items Source: Nitko (1983) 3.14.3. Discrimination Index According to Nunnaly (1964), discrimination index of an item indicates the extent to which the item distinguishes between the testees separating the more able testees from the less able. The index of discrimination (D) tells us whether students who do well on the entire test tend to do well or badly on each item of the test. In order to calculate the index discrimination, upper and lower groups were formed from the top 14 and bottom 14 test takers in MAT, SAT, and EAT test on the total score. These was done because Nitko(1983) proposed that upper and lower 27% with middle 46% could lead to optimal point when the total test scores are normally distributed. It was found that 13 of the test takers in the upper group and 7 in the lower group in MAT passed the item. The index discrimination was therefore calculated by the formula: D = Up – L p/U, where Up and Lp indicate the number of test takers in the upper and lower groups who pass the item, and U is the total number of test-takers in the upper group. Thus, D = 13 – 7/ 14 = 0.43. With regard to the SAT, 11 of the test-takers in the upper 65 group and 4 in the lower group passed the items. Using the same formula, D = 0.50. With EAT 10 in the upper group and 4 in the lower group passed the items, hence D = 0.43. The higher the discrimination index, the better the item can determine the difference between those with high test scores and those with low ones. As table 3.7 below indicates, Discrimination indexes of 0.43 and 0.50 were excellent. Table 3.9 Discrimination power of the answers according to their D value D= Quality Recommendations > 0.40 Excellent Retain 0.30 – 0.39 Good Possibilities for improvement 0.20 – 0.29 Mediocre Need to check/review 0.00 – 0.19 Poor Discard or review in depth < -0.01 Worst Definitely discard Source: Nitko (1983) 3.14.4. Validity Validity of a test determines whether or not the test is appropriate as the research instrument. In other words, validity of a test is the extent to which a test measures what it is supposed to measure. Since the tests were developed based on the course objectives, it can be said that the test has content validity 66 3.14.5. Reliability. This is the extent to which a test produces consistent result when administered under similar conditions (Nunnaly, 1964). The reliability of the test items was calculated using SPSS version 16.0, employing the cronbach alpha. The researcher inspected the corrected item-total correlations and the alpha if deleted statistics and found that none of the items when removed increases the overall alpha and so we have no cause for alarm regarding omitting an item. The reliability of the MAT was 0.78, 0.81 for EAT, and 0.88 for SAT. 3.15. Data Collection Procedure Students and the teacher assisting the researcher in the administration of the survey were identified. The approval to conduct the survey was received from the school administrators. The students respondents were seated and well spaced out to ensure that they worked independently. The researcher and his assistant briefed the students' respondents on how to respond to the items. The respondents also had the opportunity to ask questions or call the attention of the researcher to clarify difficulties they came across in the course of responding to the items. The researcher together with the assistant (teacher from the school) handed out the research instrument to the students respondents to respond to the items on their perceptions on interactions with their parents in the home environment from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). The administration of the instrument was carried out 67 during the morning hours and lasted for fifty minutes (50) minutes. After completion, the instrument were collected back and scored. In regards to the student’s academic performance, average academic performance in MAT, EAT and SAT scores were used. In order to achieve standardization of students’ academic performance across different classes and different schools, the researcher used the standard score (Z-SCORES) rather than actual academic performance of students. 3.16: Scoring the instrument The researcher used manual means to score the instrument. The total scores for each respondent were found. The instrument was used to group the students’ respondents into permissive, authoritarian and authoritative patterns of parenting and the type of their parents' involvement. 3.17. Procedure for Data Analysis Data analysis refers to the systematic organization and synthesis of research data, and the testing of research hypotheses (Burns and Grove, 2003). Data analysis gave meaning to the data collected during the research. The data generated was subjected to descriptive and inferential statistics. The inferential statistics used included two-way ANOVA analysis to test each of the stated hypotheses at 0.05 level of significance. Post hoc comparisons were conducted to investigate pair wise differences among parenting style and parental involvement group means. Multiple regression was used to investigate the contribution of parenting style and parental involvement on the student academic performance. In addition, chi-square test of independence was used to determine the 68 differences in parenting styles and parental involvement on the stated parents’ educational level or background and lastly independent t-test was conducted on the parents’ demographical variables of marital status and sex. The SPSS computer programme was used to analyze the responses gathered. 3.18. Ethical Considerations Having discussed the methodological aspects of the research, the researcher contemplated on ethical issues. This became evident since parenting styles and parental involvement describe parents moral, cultural, and social behaviours (Baumrind, 1991). Asking students to reveal their parents behaviours therefore raises important ethical considerations. The ethical issues the researcher considered were: maintaining the anonymity of respondents, protection from harm, data security, benefits to students, confidentiality, and consent form. 3.18.1. Maintaining the Anonymity of Respondents The first important ethical consideration the researcher considered was ‘’maintaining the anonymity of respondents’’. Providing anonymity of information collected from research participants means that either the project does not collect identifying information of individual subjects (e.g., name, address, Email address, etc.), or the project cannot link individual responses with participants’ identities. In this study, the respondents were given the opportunity to enter identifying name or code on all the test instruments or the research surveys, though they were requested not to use their real names. The purpose of this was to allow the researcher to identify the 69 respondents’ responses across all the tests and surveys. This was done to protect the identity of research respondents. Thus the data should be presented in such a way that respondents should be able to recognize themselves, while the reader should not be able to identify them. Personal anonymity may be central to gaining reliable information and that the issue of anonymity was dealt with when one respondent asked whether they had to give their names on the questionnaire. 3.18.2. Protection from Harm Ensuring the well-being of the respondents is of paramount importance in any study. In this study, two aspects of protecting respondents from harm were dealt with. The first was related to ensuring the information the respondents have entrusted to the researcher was dealt with in a sensitive way. All research practices pose potential risk or harm to participants. It is therefore rational that reasonable steps should be taken into account to protect students at all times from risks that are reasonably foreseeable including physical, psychological, emotional and other forms of harm. Accordingly, the researcher followed the following indicators to avoid practices which impose an undue risk of harm or unsafely. The data collection process is clearly relevant to the research purpose. Research will not cause undue stress, anxiety or raise any personal or upsetting issues. 70 Research does not involve any personal, sensitive or incriminating questions/topics which could place participants at risk. Statements and questions are not racist, sexist and not discriminatory of religious beliefs. 3.18.3. Data Security The researcher used the mechanism of de – identifying the data sets. The researcher allowed the respondents to use their own codes. Thus when responding, it may be assumed by the respondents that: (1) the researcher does not know their identity; and (2) that people other than the researcher would not be able to gain access to the information. 3.18.4. Confidentiality Maintaining confidentiality of information collected from research participants means that only the researcher can identify the responses of individual subjects; however, the researchers must make every effort to prevent anyone outside from connecting individual subjects with their responses. The use of study codes is an effective method for protecting the confidentiality of research participants. 3.18.5. Benefits to Respondents Given the personal investment by the respondents in the study, the researcher considered it good ethical practice to provide feedback regarding the findings of the study. This will enable the students to know the potentials of parenting styles and parental involvement on their academic performance. 71 3.18.6. Informed Consent Ethical research requires that research participants, to the degree that they are capable, be given the opportunity to consent to participating in the study. This is the ‘’informed consent’’. The students and their parents were informed about the research before becoming research participants. The parents were informed because the students were less than 18 years old. The informed consent process included the following two main components: 3.18.6.1 Information This included information about the research procedure, purpose, methods, demands, risks, inconveniences, discomforts, possible outcomes of the research and statements offering the respondents the opportunity to ask questions concerning the study. The researcher made sure that the potential research participant has comprehended the information before giving informed consent, has provided information in a way that allows time for consideration or questioning, has been presented the information in the preferred language, and made sure that it did not require high-level literacy skills. 3.18.6.2. Voluntary Consent The consent to participate in the study was made voluntary, without coercion, undue influence, or pressure. A consent letter was given to the students to be given to their parents to allow them to participate in the study (See Appendix E). 72 CHAPTER FOUR PRESENTATION OF DATA AND ANALYSIS OF RESULTS 4.1. Introduction The purpose of this study was twofold. The main purpose was to investigate the effects of parenting styles and parental involvement on the students’ academic performances. The second was to investigate whether there are differences in parenting styles and parental involvement vis – a - vis the demographic variables of the students’ parents’ educational background, marital status, and sex. The data was discussed in terms of the study’s hypotheses. It was predicted that: i. There will be statistically significance difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ parenting styles ii. There will be statistically significance difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ school involvement iii. There will be a statistically significant interaction effect between parenting styles and school involvement on academic performance. iv. There will be a significant contribution of parenting styles and parental involvement on the students academic performance v. There will be statistically significant differences in parenting styles and selected parents demographic variables of educational background, marital status, and sex The results of these predictions are presented in this section. This chapter is in two sections: A and B. Section A describes the demographic variables of the students’ parents 73 in terms of educational background, marital status, and sex. The results were presented in bar graphs. In section B, inferential statistics (i.e. two-way ANOVA, multiple regression, and ChiSquare test of independent were used to test the predictions stated above. SECTION A 4.2. Demography of the students’ parents This section describes the educational background, marital status, and the sex of the students’ parents. 4.2.1. Educational Background As can be seen in figure 4.1 below, 10.1 %( 21 parents had no high school education. Fifty (50) parents representing 24% completed high school. Again, 54 parents representing 26% had completed some college, with 83 parents representing 39% were graduates. These findings indicated that the majority of the parents of the students’ respondents were found to have completed university. 74 Figure 4.1 Educational background distributions of the respondents’ parents Note: EBG = Educational Background 4.2.2. Sex The sex distribution of the students parents in the study area indicated that 109 parents representing 52.4% were males, whiles 99 parents representing 47.6% were found to be females the figure 4.2 below illustrate this relationship. 75 Figure 4.2. Sex distributions of the students’ parents 4.2.3. Marital Status Figure 4.3 below indicates the various marital statuses of the parents. 61 parents representing 29.3% were found to be single, whiles 147 parents representing 70.7% were intact. It is therefore clear that the majority of the students’ respondents’ parents were intact. 76 Figure 4.3. Marital status distributions of the students’ parents Note: MST = Marital Status. SECTION B—INFERENTIAL STATISTICS 4.3. Testing of the Study Hypotheses In order for the researcher to be sure that the data meet the requirement for using the inferential statistics (i.e. parametric test), the researcher therefore checked whether the data collected was normally distributed. There are several ways in which you can use SPSS to assess the normality of a distribution (e.g. Q.-Q plot, scatterplot, linearity, komogorov-smirnov test, skewness and kurtosis etc). The researcher assessed the normality of the data using the z-score for skewness. Howell (2002) emphasized that if the study sample size is small (n < 100 in a correlational study or n < 50 in each group if comparing means) then calculate z-scores for skewness and reject as non-normal those variables with either z-score greater than an absolute value of -+1.96. If your sample is 77 of medium size (100 < n < 300 for correlational studies or 50 < n < 150 in each group if comparing means) then calculate z-scores for skewness and reject as non-normal those variables with either z-score greater than an absolute value of -+3.29. Thus, any variable having z-score for skewness greater than - +3.29 are considered abnormal. However, since the sample size for the study is 208, the z-score skewness of -+3.29 was used. The table 4.5 below indicates the descriptive of the three variables of parenting styles, parental involvement and academic performance. Table 4:1 Table indicating skewness and kurtosis values PS N Valid Missing PI ACP 208 208 208 0 0 0 Mean 2.2692 1.8750 63.7356 Median 2.0000 2.0000 65.0000 Mode 3.00 1.00 70.00 Std. Deviation 0.77060 0.83623 11.50519 Skewness -0.505 0.239 -0.164 Std Error of Skewness 0.169 0.169 0.169 Kurtosis -1.148 -1.531 -0.539 Std. Error of Kurtosis 0.336 0.336 0.336 Note: PS = Parenting Style, PI = Parental Involvement, and ACP = Academic Performance. Getting z-scores for skewness(Zskew) is simply the skewness divided by standard error skew. 78 Thus, Zskew = Skewness/Std.Error of Skewness = -0.505/0.169 = -2.98. This is the z-score for skewness for the data parenting styles Again, the skewness for the data parental involvement is: Thus, z-score for skewness = Skewness/Std.Error of Skewness =0.239/0.169 =1.41 Finally, the z-score for skewness for the academic performance is: Z skew = Skewness/Std.Error of Skewness = -0.164/0.169 = -0.97 The z-score for skewness values above suggesting that the data meet the requirement of the parametric test (i.e., normal distributed), therefore the study hypotheses could be tested. 4.4. Statistical Analysis A Two- Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) of Academic Performance was conducted. The table 4:4 below indicates the results of the full factorial analysis. 79 Table 4:2. Indicating the results of the full factorial analysis Source Corrected Model SS 8581.189 df MS F Sig. 8 1072.649 11.277 0.000 PS 3406.002 2 1703.001 17.903 0 .000 PI 3526.364 2 1763.182 18.536 0 .000 PS * PI 1169.748 4 292.437 3.074 O.017 Error 18929.191 199 95.122 Total 873473.000 208 Corrected Total 27510.380 207 a. R Squared = .312(Adjusted R Squared = .284) Figure 4.4 graphically present the two-way ANOVA design between parenting styles and parental involvement on academic performance. Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance indicated that homogeneous populations for parenting styles and parental involvement were assumed. The non-significant p-value is indicative of the homogeneity variance assumption being met. The table below indicates the Levene’s test of equality of error of variance. Table 4:3. Indicating the Levene’s test of Equality of Error Variance in Academic Performance F df1 df2 sig 1.590 8 199 0.279 80 From the table above, we can see that we have homogeneity of variance of the dependent variable (academic performance) across groups. We know this as the significant value greater than 0.05, which is the level we set for alpha. The main result of the full factorial analysis gives us important information. First, the table in this output (Table 4.2) gives us the values across the row headed Corrected Model. The important columns across this row are ones labeled F. and Sig. So with F of 11.277 with its significant p- value of .000 against the row headed Corrected Model tells us that, overall, there is a significant difference in the group mean academic performance. 4.4.1. Calculation of eta-squared from SPSS Output of two-way ANOVA SPSS does not provide the recommended eta-squared (2) as a measure of effect size for ANOVA effects (Howell, 2002). The eta-squared (2) can be calculated from the ANOVA table. The Factorial ANOVA conducted has two IVs and one DV: IV1 = Parenting Style (PS) IV2 = Parental Involvement (PI) DV = Academic Performance 2 = SSbetween / SStotal SST = 3406.002 + 3526.364+ 1169.748 +18929.191 = 27,031.31 2 for parenting style = 3406.002 /27.031.31 = 0.13 2 for parental involvement = 3526.364 / 27,031.31 = 0.13 81 2 for interaction 2 for error = 1169.748 / 27,031.31 = 0.04 = 18929.191 /27031.31 =0.70 The sum of the 2 is 1.00. In this study, the IVs explain 26% of the variance in academic performance. Therefore the strength of the relationship between parenting style and students academic performance, as assessed by eta-squared, was not quite strong, with parenting style accounting for 13% of the variance in student academic performance. 4.4.2. Hypothesis One The first hypothesis for the study was that there will be statistically significance differences in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ parenting styles The row of information across the variable parenting styles (see table 4:2) tells us that with an F of 17.903 with the probability value of 0.000 is highly significant. Thus, the variable parenting style has significant effect F (2,208) = 17.903, p = 0.000). Therefore, the prediction that there will be statistically significant difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ parenting styles is supported. Table 4.4 below illustrates the descriptive statistics of academic performance on parenting styles. 82 Table 4:4 Descriptive Statistics for Academic Performance (PS) Parenting styles Mean Authoritarian 57.205 10.4 Permissive 63.207 10.1 Authoritative Standard Deviation 68.249 10.0 Table 4.4 above shows significant differences in the means between the authoritarian (M=57.205, SD=10.4), and permissive (M = 63.207, SD = 10.1) parenting styles, authoritarian and authoritative (M = 68.249, SD=10.0) parenting styles, permissive and authoritative in terms of students academic performance with the parenting styles. Figure 4.4 below shows that when you ignore parental involvement the overall academic performance due to the three parenting styles are not similar (the means of these groups are not equal).This finding seems to indicate that students are likely to perform better when they are from authoritative home. 83 Figure 4.4: Showing the main effect of parenting style. Post hoc comparisons were conducted at the 0.05 alpha levels to investigate pair wise difference among the parenting style group means. Post hoc comparisons were conducted using the Tukey test, a test that assumes equal variances among the three groups. The result of the Post hoc analysis is presented in Table 4:5 below: 84 Table 4:5 Post Hoc Comparisons of Parenting Styles on Academic Performance Group comparison Confidence lower - Different between bound mean Confidence upperbound 1 -2 -11.15 -6.002* -1.96 1–3 -15.85 - 11.044* -7.14 2–3 -8.65 - 5.043* -1.25 *indicates that the mean difference is significant at the 0.05 alpha level Note 1=Authoritarian 2=Permissive 3= Authoritative Figure 4:5 above graphically presents the academic performance means of the three parenting styles. The result shows that students experiencing authoritative parenting had the highest level of academic performance. In addition, students experiencing the permissive type of parenting style showed the second highest means whiles students from authoritarian homes had the lowest level of academic performance with the parenting style relationship. These findings supported the prediction that parenting style related to academic performance of the students. 85 4.4.3 Hypothesis Two The second hypothesis for the study was that there will be statistically significant differences in students’ academic performance in the variation of parents’ school involvement Similarly, the values across the parental involvement (see Table 4.2 ) also tells us that parental involvement also had significant effect with an F of 18.536 with the probability value of 0.000. This means that there is significant difference between at least two of the parental involvement groups in terms of academic performance F (2,208) = 18.536; p = 0.000). Therefore, the prediction that there will be statistically significant differences in students’ academic performance in the variation of parents’ school involvement was supported. Post hoc comparisons were conducted at the 0.05 alpha levels, to investigate pair wise different as among the parental involvement group means. The strength of the relationship between parenting style and academic performance, as assessed by eta squared, was not strong, with parental involvement accounting for 13% of the variance in academic performance. Table 4.6 below illustrates the descriptive statistics of parental involvement on academic performance. 86 Table 4:6 Table indicating Descriptive Statistics for Academic Performance (PI) Parental involvement Mean Decision making 59.411 10.15 Communication 59.640 10.20 Learning at home 69.609 11.03 Note Standard Deviation 1= decision- making, 2 = communication, 3 = learning at home. Table 4.6 above indicates that there are significant difference in the means between the decision-making (M= 59.411, SD=10.15), and communication (M = 59.640, SD = 10.20) parents school involvement, decision – making and learning at home (M =69.609, SD=11.03) parental involvement, communication and learning at home is terms students academic performance with the parent school involvement. On the other hand figure 4.5 below shows that when you ignore parenting styles, the overall academic performance is very similar for decision making and communication (the means of these groups are approximately equal). The significant effect is shown between learning at home and communication and between learning at home and decision – making. This finding indicates that students are likely to perform better when their parents encourage them to learn at home. 87 Figure 4.5: Showing the main effect of parental Involvement. Post hoc comparison was conducted using the Tukey test, a test that assumes equal variance among the three groups. The result of the post hoc analysis is presented in Table 4.7 below: 88 Table 4.7. Post Hoc Comparisons of Parental Involvement on Academic Performance Group comparison Confident lower Different between Confident upper bound mean bound 1 -2 -4.54 0.23 3.32 1 -3 -13.87 -9.97* -5.96 2–3 -13.59 -10.198* -5.02 *indicates that the mean difference is significant at the 0.05 alpha level Note 1= decision- making, 2 = communication, 3 = learning at home. Figure 4:5 above graphically presents the academic performance means of the three parental involvements. The result shows that students experiencing learning at home had the highest level of academic performance followed by students experiencing communication, whiles students whose parents use decision – making the way of involving in their school had the lowest level of academic performance with the parental involvement relationship. These findings supported the prediction that parental involvement related to academic performance with the parental involvement relationship. 4.4.4 Hypothesis Three The third hypothesis for the study is that there will be an interaction effect of parenting styles and parental involvement on academic performance The information across the row labeled PS * PI (parenting style by parental involvement) also tells us that there is significant interaction (i.e. an effect that is produced by a combination of two or more variables). In this case, it is the effect of the interaction parenting style and parental involvement on academic performance (F= 3.074; 89 df = 206; p=0.017). This implies that the contribution of parenting styles to students’ academic performance depended on the levels of parents’ school involvement. Therefore, the interaction tells us that the main effect of parenting style (i.e. authoritative) depended on the type of parents’ school involvement. Figure 4.6 below clearly indicates the interaction effect. Figure 4:6 Showing the Main Effects and interaction Effects of Parenting Styles and Parental Involvement on Academic Performance Note: Academicperf = Academic Performance The footnote under the table also informs us that the researcher’s model explained 0.312(31%). The Adjusted R Squared is 0.284 of the variance in the academic performance. Figure 4.6 above graphically present the interaction effect of parenting 90 styles and parental involvement on academic performance. When the means are graphed, the interaction is visible. According to Howell (2002), the interaction effect is recognized when the lines cross or flare like a trumpet. Figure 4.6 indicates that decision making and communication interact on parenting style levels of authoritarian and permissiveness. As the point of the interaction indicates, the characteristics of permissiveness dominate. 4.4.5. Hypothesis four The fourth hypothesis for the study is that there will be a significant effect of parenting styles and parental involvement on academic performance of students. Regression analyses were performed to assess the effects of parenting styles and parental involvement of academic performance of S.H.S students in the study area using SPSS version 16.0. The results of the regression were represented in the Table 4:8 below: Table 4:8 Forced entry regression of Academic Performance on Parenting Style and Parental Involvement. (208) Variable b Beta R R2 t Sig(p) Step 1 Constant 44.470 Parenting Style 5.045 Parental Involvement 4.341 17. 838 0.348 0.339 0.49 91 0.24 0.000 5. 696 0.000 5.815 0.000 Table 4.8 displays unstandardized (b) and standardized (beta) regression coefficients, the multiple regression coefficients, adjusted R2, t and its associated p – value for each variable that entered into the equation. As shown in Table 4.8 above, parenting styles and parental involvement collectively explained 24 %( adjusted R2 = 0.24) of the variance in academic performance. Based on the order of entry chosen for the present sample, it indicates that parenting styles explained most of the variance in the academic performance of the students (Beta=0.348, t=5.696, p<0.000) and portrayed as best contributor of academic performance. Also as Table 4:8 indicates, the contribution of parental involvement to the variance in academic performance was also significant (Beta=0.339, t=5.815, p<0.000). It is also a good predictor but not as the parenting styles. This interpretation is in order as there appeared to be a non-variation of the assumption of multicolinearity (a term used when there is difficulty in explaining which construct is actually making the impact) since we can differentiate the impact in the relationship between parenting styles and parental involvement. In summary, it could be noted that parenting styles emerged as the best contributor due to the extent it impacts students’ academic performance. In general, the model predicts the students’ academic performance in the equation that emerges from the model. The equation of a multiple regression is: Y = a +b1X1 + b2X2, Where: Y is the value of the dependent variable, what is being predicted or explained. 92 a is the constant, b1 is the slope(Beta coefficient) for X1, X1 is the first independent variable. In this study, it is parenting styles explaining the variance in academic performance. As shown in the Table 4:9, the B values have the regression constant of 44.47 which is `a` and our b1 and b2 were partial regression coefficient of parenting styles (5.05), and parental involvement(4.34)b2 is the slope(Beta Coefficient) for X2(parental involvement), X2 is the second independent variable that is explaining the variance in Y(academic performance). From these, academic performance=44.47+5.05X1+4.34X2. Therefore, a student who is experiencing authoritative type (3), and communication (2) would expect to have academic performance of: Academic performance=44.47+5.05(3) +4.34(2) = 44.47+15.15+8.68 = 68.3 The outcome given indicates that our model is a good predictor of students’ academic performance. 4.4.6 Hypothesis five The fifth hypothesis for the study is that there will be statistically significant differences in parenting styles and selected parents’ demographic variables of educational background, marital status, and sex. 4.4.6.1. Hypothesis 5(a)—Differences in Parenting Styles by Marital Status. Independent t - test was used to analyze the data with the parenting styles as one variable (dependent variable) and the marital status as the second variable (independent variable). The table below presents the results of the analysis. 93 Table 4:9 Independent sample t-test of Parenting Styles by Marital Status Marital Status M SD N Single 1.98 0.79 61 Intact 2.73 0.74 147 t -3.397 df Sig. 206 0.001 As can be seen in the Table 4.9 above, the results of the analysis indicates that there was statistically significant difference in the mean between the two groups (t = -3.397, df =206, p = 0.001). Therefore, the study hypothesis that there will be statistically significant differences in parenting styles and parents marital status was supported. Closer examination of Table 4.110 below indicates that we tended to get larger number of authoritative parents in intact family (78, 53.06%), followed by permissive (46, 31.29%), and authoritarian (23, 15.64%). On the single home, permissive parenting style was the highest (24, 39.34%), followed by authoritarian, (19, 31.15%), and the authoritative being the last, (18, 29.51%).Thus, substantial differences is observed with regard to the parenting styles. Thus, the percentage of parenting styles in intact families and single homes tend to differ significantly. 94 Table 4.10 Showing Parenting Style Differences on Marital Status Variables Single % Intact % Total % Authoritative 18 29.51 78 53.06 96 46.15 Permissive 24 39.34 46 31.29 71 34.14 Authoritarian 19 31.15 23 15.64 41 19.71 Total 61 100 147 100 208 100.00 4.4.6.2. Hypothesis 5(b) -- Differences in Parenting Styles by Sex The hypothesis 5(b) for the study was that, there will be a significant difference between parenting styles and sex. Table 4.11 below presents the parenting styles distribution on sex. Table 4.11 Indicating Parenting Style Differences in Sex Male % Female % Total % Authoritative 52 47.71 44 45.45 96 46.15 Permissive 39 35.78 31 31.31 71 34.14 Authoritarian 18 16.51 24 24.24 41 19.71 Total 109 100.00 99 100.00 208 100.00 The closer examination of Table 4.11 above reveals the percentage of authoritative in males (52,47.71%), permissive(39,35.78%), and authoritarian(18,16.51%) is not significantly different from the females, permissive(31,30.31%), and authoritarian(24,24.24%). 95 authoritative (44,45.45%), Independence t –test was used to analyze the data with parenting styles as one variable and sex as second variable. Table 4.12 below presents the results of the analysis. Table 4:12 Independent sample t-test of Parenting Styles by Sex Sex M SD N t df Sig. Male 2.21 0.74 109 1.023 206 0.154 Female 2.20 0.81 99 As can be seen from the Table 4.12 above, parenting styles employed in sex is independent. This means that the proportion in both groups is close to the same size. There was no significant effect (t = 1.023, df = 206, p=0.15), such that males and females did not differ in their parenting styles. 4.4.6.3. Hypothesis 5(c) -- Differences in Parenting Styles by Educational Background The hypothesis of 5(c) is that, there will be a significant difference between parenting styles and educational background. Table 4.13 below presents parenting style distribution on educational background. 96 Table 4.13 Indicating Parenting Style Differences by Educational Background No High High School School. % Completed Authoritative 6 28.6 10 Permissive 19.0 24 Authoritarian 11 52.4 16 Total 50 4 21 100 % Some College % Graduate % Total % 20 27 48 50 53 63.9 96 46.15 21 38.9 21 25.3 71 34.14 32 6 11.1 9 10.8 41 19.71 100 54 100 83 100 208 100 A chi-square test of independent was used to analyze the data with parenting style as one variable and educational background as the second variable. There was a significant effect,X2 (6, N=208) =40.73, p=0.000. Table 4.13 above presents that graduate parents employed in highest authoritative type (53, 63.9%), followed by the parents who have completed some college (27, 50%), high school completed (10, 20%), and parents with no high school completed being the last (6, 28.6%). The parents who have completed high school were highest in employing permissive type (24, 48%), followed by some college and graduate (22, 40.7%) respectively, and the parents with no high school education (4, 19%) were the least. With regard to the authoritarian type, parents with high school education were the highest (16, 32%), followed by parents with no high school education (11, 52.4%), graduates parents (9, 10.8%), and parents who had completed some college (6, 11.1%) were the least. The data are graphed in the figure 4.5 below: 97 Figure 4:7. Parenting Styles Differences on Respondents Parents Educational Background Chi-square post hoc test were conducted to determine which of the cell responsible for the differences at alpha level of 0.05.The residual, or the differences between the observed frequencies and the expected frequencies were found. These values were converted to z-score and then compared to the critical value equivalent to the alpha for the problem. The SPSS output shows the standardized residuals (converted to a z-score) computed for each cell. It does not produce the probability or significance. Since the hypothesis was a two-tailed test, the significance level is 0.05, and the critical value for which the standardized residual will be compared will be from -1.9 to +1.9(i.e., 1-.05 = 0.95/2=0.475). Checking 0.475 on the z-score gives the value 1.9 and it is found under 98 0.06 (see Appendix F). The researcher therefore added 0.06 and 1.9 to get 1.96(looking for both left hand and a right hand tail = +1.96 to -1.96). The results of the chi-square post hoc analysis are presented in the Table 4.14 below: Table 4.14 Indicating Standardized Residuals of Parenting Style * Educational Background Crosstabulation Educational Background Parenting Style No High School High School Some College Completed Completed Graduate Authoritative -1.2 -2.7 0.4 2.4 Permissive -1.2 1.7 0.7 -1.3 Authoritarian 3.3 1.9 -1.5 -1.9 Using the standardized residuals, we would find that cells highlighted were responsible for the difference. Thus, authoritative style on high school completed (-2.7), authoritative on graduate (2.4), authoritarian style on no high school (3.3), were responsible for the difference. This is because these values are lower and higher than the critical z-value at 0.05 alpha level (-1.96 and +1.96). Another characteristics of standardized residual was that the negative indicates that those cells were under-represented in the actual sample (i.e., there were more subjects in this category as expected), thus, authoritative on high school completed subjects were under99 represented. On the other hand, authoritative on graduate, authoritarian on no high school completed and authoritarian on high school completed were over-represented in the actual sample (i.e., there were few respondents in this category that the researcher expected). 4.4.7 Hypothesis six. The sixth hypothesis for the study is that there will be statistically significant differences in parents’ school involvement and selected parents’ demographic variables of educational background, marital status, and sex 4.4.7.1. Hypothesis 6(a)—Differences in Parental Involvement by Marital Status To test this hypothesis, Independence t –test was used to analyze the data with parental involvement (decision making, communication, and learning at home) as one variable and marital status (intact, and single) as the second variable. Table 4.15 below presents the results of the analysis. Table 4.15 Independent sample t-test of Parental Involvement by Marital Status Marital Status M SD N Single 1.77 0.87 147 Intact 1.96 0.78 t 1.700 df Sig. 206 0.04 61 As can be seen in the Table 4.15 above, the results of the analysis indicates that there was statistically significant difference in the mean between the two groups (t = -3.397, df =206, p = 0.04). Therefore, the study hypothesis that there will be significant differences in parenting styles and marital status was supported. 100 The result also indicates that intact homes tended to be high in employing all the levels of parental involvement. A closer examination of the Table 4:16 below indicate the percentage differences. It shows that on intact parents, decision making was the highest (61, 41.50%), followed by learning at home (47, 31.97%), and lastly communication (39, 26.53%). With the single home, decision making also was the highest (26, 46.62%), followed by communication (22, 30.07%), and lastly learning at home 13, 21.31%). Table 4.16 Parental Involvement Differences by Marital Status Single % Intact Decision-Making 26 46.62 Communication 22 30.07 39 26.53 61 29.33 Learning at Home 13 21.31 47 31.97 60 29.33 Total 61 100.00 147 100.00 208 100.00 61 % Total % 41.50 87 41.34 4.4.7.2 Hypothesis 6(b)—Differences in Parental Involvement by Sex To test this hypothesis, independence t-test was used to analyze the data with parental involvement (decision making, communication, and learning at home) as one variable and marital status (male and female) as the second variable. Table 4.17 below presents the results of the analysis. 101 Table 4.17 Independent sample t-test of Parental Involvement by Sex Sex M SD N Male 1.90 0.78 109 Female 1.79 0.85 99 t -0.929 df Sig. 206 0.354 As can be seen in the Table 4.17 above, the results of the analysis indicates that there was no statistically significant difference in the mean between the two groups (t = -0.929, df =206, p = 0.354). Therefore, the study hypothesis that there will be significant differences between sex by parental involvement was not supported. A closer examination of the Table 4:18 below indicate the percentage differences. It shows that males and females on decision making had nearly the same parents, followed by learning at home, the males were 39 representing 35.78%, whiles the females were 21 representing 21.21%. On communication, the females were second (i.e.34 representing 34.34%), whiles they were last on learning at home (21, 21.21%). Table 4.18 Parental Involvement Differences on Sex Male % Female % Total % Decision-Making 43 39.45 44 44.45 87 41.83 Communication 27 24.77 34 34.34 61 29.32 Learning at Home 39 35.78 21 21.21 60 28.85 Total 109 100 99 102 100 208 100 4.4.7.3 Hypothesis 6(c) -- Differences in Parental Involvement by Educational Background The hypothesis of 5(c) is that, there will be a significant difference between parental involvements and the parents’ educational background. Table 4.19 below presents parental involvement distribution on educational background. Table 4.19 Parental Involvement Differences by Educational Background No High High School School. % Completed Decision making Some % College % Graduate % 16 76.2 21 42 26 48.1 4 19.0 20 40 19 35.2 1 4.8 9 18 9 21 100 24 Total % 28.9 86 20.5 61 41.34 Communication 17 29.33 Learning at Home 16.7 42 83 100 50.6 61 29.33 Total 50 100 54 100 208 100 A chi-square test of independent was used to analyze the data with parental involvement as one variable and the parents’ educational background as the second variable. There was a significant effect,[X2 (6, N=208) =33.81, p=0.000]. Table 4.19 above presents that graduate parents employed highest in learning at home only (42, 50.6%), whiles parents who have completed some college and high school were the second (9, 16.7% and 9, 18%) respectively, and parents no high school completed were the least (1, 4.8%). On decision making, parents who have completed some college were the highest (26, 48.1%), graduates parents were second in decision making (24, 28.92%) and parents who have had high school education 103 reported third in decision making (21,42%). With regard to communication parents who have had high school education were the highest (20, 40%), whiles parents who have completed some college were the second (19, 35.2%), graduates parents were the third (17, 20.5%). Parents who had no high school (4, 19%) were the last in all of the levels of parental involvement. The data are graphed in the figure 4.6 below: Figure 4:8: Parental Involvement Differences on Educational Background Note: = PI = Parental Involvement Chi-square post hoc test were conducted to determine which of the cells were responsible for the differences at alpha level of 0.05. The residual or the differences between the observed frequencies and the expected frequencies were found. These values were converted to z-score and then compared to the critical value equivalent to the alpha for the problem. 104 The SPSS output shows the standardized residuals (converted to a z-score) computed for each cell. It does not produce the probability or significance. As stated earlier, the critical value with the p-value of 0.05, and two=tailed hypothesis ranges from -1.96 to + 1.96. The results of the chi-square post hoc analysis are presented in the Table 4.20 below: Table 4.20 Indicating Standardized Residual of Parental Involvement * Educational Background Crosstabulation Educational Background Parental Involvement No High School High School Some College Graduate Completed Completed Decision Making 2.4 0.01 0.7 -1.8 Communication -0.9 1.4 0.8 -1.3 Learning at Home -2.1 -1.4 -1.7 3.5 Using the standardized residuals, we find that cells highlighted were responsible for the difference. Thus, learning at home on no high school completed (-2.1) and on graduate were responsible for the difference. This is because these values are lower and higher than the critical z-value at 0.05 alpha level (i.e. -1.96 and +1.96). Another characteristics of standardized residual was that the negative indicates that those cells were under-represented in the actual sample (i.e., there were more subjects in this 105 category as expected), thus, learning at home on no high school completed subjects were under- represented, whiles on learning at home on graduate was over-represented in the actual sample (i.e., there were few respondents in this category that the researcher expected). 106 CHAPTER FIVE SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, RECOMMENDATIONS AND CONCLUSION 5.1. Summary of the Study The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of parenting styles and parental involvement on academic performance. The study focused on cross-sectional design and descriptive survey using the quantitative model. Two sets of instruments were used. The first was Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) developed by Buri (1991), which was to assess the various types of the students’ parents parenting styles. The second instrument was the Parental Involvement Questionnaire (PIQ) developed by the researcher to assess the parents’ school involvement, preferably, decision making, communication, and learning at home and compared to average seven terms of the students’ academic performance. Data were analyzed by the use of descriptive statistics, One – Way ANOVE, Two-Way ANOVA, multiple regression, independent sample t-test and chi-square test of independent. The alpha level was set at 0.05 The sample for the study was 208 from the third year class of the two main senior high schools at the study area namely; Dwamena Akenten Senior High School and Namong Senior High Technical School. Thirteen students were randomly selected from each third year class of the mentioned schools. 107 The following section begins with a discussion of the findings, and continues with theoretical, educational, and counselling implications. The chapter concludes with recommendations and offer suggestions for future research. 5.2. Discussion of Findings 5.2.1. Hypothesis One – Parenting Styles and Academic Performance A significant difference was found between the parenting style group means on the dependent variable (academic performance), which supports the contention that parenting style is related to students academic performance. The study revealed that students experiencing the authoritative parenting style reported the highest levels of academic performance, and students who had experienced permissive style of parenting reported second highest level of academic performance. The study further indicated that students who had experienced authoritarian style of parenting reported the lowest level of academic performance. These findings are interesting for two reasons. First, both the authoritative and permissive parenting styles are high on the dimension of responsiveness (see page 6). According to Baumrind (1991) responsiveness includes both cognitive and emotional components. The cognitive component refers to encouraging the child to express his or her thoughts and opinions, and frequently takes place in the context of rule making and enforcement of discipline. The emotional component, affective warmth, does not imply only unconditional acceptance, but is instead characterized by expressions of both warmth and anger. Unconditional acceptance, `` noncontingent positive reinforcement``, and 108 ``noncontingent rejection`` are all forms of responsiveness, but are not beneficial because they lead the child to believe that his or her behavior has no effect on others. In other words, if a child’s actions are continuously approved, or disapproved, he or she forms the opinion that no matter what he or she does, the response from the parent will be the same. The child then generalizes this supposition to the larger society. With a lack of understanding of behavioral causality, Baumrind (1991) suggests that the child’s motivation will be damaged and he or she will be unlikely to try harder when confronted with a problem concerning achievement of a goal. Conditional acceptance, then allows the child to learn coping skills. For example, when the child’s actions elicit disapproval from the parent, the child then either learns ways to accept the disapproval without suffering damage to his or her sense of self-worth (Baumrind, 1991). Parents who exhibited responsiveness as behaviors were found by Romberg (1993) to be desirable among students. To Romberg (1993) students described favored parent behaviors such as being available, dependable, approachable, listening, and being responsive. These behaviors are intuitively components of a propensity for warmth and sensitivity and certainly indicate a willingness to participate in the parenting relationship. Secondly, the sample size of 208 was used to examine the differences between the three parenting styles. The data revealed that parenting styles accounted for over 45% of the variation in the students’ academic performance with the parenting relationship. Heinrich’s (1991) observed that students frequently find parenting relationship to be a disappointment and attributed these disappointment to the parents behavior. Baumrind (1991) emphasized that if parental involvement and commitment are manifested only by 109 demands (e.g. authoritarian parents) and attempts to shape the child’s behavior, the child will seek to escape rather to engage the caretaker. Therefore, conditional acceptance should be balanced by expressions of conditional affection. The findings in this study support this even further, indicating that parenting styles of parent is an important contributor to the students’ academic success or failures. 5.2.2. Hypothesis Two- Parental Involvement and Academic Performance The hypothesis two states that there will be statistically significance difference in students’ academic performance and variation of their parents’ school involvement. In other words, there will be main effect - a difference in the group means for the constructs of the parental involvement. The analysis indicated that parental involvement had a statistically significant impact on the students’ academic performance. Parents’ school involvement was measured using three variables of decision making, communication, and learning at home, all are related to academic performance. The data revealed that students whose parents are involved in their learning at home reported the highest level of academic performance. This support the previous research that has examined parental involvement that youth who experienced learning at home had the highest grades in schools (Epstein, 2001, Goldstein, 1984). Students of parents who employ communication as a way of school involvement reported the second highest level of academic performance, while the students who had experienced decision-making reported the lowest level of academic performance. The group differences were evident, 110 however, when the mean of learning at home group was compared with the means of communication and decision-making. One needs to understand socioeconomic and educational factors of the students’ home in order to understand parent-school relationships (Epstein, 2001). A situation when the parents have the lowest home ownership rate, receive less preventive care, and have higher rates of illness, those parents school involvement will be rare in all components. It is within these situations that students are expected to learn and have high expectations. In addition, it is within these contexts that parents are supposed to be involved their children’s education. In reality, poor socioeconomic conditions result in disorganized homes, erratic parental supervision and limited involvement in schools. Just as homes with families who work hard try to improve themselves and their children academic achievements. Although material resources might be scarce and social conditioning might be poor, some parents have strong value for education. Apart from the parent value for education, Georgiou (1997) suggests that other behaviors that are not directly related to school should be incorporated in the construct of parental involvement. Among them are; limit TV watching time, following specific set rules to discipline the child, limiting the amount of time for going out with friends and so on. Although not identical with parental involvement, Steinberg et al (1992) associated more general parenting behaviors (i.e. authoritative parenting) with highly involved parent. There was no significant difference of the students’ respondents who had experienced communication (i.e. 60 respondents) and students’ who also experienced learning at 111 home (i.e. 61), but the students who reported experiencing decision-making were 87. Students whose parents’ school involvement is learning activities with the students at home enable the students to tend to develop a sense of purpose and cooperativeness (Muller, 1995). He further emphasized that such parents were more proactive, preventing problems before they occur. Additionally, while the researcher can conclude that students’ academic work improves when they conduct interactive homework with parents or family members, he cannot conclude that other constructs- communication and decision making have negative effects since there are other factors such as family relations, personality, cultural and language that influence academic behavior in the schools (Epstein, 1991) 5.2.3. Hypothesis Three- The Interaction Effect between Parenting Style and Parental Involvement. A two – way between- subject ANOVA was conducted with academic performance as dependent variable and parenting styles (authoritative, permissive, and authoritative) and parental involvement (decision making, communication, and learning at home) as independent variables. The analyses revealed a significant interaction effect which indicates that the mean of sample of parenting style depended on the parental involvement. A statistically significant interaction implies that the main effects of parenting style and parental involvement are not directly interpretable. This finding supported the earlier research that for authoritative parents who are not involved with the school, the outcomes for the students are not clearly positive. On the 112 other hand, students whose parents are highly involved with the school but not authoritative show less optimal outcomes. It is the combination of an authoritativeness and school involvement that is associated with the best academic performance (Steinberg et al, 1994). Figure 4.6 explains the following: Irrespective of parenting styles, learning at home has more influence on the students’ academic performance than communication and decision-making. In other words, parenting styles do not have much influence on students’ academic performance with regard to learning at home. As figure 4.6 shows, the academic performance of students experiencing learning at home were quite stable across the three parenting styles (nearlyhorizontal line). Under the authoritarian type of parenting, decision-making has more influence of students’ academic performance than communication. The probable explanation might be that when parents who are autocrats allowed negotiations with their children, take part in school leadership (e.g. PTA chairperson). This may encourage the child to be conscious of themselves to study hard. The low influence of communication on academic performance did not come as surprise. This may be explained to be that, looking at the nature of authoritarian parents (i.e., rigid, strict, dictatorial, totalitarian etc), and the explanation of communication – home – to – school, and school – to – home on the child progress did not auger well for the students studies. Under the permissive parenting style, the decision-making and communication interacted to influence students’ academic performance. Baumrind (1991) explained permissive parents as lenient, liberal, lax and hands-off. With these qualities, it was not surprise that 113 communication and decision-making interacted to influence the students’ academic performance as Baumrind (1991) referred permissive parents as non- directive parents. However, under the authoritative parenting style, the result revealed that communication influenced students’ academic performance than decision-making. This finding is consistent with the study of Epstein (2001) that found that the authoritative parents attitude of give – and – take with their children encourage the children to be high in self awareness of social responsibility through open discussions which assist them in their academic endeavour. In addition previous research supports this trend that authoritative parenting due to its investment in parental monitoring while simultaneously engaging in emotional support of the child has the most beneficial effect for youth (Baumrind,1991, Epstein,2001, Steinberg et al, 1994). In summary, the results of this study support the decision to adapt the parent- child interaction model on the Baumrind (1966, 1991) patterns of the parent – student relationship. From the results, it is evident that parenting style measured along the dimensions of; authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive is a practical method of evaluating the effectiveness of the parenting relationship on student academic performance. 5.2.4. Hypothesis four—Effect of Parenting Style and Parental Involvement on Academic Performance. Hypothesis four states that there will be a significant effect of parenting style and parental involvement on the academic performance of students. Multiple regression procedure 114 was performed with academic performance as the dependent variable with both parenting style and parental involvement entered simultaneously as independent variables. According to Howell (2002), a large value of the standardized Beta indicates that a unit change in this predictor variable has a large effect on the criterion variable. The t and significant value (p) give a rough indication of the impact of each predictor variable – a big absolute t-value and small p-value suggest that a predictor variable is having a large impact on the dependent variable. The result of the regression analysis examining the effect of parenting styles and parental involvement on academic performance suggest that parenting style contribute more (Beta= 0.348,t=5.70, p,0.001) – the direct effect of the parenting style on academic performance removing the effect of parental involvement than the parental involvement. On the other hand, the effect of parental involvement on academic performance holding parenting styles constant was (i.e. Beta=0.339, t=5.815, p=0<0.001). The results of regression analysis examining the significant effect of parenting style and parental involvement on the students academic performance was found that parental involvement is a good mediator. This finding indicates that parenting style relate with parental involvement to produce best academic performance. This relationship was in the direction expected by the researcher and implicated by previous studies. This finding is particularly important because combining both factors contributes higher academic performance. This may imply that researchers will stand to greater ability to predict students’ academic performance and perhaps better understand how to prevent poor academic performance of students. 115 The results also revealed that parental involvement appear to mediate the association between parenting style and academic performance of students. A logical explanation for this finding is that if parents are utilizing their parenting style, they appear most likely involving in their children schooling and these combination yielded better academic performance of students. This research matches Lamborn et al (1991) that the combination of parenting style and parental involvement offer a clear prediction of academic performance of students. As mentioned earlier, parenting style such as authoritative is high in responsiveness, and make the children from that home are characterized by self-confident, ability to master tasks, well developed emotion regulation, lively and happy disposition etc. Lamborn et al (1991) emphasized that these behaviours are also characteristics of students whose parents involved in their school. 5.2.5. Hypothesis five - Differences in Parenting Style by Students Parents Demographic Variables (Marital Status, Sex, and Educational Background) 5.2.5.1 Hypothesis five (a)—Differences in Parenting Styles by Marital Status In general, the present study revealed that the parenting style employed in a family is dependent, at least in part, on whether the family is intact or not. More specifically, more parents from intact homes than from single homes are authoritative. This result is in line with general expectation. That is, all other things being equal, parents in intact families tend to be more supportive, more loving, and more controlling and demanding than parents in single homes. It is also consistent with results of other studies (e.g., Dornbusch 116 et.al. 1987). Particularly the result, which indicated that five times as many parents in single homes as those in intact homes are authoritative, is of importance here. Actually, this finding is consistent with findings from other studies (e.g., Dornbusch et.al, (1987). Dornbusch and his associates (1987) noted, among others, that both step-parent and single-parent families showed a higher level of permissive parenting than did two natural parents. This reflected in the study that permissiveness was the highest among the single parents. One possible explanation for this finding may exist in the culture in the study area. For instance, when a child loses his/her father or mother because of divorce or particularly because of death, the remaining parent considers the child as lonely and helpless. Thus, the parent believes that controlling the child and demanding from him/her, is tantamount to further victimizing, the child hence, parents in such conditions are more likely to be warm and accepting and less likely to be controlling and demanding. In other words, more parents in single homes than those in intact homes tend to be permissive. Overall, from the study, one can see that relatively more parents from intact homes than those from single homes tend to be authoritative followed by permissive and authoritarian being the least. Nevertheless, nearly equal number of parents from both intact and broken homes is authoritarian. We can, therefore, observe the relative advantages of intact homes over single homes. 5.2.5.2 Hypothesis five (b) - Differences in Parenting Style by Sex There is continuous debate regarding the role sex play in relation to parenting. Results from this study stood neutral and thus the issue of sex continuous to be of interest. 117 Findings of the study indicated that parenting styles did not differ as a function of the sex of parents. The data revealed, for example, that 46.79% of the students have male authoritative parents. In contrast, only 45.45% of the students also reported that their parents were female authoritative. This simply means that more male parents are warm, loving, controlling, and demanding toward their students. On the other hand, the results indicated that females are more authoritarian as compared to the males. This also means that female parents in the study area are less acceptant, more demanding, and more hostile toward their children. This finding is consistent with findings of other studies. Maccoby and Martin (l983), for example, indicated that male parents are more accepting and nonpunutive, making few demands on their children than females. In addition, Maccoby and Martin (1983) suggested that male parents are more warm and concerned about their children well-being. 5.2.5.3. Hypothesis five(c) – Differences in Parenting Style by Educational Backgrounds One of the purposes of the study was to examine parenting styles differences among the students parents educational background. The result indicated that each parenting style is exercised in each of the parents’ educational background. These suggest that the parenting style employed by a parent depended on the parent’s educational background. This finding is consistent with the result of other studies of Reigness and Gander (1974). Another point of importance in the study was related to identifying the predominant parenting style practiced among the parents of the four educational backgrounds in question. The study revealed that authoritative parenting is the most dominantly practiced style among the parents who had completed some college, and those who were graduates. 118 One possible explanation for the predominance of the authoritative style of parenting among the parents completed some college, and the graduates could be due the parental education they had. Another possible and more appealing explanation could be the sociocultural change in their lives. The socio-cultural changes have strong impact on the traditional model of child-rearing (Baumrind, 1971). The increasing awareness of parents about the influence of child rearing methods on children's all aspects of development could also be taken as an important explanation. Here, the role of the mass media is paramount. The information regarding child rearing methods and associated outcomes transmitted through mass media may have helped parents question their ways of upbringing and may have assisted them understand the negative consequences of traditional values, beliefs, and practices in child rearing on all round development of children. This, in turn, may have provided a context in which authoritative parenting become more prevalent. 5.2.6 Hypothesis six - Differences in Parental Involvement by Students Parents Demographic Variables (Marital Status, Sex, and Educational Background) 5.2.6.1. Hypothesis six (a)—Differences in Parental Involvement by Marital Status Marital status was measured using single and intact homes and all found related to the type of parents school involvement. There was a significant difference in the marital status groups. The group differences were evident when compared with the percentages of the group. It was found that intact parents were involved in learning at home more than those from single homes and students from these intact homes perform better as compared to other types. Regardless, parents from intact homes, they were the highest in 119 all the three levels of the parental involvement when compared to that of the parents from single homes. These findings complement that of Christenson &Sheridan (2001) research. They emphasized that families are essential, not just desirable to educational success of their students. They further identified that intact families support their children to learn at home strongly than the single homes because the former, focuses on relationship, creates a vehicle to look at the bigger picture about student learning, share information and resources, and establish meaningful roles for their children than single parents. As a result, students from intact homes have beneficial effect than those from single homes in terms of both academic performance and disciplinary incidents This finding could be explained that culture and family characteristics influence the degree of parental involvement, and adequately impact on students’ academic performance. Lower income families and single homes are less supportive of their children in their learning at home because of financial problems (Baumrind.1971). Finn (1988) supported that parents’ involvement at home influences students’ academic performance strongly. He identified three types of parents involvement at home that are consistently related to students academic performance as; organizing and monitoring children’s time especially relating to television viewing, helping with homework, and discussing school-related issues with children. 120 5.2.6.2. Hypothesis six (b)—Differences in Parental Involvement by Sex. In the area of sex, no significant difference was found. This could be observed when the group percentages are compared. The finding shows that the type of parents school involvement did not depend on sex. The finding supports that of Amato and Gilbreth (1999) research that females and males employed parental involvement equally. This could be explained that since education is the key to development, males and females are more equally serious in their students schooling for better results. Epstein (2001) redefined homework to mean not only work done, but also interactive activities shared with children at home, linking schoolwork to real life. This, she emphasized that females are consistent in training with their children. This situation of helping children at home, mean listening,reacting,praising,guiding, monitoring, and discussing—not ``teaching`` school subjects. She further stressed that, this style enable the students to develop positive attitude toward schoolwork or learning, gain in skills, abilities, and test scores linked to homework and class work, and finally complete homework. 5.2.6.3 Hypothesis six (c)—Differences in Parental Involvement by Educational Background. Educational background of the students parents were measured on; no high school completed, high school completed some college completed, and graduate and all were found related to type of parental involvement the students are experiencing. There were significant differences in the educational background groups. These differences were 121 evident when the percentages of the educational background group were compared. These suggest that the parental involvement employed by a parent depended on the parent’s educational background. The findings seem to suggest that some parents are more proactive on their school involvements and work to avoid a decrease in their students’ academic performance scores. However, the differences in parental ability (e.g. parents’ education level) and available resources, such as time (Muller, 1995) to help children, may temper with this approach. This would account for the differences in the parental involvement among the educational background group when the parents’ education is considered. McNeal (1991) indicates that because school-home communication and levels of parents’ involvement vary on income and educational level, some group of parents may feel more comfortable communicating or getting involved than others. Parents helping their students with homework or checking homework all depend on the parents understanding of education. Wang and Wildman (1994) emphasized that parents checking and helping their children homework will yield negative results if the parents had no or little insight in what they are helping the children with. All these might be the reasons for the differences. 122 5.3. Summary of Findings 1. Students from authoritative home performed better than their counterparts from other homes (i.e. permissive and authoritarian homes) 2. The students whose parents employ learning at home as a way of involving themselves in the child schooling performed better than those parents who use communication and decision making as the way of involving in the child schooling. 3. Irrespective of parenting styles learning at home influence the students’ academic performance. 4. Communication and decision making interacted to influence students’ academic performance 5. Parenting styles was found to be a better predictor of students’ academic performance than parental involvement. Moreover, children of parents who use both parenting styles and parental involvement tend to perform better than those parents who use either parenting styles or parental involvement. 6. It was found that parents of single home use more permissive style of parenting while those from intact home use authoritative style of parenting. 7. There was no gender difference with regard to parenting styles and parental involvement. 8. Graduate parents use more authoritative parenting than other parents of different educational status. 123 9. Graduate parents encourage their children to learn at home while parents with no high school education do not. 5.4. Conclusion This study attempted to offer a model of parenting style and parental involvement that will be useful for systematic evaluation of academic performance of students. A two-way ANOVA analysis was used to test the conceptual model of parenting style and parental involvement of the students’ academic performance in order to explain the academic performance of the students. The results suggested that there was a significant difference in the group mean of academic performance. On examining the significant effects of parenting style and parental involvement on academic performance, the results suggested that the model explained 24 % (R2= 0.24) of the variance in academic performance. Therefore, the variables in combination explained a larger effect on the students’ academic performance. In addition, marital status and parents’ educational backgrounds were also important when examining the students’ academic performance 5.5. Implications and Recommendations Based on the findings of the study, the following implications and recommendations could be made: 5.5.1. Theoretical Implications The parenting style model presented in this study provides a basis for other researchers interested in investigating the effectiveness of different parenting styles. This study 124 indicates that the authoritative parenting provides for the highest academic performance of students. As Baumrind (1991) discussed in her parenting patterns, the authoritative style of parenting promoted optimal development in adolescents. Authoritative style is based on high demandingness and high responsiveness, and according to Baumrind, demandingness is important to the development of competence. Competence is developed in the adolescent, over time, internalizes such demands. Though the use of monitoring and direct confrontation, are forms of demandingness, the adolescent internalizes autonomy, self- reliance, and self- assertiveness. Demandingness alone, however, is not enough to enable the adolescent to develop competence. Responsiveness is also important in developing competence in adolescents. Emotional responsiveness enables the adolescent to understand that his or her behaviors have effects on others. As the adolescent internalizes this knowledge, he or she learns coping skills which enhances his or her emotional maturity. Cognitive responsiveness encourages the adolescent to express his or her views and ideas. Once this behavior is internalized by the adolescent, he or she becomes self- confident and purposeful. In sum, the adolescent reared under the authoritative parenting style typically develops selfconfidence, self- respect, psychological health, purposefulness, and self- reliance. In this study, the students are found to respond similarly under the authoritative parenting style. Cangemi (1984) found that the purpose of education was to help students become fully- functioning independent, trusting self-confident, self respectful, and knowledgeable in a particular academic discipline. 125 Although permissively advised students were satisfied, they were no better off in their academic success than the students advised under authoritarian style. The reason for the difference between these two styles and the authoritative style lies in the low demandingness on the part of the permissive parents, and low responsiveness on the part of authoritarian parents when students parents do not place high enough demands on their students, as in the permissive style of parenting, the students are at greater risk of failing to develop competence in their learning than students whose parents are more demanding. Similarly, when students’ parents are not responsive to their students’ needs, as in the authoritarian style of parenting, the students are also at risk of failing to develop academic competence. In sum, demandingness or responsiveness alone is not sufficient for the optimal development of students. The development of academic competence is what will make the students complete their academic programs successfully. When students are self- confident and self reliant, they are more likely to be self-regulated learners. According to social learning theory, selfregulated learners monitor their own progress through three sub-processes: selfobservation, self-judgment, and self-reaction (Schunk, 2000). When students monitor their own performance and evaluate that performance, it becomes evident that they are becoming more competent and therefore closer to their goal. The internalization of the psychological attributes that authoritative parenting enhances – self-confidence, selfrespect autonomy – set the stage for academic competence to be internalized and therefore self-perpetuating, which leads to academic success. 126 5.5.2 Educational Implications It is clear that parenting relationship is important to students, as well as important in influencing their educational experience. During the administration of the instrument, a student who was part of the sample described her father as very demanding, facilitates his progress and makes him as marketable as possible. Another student talked about his parenting relationship with less enthusiasm. He pointed out that his father does not really provide him any guidance. He further emphasized that when he goes to him for advice and help, the father mostly response that he is a big boy to handle it, which to him was totally inappropriate. These two students support the contention that parenting style is important to students. From the study, it was clear that differing levels of parenting styles are related to the students’ academic performance. This discovery can be important in other areas of education as well, not just parent- student relationships. For example, the communication style used by a teacher with his or her teaching in turn could be evaluated along the dimensions of demandingness and responsiveness. Regardless of what type of parenting and parents’ school involvement under investigation, the practice of the proper combination of demandingness and responsiveness on the part of the parent would help alleviate many of the problems students encounter in their schooling. The students in this study performed academically better under authoritative parenting style and the learning at home, which mixes high levels of responsiveness with low levels of demandingness. High demandingness is an important aspect of parenting when 127 properly utilized by the parent. As one student comments, `` My father expects me to work hard. His expectations are sometimes too high for me. However, he supports me and wants me to succeed in my field``. However, high demandingness on the part of parents can be harmful to the students and can cause excessive stress if the parent does not participate in the decision-making of the student. Demandingness without proper support on the parent’s part appears to be perceived by the student as either insurmountable obstacle. On the other hand, low demandingness (e.g. permissive parenting) can just as harmful to students’ development. Progress toward completion of work, and the degree itself, appears to be dependent in part on the parent’s ability to communicate his or her expectation in such a way to motivate the student, rather than simply exert control over the student’s decision. Again, high parent responsiveness (e.g. authoritative and permissive parenting) is also important to students’ academic performance or experience. Supportive such as being attentive and interested in the students progress appear to be imperative. Without such responsiveness, students appear to feel overwhelmed by the educational process. Again, students are evidently dependent on their parents for being responsive to their needs in areas such as choosing their career, and also being available to discuss personal concerns related to their academic success. 5.5.3 Counselling Implications The findings of this study have far researching implication not just for parents but also for counselors’. First, there is the need to forge home-school relationship for not only enhancing the academic performance or well-being of the students but their overall well128 being as well. The responsibility of educating the child should not be left to the school alone. According to Howell (2002), children spend 87% of their waking life with parents and the remaining 13% in the school. This study has confirmed the strong impact the parenting style and parents’ school involvement has on students’ academic performance. There is therefore the need for counsellors to put in place appropriate programmes that will facilitate the improvement of parents’ school involvement and their parenting skills in the education of their children. School plays a significant role in the lives of children therefore counsellors who work with students’ populations has the opportunity to learn how the unique role of parents’ relationship with their children affects their children’s learning. More importantly, they should inform the teachers about the students who fall victim on the lack of the studentparent relationship in order for the teachers to handle them accordingly. Counsellors are therefore in a position to influence the school environment in which they work as well as the environment created by children and their parental relations. For a child to be academically effective, the need to foster the home-school relationship is indeed highly encouraged. As parenting and parental involvement require training, school counselors can organize seminars, educational forums and or workshops where parents are exposed to parenting and parental involvement skills and practices. 5.6. Limitations of the Study The present study has a number of limitations related to the methodology. First, information regarding the type of parenting style and parental involvement was gathered via self-report. In any self-report measure, the researcher runs the risk of receiving 129 inaccurate data due to a lack of honesty on the part of the respondents. This implies that the type of information collected can lead to the problems that, the respondents may report what they believe is the most desirable information. Another limitation is that the study focused entirely on students perceptions on their relationships with their parents. Although students’ perceptions are an important aspect of parent relationships, they may not provide a completely accurate picture of the parenting and the type of their parents’ school involvement the students are experiencing. Finally, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution. Since the study is correlational, it is not possible to postulate that students’ academic performance is caused by a particular parenting style or parental involvement. It is equally possible that some other variable such as students’ personality, elicits particular parenting style and parental involvement from parents. 5.7. Suggestions for Future Research The result of this study open up several future research opportunities. First, in this study, only the perspectives of the students were considered. In order to provide a more complete picture of the relationship between the two independent variables- parenting styles and parental involvement and students academic performance, future research should take into account the parents perspectives of their own parenting style and the type of their school involvement- rather than relying solely on the impressions of their children. 130 A second suggestion for future research centers on the consistency of the parenting style a parent adopts. In future studies, it would be helpful to collect information from several students who have the same parent to evaluate whether the parent is consistent in his or her parenting style across students. This information could be useful in pointing out possible intervening variables that may provide fuller explanation of parent behaviour. A third suggestion for future research involves including the variable of student and parent personality. It is possible that the interaction of the personality of the student and that of the parent influences the parenting and the type of parent school involvement a parent exhibits. Personality has been shown to be a factor in other relationships, such as romantic, mentoring, and friendship relationships (Berry and Willingham, 1997). In these varying other relationships, personality has been found to be an important factor in quality of the relationship in the context of warmth and conflict. Levels of warmth and conflict within the parent- child relationship may influence the amount of parenting style and the type of parent school involvement a parent shows his or her child. For example, if personality factors introduce high levels of conflict into the parent – child relationship, what might have been otherwise high levels of support on the parent’s part may well become a much lower level of support. A final suggestion for the future research involves the variable sex. It would be helpful to explore the effects of sex on the relationship in terms of whether same-sexed parents and children operate within the parenting relationship differently from different sexed pairs. 131 REFERENCES Alhassan, S. (2007) Modern Approaches to Research in Educational Administration for Research students, Kumasi, Payless Publications. Arendell, T. (1997) Contemporary parenting: Challenging and issues, California: Sage. Baker, J. L., & Soden, L. M. (1998). The challenges of parent involvement research. (Report No. EDO-UD-98-4). New York, NY: ERIC Clearinghouse on Urban Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 419 030). Balli, S.J., Demo, D.H., &Wedman, J.F. (1998). Family involvement with children's homework:An intervention in the middle grades. Family Relations, 47(2), 149157 Barber, B. K. (1996) Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct, Child Development, 67(6), 3296-3319. Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D.A.(1986). The moderato-mediator variable distinction in social Psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 51, NO.6 pp. 1173-1182. Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parental style on adolescent competence and Substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11 (1), 56-95. Baumrind, D. (1989) Rearing competent children, In W. Damon (Ed) Child Development Today and Tomorrow (pp.349-378). San Francisco; Jossey-Bass. Baumrind, D. (1978) Parental disciplinary patterns and social competence in children. Youth and Society, 9, 238-276 Baumrind, D. (1971) Current pattern of parental authority. Development Psychology. 4 (1, pt. 2), 1-103 132 Baumrind, D. (1967) Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behaviour. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75 (1), 43-88. Baumrind, D. (1966) Effects of authoritative parental control behaviour, Child Development, 37 (4), 887-907. Beck, L., &Mulphy, J. (1999).Parental involvement in site-based management: lessons from one site. International Journal of Leadership in Education, 2(2), 81- 102 Bernard, H. R. (2002) Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approach, Thousand Oaks: Sage. Berry, D.S., & Willingham, J.K. (1997). Affective traits responses to conflict and satisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 564-576 Buri, J. (1991) Parental authority questionnaire: Journal of Personality Assessment, 57 (1), 110-119. Cangemi, J.P. (1984). The real purpose of higher education: Developing self-actualizing personalities. Education, 105(2), 151-154. Clark, R.M. (1990). Why disadvantaged children succeed. Public Welfare (Spring): 1723. Couchman, W& Dawson,J.(1995). Nursing and health care research: A practical guide edition. London: Scutari Press. 133 Creswell, J. W. (2002) Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating Qualitative and quantitative research, Columbus.OH: Merrill Patience-Hall Creswell, J. W. (`1998) Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Creswell, J. W. (1994) Research design: Qualitative and quantitative approaches, London: Sage Publications. Crozier, G. (2000). Parents and schools: parents or protagonists? Sterling, VA:Trentham D'Agostino, J., Hedges, L., & Borman, G. (2001). Title I parent-involvement programs: Effects on parenting practices and student achievement. In G Borman, S. Springfield, & R.Slavin (Eds.), Title I: Compensatory education at the crossroads (pp. 117-136) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum Associates. Desimone, L. (1999). Linking parent involvement with student achievement: Do race and income matter? The Journal of Educational Research, 93(1), 11-30. Drake, D.D. (2000). Parents and families as partners in the education progress: Collaboration for the success for students in public schools, ERS Spectrum, 34- 35 Dwairy, M., Fayad, M., Khan, H.K., & Menshar, K.E. (2006) .Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. Vol. 37. No.3, 1-11, Sage Publications. 134 Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Robert, D. F., & Fraleigh, M. J. (1987).The relation of parenting styles to adolescent school performance. Child Development, 58, 1244-1257 Drake,D.D.(2000). Parents and families as partners in the education process: Collaborating for the success for students in public school schools. ERS Spectrum, 34-35. Eccles, J.S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U.(1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon and Eisenberg (Eds.). Handbook of Child Psychology,3, 1017-1095. Eisner, E. W. (1991). The enlightened eye: Qualitative inquiry and the enhancement of educational practice. New York: Macmillan. Epstein, J.L. (2001). School, family, and community partnership: Preparing educators and improving schools. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Epstein, J.L. (1995). School/family/community partnerships: Caring for the Children we Share. Phi Delta Kappan, 76(9), 701-712 Epstein, J.L (1984). Single parent and schools: the Effects of Marital Status, Parent and Teacher. Phi Delta Kappan, 32(8) 401-435 Epstein.J. Sanders., M., Simon, B., Salinas, K., Jonsorn, N., & Van Voorhis, F.(2002). School, family and community partnerships: Your handbook for action (2ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Crown Press. Fan, X.T., & Chen, M., (2001). Parental involvement and students' academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 1-22 135 Fantuzzo, J.W., Davis, G.Y., & Ginsburg, M.D.(1995). Effects of parent involvement in isolation or in combination with peer tutoring on student self-concept and mathematics achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 272-281 Garson, D.(2006). Reliability analysis. http://www2.chass.nsc.edu/garson/PA765. reliab. htm. Georgiou, S.N.(1997). Parental Involvement: Definitions and Outcomes. Social Psychology of Education, 1, 189-209 Glasgow, K. L., Dornbusch, S. M., Troyer, L., Steinberg, L., & Ritter, P. L. (1997) Parenting Styles, adolescents' attributions, and educational outcomes in nine heterogeneous high schools. Child Development, 68, 507-529 Goldtein, H.S. (1984). Parental Composition, Supervision, and Conduct problems in youth 12 to 7 years old. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23(6), p. 679-684 Gottfried, A.E.(1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in young elementary school. Journal of Education Psychology 82(3), 525-538 Grolnick, W.S & Slowiaczek, M.L (1987). Parents in children Schooling: A multidimensional conceptualization and motivational mode. Child Development, 65, 1, 237-252. Gronlund, N. E.(1985). Measurement and evaluation in teaching, New York: Macmillan Publishing Co., fifth edition Hammer, B. (2003) ETS identifies affecting student achievement. Washington update. 136 Heinrich, K.T. (1991). Loving partnership: Dealing with sexual attraction and power in doctoral advisement relationships. Journal of Higher Education, 62, 514-538 Howell, D.C. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology (5th.ed). Pacific Grove :Duxberry. Hyman, I. A. (1988) Eliminating corporal punishment in schools: Moving from advocacy research to policy implementation, Paper presented at the 96th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Atlanta, and G.A. Jackson, C., Henriksen, L., & Foshee, V.A.(1998). The authoritative parenting index: parenting health risk behaviors among children and adolescents. Health Education & Behaviour. Kaiser. H.F.(1974) ‘’An index of factorial simplicity’’, psychometrika, vol.39 pp 31 – 36 Keith, T.Z., Keith, P.B., Troutman, G.C., Bickley, P.G., Trivette, P.S., &Singh, K. (1993). Does parental involvement affect eighth-grade student achievement? Structural analysis of national data. School Psychology Review, 22(3), 474-496 Kozol, J. (1991) Savage inequalities. New York; Crown. Kurdek,L,A,& Fine, M.A.(1994). Family control as predictors of adjustment in young adolescents: Linear, curvilear, or interactive effects? Child Development,65, 1137- 1146. Kushman, J.W., Sieber, C., & Harold, K. P.(2000). This isn’t the place for me: School dropout. American Counselling Association (pp. 471-507) Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991) Child Development, 62, 1049-1065. 137 Leler, H. (1983) Parent education and Involvement in relation to the schools and to Parents of School-aged Children’ in R. Haskins &D. Adams(Eds), Parent education and public policy. Ablex. Norwood. Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983) Socialization in the context of the family: Parentchild interaction. In P.H.Mussen (Ed), & E.M Hetherington (vol. Ed), Handbook of Child psychology: vol 4. Socialization, personality, and Development (4th ed., pp. 1-101), New York: Wiley. .Mead, M. (1935) Sex and temperament in three primitive societies. New York: Mentor. McIntyre, T. (1992) A primer on cultural diversity for educators. Multicultural Forum, 1 (1), 6-7&12. McNeal, R. B. (1999). Parental involvement as social capital: Differential effectiveness science achievement, truancy, and dropping out. Social Forces, 78(1), 117-144. Muller, C. (1995). Maternal employment, parent involvement, and mathematics achievement among adolescents. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 57(1), 95-100. Nitko, J.A.(1983). Educational test and measurement. An introduction. New York, Harcourt Brace Jovanich inch. Nunnaly, J.C. (1964). Educational measurement and evaluation. New York: MacGraw Hill Book Company Oyediran, A. I. (1981) Measurement and Evaluation in Education, Ibadan Sterling Holden Publishers. 138 Paulson, S.E. (1994). Relations of parenting style and parental involvement with ninthgrade students’ achievement. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 250. Philips, M.(1998). Family background, parenting practices, and the black-white test score gap. The black-white test score gap. Washington, D.C., Brooking Institution Press. Rich, D.; VanDien, J., & Mallox, B. (1979). Families as educators of their own children. In R.Brandt (Ed.). Parents and schools. Alexandria V.A.: Association for supervision and curriculum development. Romberg,E.(1993). Mentoring the individual student: Qualities that distinguish between effective and ineffective advisors. Journal of Dental Education, 57, 287-290 Rubin, A. (2005). Research method for social work. 5th Edition. California: Brooks/Cole Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest learning and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26,299323. Schunck.D.H. (1991).Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychology,26,207-213 Simon, B.(2001). Family involvement in high school: Predictors and effects. NASSP Bulletin, 85(627), 8-19. Spinetta, J. J. & Rigler, D. (1972) The Child-Abusing Parent: A psychological Review. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 296-304. Stegelin,D. (2002).Early literacy education: First steps toward dropout prevention. Effective strategies for school improvement and dropout prevention. ERIC Document No. ED472188. 139 Steinberg, L. Darling, N., & Fletcher, A. C. (1995) Authoritative Parenting and Adolescent Adjustment: An ecological journey. In P. Moen, G. H.Elder Jr, & K, Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspective on the ecology of human development (pp. 423-466), Washington D. C: American Psychological Assn. Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989) Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity and academic success among adolescents. Child Development, 60, 1244-1436 Sui-Chu, E. S., & Willms, J. D. (1996). Effects of parental involvement on eighth-grade Achievement. Sociology of Education, 69, 126-141. Wang. Wildman. & Calhoun, G.,(1996). The relationship between parental influences and student achievement in seventh grade mathematics, school science and mathematics. Child Development 96(8), 395-400 Weiss, L. H., & Schwarz, J. C. (1996) The relationship between parenting types and older adolescents' personality, academic achievement, adjustment, and substance use. Child Development, 67(5), 2101-2114 140 APPENDIX A 141 APPINDIX B UNIVERSITY OF EDUCATION, WINNEBA DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH QUESTIONNAIRE FOR STUDENTS I am currently completing my MPHIL in GUIDANCE AND COUNSELLING at University of Education, Winneba and am investigating the ``Effects of Parenting Styles and Parental Involvement on Students Academic Performance in Senior High School Students particularly the third years. You have been randomly selected from your class. I am therefore asking you to help in gathering the data concerning the above research topic. Along is two instruments – Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ), and Parental Involvement Questionnaires (PIQ) which you are kindly expected to respond accordingly. Your individual responses will be kept confidential. Thank you, in advance, for assistance Mr. Addai Francis------ The researcher. 142 SECTION A RESPONDENTS PARENTS BACKGROUND INFORMATION Direction: Here are some items regarding your parents’ background information. Kindly provide the information as requested by 1. Type of sex you stay with 5. Family type 1.Male 2. Female 1.Intact 2. Single 6. Educational background 1. No high school completed. 2. High school completed 3. Some college completed. 4. Graduate SECTION B STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE - PAQ Instructions. For each of the following statements, circle the number of the 4-point scale (1= strongly disagree, 5= strongly agree) that best describes how that statement applies to you and your father. Try to read and think about each statement as it applies to you and your father of growing up at home. There are no right or wrong answers, so don’t spend a lot of time on any one item. We are looking for your overall impression regarding each statement. Be sure not to omit any items. 143 1. My parents have always felt that what children need is to be free to make up their minds and to do what they want to do, even if this does not agree with what the parents might want 1 2 3 Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 2. My parents have always felt that more force should be used by parents in order to get their children to behave the way they are suppose to 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neither 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 3. While I was growing up, my parents felt that in a well-run home children should have their way in the family as often as parents do 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree 4. As I was growing up, I knew what my parents expected of me in the family and they insisted that I conform to those expectations simply out of respect for their authority 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neither 144 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 5. Whenever my parents told me to do something as I was growing up, they expected me to do it immediately without asking any question 1 Strongly Disagree 2 3 Disagree Neither 4 5 Agree Strongly Agree 6. As I was growing up my parents seldom gave me expectations guidance’s for my behavior 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neither 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 7. My parents had clear standards of behavior for the children in our home as I was growing up, but they were willing to adjust those standards to the needs of each of the in child, in the family 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree 8. My parents felt that wise parents should teach their children early just who is boss in the family 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neither 145 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 9. Most of my time as I was growing up my parents did what the children in the family wanted when making family decisions 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 10. As I was growing up, my parents would get very upset if I tried to disagree with them 1 2 Strongly Disagree Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree 11. As I was growing up my parents allowed me to form my own point of view on family matters and they generally allowed me to decide for myself what I was going to do 1 2 3 Strongly Disagree Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 12. As I was growing up, my parents allowed me to decide most things for myself without a lot of direction from them 1 Strongly Disagree 2 3 4 Disagree Neither Agree 146 5 Strongly Agree 13. My parents have always encouraged verbal give-and-take whenever I have felt that my family rules and restrictions were unreasonable 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 14. As I was growing up, my parents gave me clear direction for my behaviors and activities, but they also understood when I disgrace with them 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 15. As I was growing up, parents did not allow me to question any decision that they had made 1 2 Strongly Disagree Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree 16. As I was growing up, once family policy had been established, my parents discussed the reasoning behind the policy with the children in the family 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neither 147 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 17. As I was growing up, my parents took the children’s opinions into consideration when making family decisions, but they would not decide for something simply because the children wanted it 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree The items that were excluded from further Analysis(PAQ) Permissive 10. As I was growing up my parents did not feel that I needed to obey rules and regulations of behaviour simply because someone in authority and established them 21. My parents did not view themselves as responsible for directing and guiding my behavior as I was growing up 28. As I was growing up, my parents did not direct the behaviors, activities, and desires of children in the family. Authoritarian 2. Even if children didn’t agree with the parents, they felt that it was for our own good if we were forced to conform to what they through were sight 18. As I was growing up, my parents let me know what behaviors they expected of me, and if I didn’t meet those expectations, they punished me 148 25. My parents have always felt that most problems in society would be solved if we would get parents to strictly and forcibly deal with their children when they don’t do what they are supposed to as they are growing up 26. As I was growing up, my parents often told me exactly what they wanted me to do and how they expected me to do Authoritative 8. As I was growing up, my parents directed activities and decisions of the children in the family through reasoning and discipline 11. As I was growing up, I knew what my parents expected of me in my family, but I felt free to discuss those expectations with my parents when I felt that they were unreasonable 15. As the children in my family were growing up, my parents consistently gave us direction and guidance in rational and objective ways 17. My parents feel that most problems would be solved if parents would not restrict their children’s activities, decisions, and desires as they are growing up 23. My parents gave me direction for my behavior and activities as I was growing up and they expected me to fellow their direction, but they were always willing to listen to my concerns and to discuss that direction with me 30. As I was growing up, if my parents made a decision in the family that hurt me, they were willing to discuss that decision with me and to admit it if they had made a mistake 149 APPENDIX C PARENTAL INVOLVEMENT QUESTIONNAIRE (PIQ) (Questionnaire after Factor Analysis) Instructions: For each of the following statements, circle the number of the 4-point scale (1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree) that best describe your parents’ school involvement. If you spend time in more than one home, answer the questions about the parents (guardians) who have the most say over your schooling. 1. I talk to my parents about things I learnt at school. 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 2. My parents monitor my schoolwork when I come home from school. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 5 Neither Agree Strongly Agree 3 4 5 Neither Agree Strongly Agree 3. My parents see that I have done my homework 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 150 4. My parents listen when I talk to them 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 5. School administration knows my parents postal address/telephone number. 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 6. I talk to my parents about problems I have at school. 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 7. My parents involve me in taking decision in the family. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree 5 Strongly Agree 8. My parents seek my concern when they want to buy any new item 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither 151 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 9. My parents discuss with me on what I learnt at school 1 2 Strongly Disagree 3 Disagree 4 Neither 5 Agree Strongly Agree 10. My parents involve me in taking decision in the family 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 4 Neither Agree The items that were excluded from further Analysis (PIQ) 1. My parents allow me to attend extra-classes when school vacate 2. When my parents want me to do something, they explain why 3. My parents attend any parents gathering or forum in the school 4. My parents have some of my teachers’ telephone numbers 5. My parents have a set time and place for me to do my home work 152 5 Strongly Agree APPENDIX D Z-SCORES z 0.00 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.04 0.05 0.06 0.07 0.08 1.0 0.3413 0.3438 0.3461 0.3485 0.3508 0.3531 0.3554 0.3577 0.3599 1.1 0.3643 0.3665 0.3686 0.3708 0.3729 0.3749 0.3770 0.3790 0.3810 1.2 0.3849 0.3869 0.3888 0.3907 0.3925 0.3944 0.3962 0.3980 0.3997 1.3 0.4032 0.4049 0.4066 0.4082 0.4099 0.4115 0.4131 0.4147 0.4162 1.4 0.4192 0.4207 0.4222 0.4236 0.4251 0.4265 0.4279 0.4292 0.4306 1.5 0.4332 0.4345 0.4357 0.4370 0.4382 0.4394 0.4406 0.4418 0.4429 1.6 0.4452 0.4463 0.4474 0.4484 0.4495 0.4505 0.4515 0.4525 0.4535 1.7 0.4554 0.4564 0.4573 0.4582 0.4591 0.4599 0.4608 0.4616 0.4625 1.8 0.4641 0.4649 0.4656 0.4664 0.4671 0.4678 0.4686 0.4693 0.4699 1.9 0.4713 0.4719 0.4726 0.4732 0.4738 0.4744 0.4750 0.4756 0.4761 Source: Adapted Howell, (2002) 153 APPENDIX E SCIENCE ACHIEVEMENT TEST (SAT) ANSWER ALL QUESTIONS Each question is followed by four options lettered A to D. Find out the correct option for each question. Give only one answer to each question. Remember to write your code on your answer sheet. 1. The vision of a person suffering from night blindness can improve when fed on A. Carbohydrates B. Protein C. Vitamin A D. Vitamin C 2. Which of the following metals is the best conductor of heat. A. Aluminium B. Copper C. Iron D. Zinc 3. The element required for the formation of strong and health teeth is A. calcium B. magnesium C. phosphorus D. sodium 4. Heat is generated in the human body during A. defecation B. expiration C. shivering D. sweating 5. Which of the following activities is an involuntary action A. Breathing B. Eating C. Reading D. Walking 6. The commonest method for propagating cocoa in Ghana is by A. seeds B. stem C. root cuttings D. budding 7. A suitable method of reducing erosion on hilly lands is by A. applying organic manure B. erecting barriers C. creating wind breaks D. terracing 8. Which of the following factors affects the loudness of sound A. Amplitude B. Frequency C. Velocity Wavelength D. 9. Friction in car engines can be reduced through a A. lubrication B. over hauling C. spraying vulcanizing D. 10. Which of the following diseases is associated with lives in humans A. Appendicitis B. Arteriosclerosis C. Bronchitis D. Cirrhosis 154 11. The thickness of the lens of the human eye is controlled by A. ciliovy muscle B. conjunctiva C. sclerotic wat D.votereous humour 12. If 1 mole of oxygen reacts with 2 moles of calcium to form calcium hydroxide, then the number of moles of calcium oxide formed is A. 1 mole B. 2 moles C. 3 moles D. 5 moles 13. A farming practice that encourages soil erosion is A. bush burning B. contour ploughing C. strip cropping D. terracing 14. The nucleus of atom contains A. electrons and protons B. neutrons and protons C. neutrons and electrons D. protons, electrons and neutrons 15. The habit of smoking may result in A. anaemia B. arteriosclerosis C. cirrhosis 16. The density of water is highest when its temperature is A. 10C B. 40C C. 30C D. meningitis D. 1000C 17. One of the chemical used to test for the presence of protein is A. Benedict’s solution B. Fehling’s solution C. Millon’s reagent D. Anhydrous copper sulphate 18. Brightly coloured petals A. accelerate ripening of fruits B. aid pollination by animals C. induce the production of large fruits D. promote the development of the ovule 19. Soil may lose its fertility through A. leaching B. overcooling ridging C. shifting cultivation D. 20. An atom of an element X has 21 protons and 23 neutrons. What is the number of elements in it A. 2 B. 21 C. 23 D. 44 155 APPENDIX F MATHEMATICS ACHIEVEMENT TEST (SAT) ANSWER ALL QUESTIONS Each question is followed by four options lettered A to D. Find out the correct option for each question. Give only one answer to each question. Remember to write your code on your answer sheet. 1. Evaluate (0.13)3 correct to three significant figures A. 0.002 B. 0.003 C. 0.00219 D. 0.00220 2. A bag contains 12 blue and 8 red balls. If 2 balls are picked at random from the bag, without replacement, what is the probability that they are both blue? A. 9/25 B. 33/95 C. ½ D. 33/100 3. Evaluate (111two + 101two) (111two – 101two) A. 10two B. 1100 two C. 1100 two D. 11000 two 4. Simplify 2/3 + 2/5 – 7/15 A. 23/15 B. 11/15 D. 2/5 C. 3/5 5. Write 0.024561, correct to 3 significant figures A. 0.03 B. 0.025 C. 0.0246 D. 0.0245 6. Solve the equation 3x/5 + 2 = x – 2 3 A. -40 B. -20 D. -5 C. -10 7. If t=p2 – 3q, calculate the value of t when p=4 and q=9 A. -11 B. -43 C. 11 D. 43 8. Arrange in ascending order of magnitude, 7/75, 2/5 and 1/3 A. 2/5, 1/3, 7/75 B. 1/3, 7/75, 2/5 C. 1/3, 2/5, 7/75, D. 7/75, 1/3, 2/5, 9. The interior angle of a regular polygon is 108 0. How many sides has the polygon? A. 5 B. 6 C. 7 D. 8 10. Which base five numeral is equivalent to (4x53) + (0x52) + (2x51) + (2x50) base ten? A. 402five B. 422 five C. 4022 five D. 4220 five 156 11. Find the range of values of x for which 5-x + 2 < x-2 3 2 A. <א12/5 B. <א9/2 C. >א28/5 D. <א8 12. Find the equation of the line whose gradient is -1/2 and which passes through the point (7, -2) A. y+2–א3 = 0 B. y+2א+3=0 C. 2y+א-3=0 D. 2y+א+3=0 13. Make c the subject of the relation ac + d = p e A. c = ep + d B. c = d - ep C. c = pp + d a a a D. c = ep – a a The table below shows the distribution of marks obtained by twenty pupils in a test Marks 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 No. of students 1 3 5 6 2 1 2 Use the table to answer Question 14 to 16 14. What is the modal mark of the distribution A. 2 B. 3 C. 4 D. 5 15. Find the median mark A. 3 B. 4 C. 5 D. 6 16. Find the mean mark for the distribution A. 2.7 B. 2.9 C. 3.8 D. 4.0 17. Solve the following simultaneous equations: x-4y = 1. 2x+3y = 15 A. (-3, -1) B. (5, -1) C. (1, -5) D. (5, 1) 18. Find the image of the point p(-12, 4) A. (-12, 4) B. (-12, 2) D. (-12, -10) C. (-12, 10) 19. The point P (3, -1) is rotated about the origin through an angle of 2700 in a clockwise direction. Find the image of P. A. (3, 1) B. (1, 3) C. (-1, 3) D. (-1, -3) 20. In an examination, Ama scored 80% in mathematics, 60% in science and א% in English. If her mean mark for the subjects was 65%, find א A. 55 B. 65 C. 70 D. 75 157 APPENDIX G ENGLISH ACHIEVEMENT TEST (SAT) ANSWER ALL QUESTIONS Each question is followed by four options lettered A to D. Find out the correct option for each question. Give only one answer to each question. Remember to write your code on your answer sheet. SECTION A From the list of words lettered A to D, choose the one that best completes each of the following sentences 1. The ………..dispute between the two royal families has now been settled. A. normal B. continuous C. protracted D. modest 2. To ensure success in examinations students should ………..laziness and complacency. A. hate B. eschew C. relinquish D. dislike 3. The …………returned to their own country after the civil war A. mercenaries B. vandals C. thugs D. rebels 4. No one should ………with the good work of the prefects A. mingle B. intercede C. interfere D. mediate SECTION B Choose from the alternatives lettered A to D the one which is nearest in meaning to the underlined word or expression in each sentence. 5. Kwaw tackled the task with zeal A. knowledge B. intelligence enthusiasm C. sympathy D. 6. The football fans marveled at the captain’s unique display of sportsmanship A. Enviable B. unequalled C. wonderful D. admirable 7. We admired her rigrous defence of women’s rights A. eager B. angry C. spirited 158 D. lively 8. His scheme is meant to foster good neighbourliness A. adopt B. help C. promote D. support SECTION C From the words or groups of words littered from A to D, choose the one that best completes each of the following sentences. 9. The new chief has no respect for ……who supported him. A. we B. use C. ours D. ourselves 10. The thieves ran into the house when the police closed ……on them. A. at B. in C. up D. around 11. No one except her friends ……….her. A. likes B. like C. have liked D. is liking SECTION D In the following passage, the numbered gaps indicate missing words. Against each number in the list below the passage, four choices are offered in columns lettered A to D for each numbered gap, choose from the options provided for that number, the word that is most suitable to fill the gap. Even after the operation, doctors say Kankam’s road to - 12 - is uncertain, despite his ability to breathe – 13 - at the moment. All the same, as his - 14 has improved slightly, Kankam has begun to - 15 - to pain. In addition, his body can now perform - 16 - functions, even though he is in - 17 - coma. Since he suffered a – 18 – stroke last week, doctors can now san now say Kankam is – 19 – danger, though a full assessment of brain – 20 – is yet to be made. A 12. health B improvement C recovery D restoration 13. alone fast unsupported unaided 159 14. situation condition predicament status 15. respond answer reply revert 16. fundamental minor basic easy 17. deliberate induced influence involved 18. great heavy massive serious 19. outside away from unaffected by out of 20. injury damage deterioration wreckage 160