Professor in charge: BSCI 465 / Biol 708D COURSE INFORMATION Fall 2013 M-W 10:00-11:15; 1117 PLS Dr. Gerald Borgia, Biology Department Office hours: After class Wednesday and by appointment Office BP 4239 Email Borgia@umd.edu Living organisms had existed on Earth, without ever knowing why, for more than three billion years before the truth finally dawned on one of them. His name was Charles Darwin. – Richard Dawkins, 1976 The question "What is man?" is probably the most profound …[and] … all attempts to answer that question before 1859 are worthless and that we will be better off if we ignore them completely. —Simpson 1966, p. 472 Overview Welcome to BSCI 465/Biol 708D, Behavioral Ecology. In this course you will become acquainted with the some fundamental principles of evolutionary biology and how they can be applied to study current issues in animal and human behavior. This course is directed at answering the question: Why do living things do what they do? We will be interested in how behaviors evolve and in determining the functional significance of behaviors in all organisms. Issues associated with behavior present some of the most significant challenges to evolutionary theory because of the complications associated with interactions between (and within) individuals that have different reproductive interests. For many years the study of animal behavior had no theoretical focus and many important and interesting questions went unanswered (or incorrectly answered) because biologists working in this area did not have an adequate theoretical model to deal with them. Much of this changed with the integration of modern evolutionary theory with the animal behavior and has let to the advent of behavioral ecology and sociobiology (to most biologists these are interchangeable labels). We will, as a consequence of this evolutionary focus, deal with a much broader definition of behavior than is commonly used in animal behavior courses. We will be as interested in the behavior of chromosomes as we are in the parental care of honey bees, or in human mate choice. We will focus on why certain behaviors and related traits evolve with particular emphasis on difficult evolutionary problems. Some questions we will consider are: Does altruism evolve? How do conflicts of interest arise and shape social interactions in mate choice, social grouping and meiosis? Why do certain adaptations, such as sexuality, that greatly affect social relationships in nearly all species, exist? One of the most useful techniques for studying evolution generally and behavior in particular is to study adaptations. Adaptations are traits that show design for reproduction of the organism carrying the trait. Natural selection is the key force shaping new adaptations and the ongoing pressure that it exerts maintains organisms in a reasonably well-adapted condition. Natural selection favors the reproduction of variants that are best able to deal with immediate environmental and social contingencies. The cumulative effect of this selection is to favor the reproduction of 1 genes that produce these effective “tools” against genetic alternatives that are less effective. To the extent that these tools (or traits) are inherited and, within the context of evolutionary time, organisms track their environment, producing and “perfecting” adaptations that have proven useful to their reproduction. The dominance of natural selection causes us to predict a close fit between the environment and the kinds of adaptations carried by an organism. This prediction is very useful for several reasons. First, it allows us to predict what kinds of behaviors (and, more generally, adaptations) we expect to see (and not see). Second, where there are deviations from these predictions there is often evidence of interesting and previously unnoticed theoretical issues. Third, a high level of adaptation causes us to look for functional significance where there is evidence of complex design. Fourth, complex adaptations are built up over time and as such their presence provides evidence of the past history of natural selection. We integrate these ideas with modern population genetics and to produce a model of behavioral evolution. We can then explore patterns of adaptation to test this general model and understand more specific and difficult cases of behavioral adaptation. Course objectives: 1) To familiarize students with important issues in behavioral ecology, broadly defined. 2) To teach students “evolutionary thinking” which provides a powerful tool for solving problems in all of biology. 3) To critically consider scientific hypotheses and related tests in the literature. Course organization: BSCI 465/708D are 3 credit courses divided into two one hour and fifteen minute sessions per week. Monday will be a full 75 minute lecture and Wednesday will be split between lecture and discussion of assigned papers. Students are required to read papers in preparation for the discussion. Your final grade will be based on a total of 500 points. There will be one mid-term exam (100 points), a final (150 points/cumulative), one essay (135 points – 100 for the exam and 35 for meeting deadlines in paper preparation) and discussion (115 points). Since grading is on total points, a poor showing in one part of the course could result in a poor grade for the entire course. To guard against this possibility, it is important that you be an active participant in all phases of the course. Students are expected to follow University Honor Code and follow University policy for absences. Essays are expected to provide critical discussion of an idea or how an idea has been treated by another author. Essays should be 2 to 5 pages in length, and must be typewritten and double spaced with a few, usually not more than 5, relevant references You should write in the style of a scientific journal, with a concise style and include the points that you believe that are needed to explain the idea you are developing. References should follow the format of the journals Animal Behaviour, Behavioral Ecology or Evolution. Essays should be submitted electronically as an attachment in MS word doc file to Borgia@umd.edu with your name in the file name and you email address in the paper header. I require that the topic and outline of the essays must be discussed with me and approved before the essay is submitted (no emails to discuss please). This commonly entails a student finding a topic he or she wishes to write about and then a 2 brief visit with me to discuss the topic (By November 1; 15 points). There should then be a second visit in which we a written outline developed by the student that indicates the flow and logical sequence of the arguments to be presented in the essay (By November 9; 20 pts.). Paper or book summaries are not suitable, although critical reviews that offer a significantly different perspective and or analysis of an idea or viewpoint are. The key point is that I am looking for am original intellectual contribution from you. Suitable paper topics should involve issues discussed in the course or which are related to the theme of the course. The goal here is not to recycle old term papers or thesis proposals. I will provide lists of paper topics well in advance of the deadline. Essays are due November 15; 100 pts) unless we agree on an alternative date. Because essays require discussions with me please do not wait until the last minute to make appointments to discuss papers. Readings: Readings for this course are based on the primary literature. I will post materials s the week before when the reading is due. Students are expected to have read the material and be ready to actively participate in class discussions based on the readings. A significant part of the course grade will depend on student preparation and participation in course discussions (115 points). For extra help the book The Selfish Gene (second edition) by Richard Dawkins is highly recommended as a helpful overview and guide to how issues are thought of in this course. Other useful books include Principals of Social Behavior, By Andrew Bourke, Behavioral ecology by J.R. Krebs and N. Davies, Adaptation and Natural Selection by George Williams, and Sex, Evolution and Behavior by M. Wilson and M. Daly. References to additional readings will be provided as the course progresses. Special notices: 1. Evolution is treated as a fact by all leading biologists and evolutionary theory is the central theoretical construct of biology. This course is centered on the view that evolution by natural selection has been critical in shaping life on earth including human behavior. While students are not required to believe that evolution has happen they should be warned that to be successful in this course they will have to use evolutionary ideas in all phases of the course. 2. Students should note that the evolution of sex and sexual tactics in animals and humans is a major component of this course. 3. To be successful in this course it’s important that you regularly attend lecture, take detailed notes and actively participate in discussions. Special considerations Academic dishonesty is incompatible with good science. As presumptive scientists I hold you to the standards of our discipline ergo cheating is not tolerated. Note that the University of Maryland sometimes treats academic dishonesty as a serious offense so it is in your interest to be familiar with the relevant sections of the student handbook. Make-up exams will not be given except in cases for which there is an appropriate medical reason. 3 Date Sep. Oct. 16 18 23 25 30 2 7 9 11 16 4 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. BSCI 465/Biol 708D Lecture Schedule Tentative Lecture Topic Introduction: Darwin, Evolution and Behavioral Ecology Natural selection and other agents of selection Adaptation : Levels of selection, selfish genetic elements, and inclusive fitness Levels of selection: Why lemmings don’t jump off cliffs Evolution as a predictive model: Adaptationism The debate over adaptationism: The clitoris debate Selfish genetic elements Gamete dimorphism and Bateman’s argument Evolution of mating systems Sexual selection models I: Runaway, Good genes, preexisting preference Sexual selection models II: Cooption and preexisting traits (bird song) Sperm competition, genetalia and male-female conflict Why do bowerbirds build bowers? Sexual conflict and cooperation 21 14. Midterm Exam 4 9 11 1. 2. 3. 23 15. Sex ratio: Inbreeding and outbreeding models 28 16. Evolution of sex 30 17. Inclusive fitness as an ESS Nov 4 18. Selection in small groups, group selection and altruism 6 19. Helping non-relatives -- reciprocity and cooperation 11 20. Memes, NS and the role of learning 13 21. Social evolution in insects and reproductive division of labor 18 22. Sociality in birds 20 23. Mammal social organization 25 24. Social structure in primates and hominids 27 25 Sexual selection in humans Dec 2 26. Human sexual conflict: MGM and FGM 4 27 Human brain and social evolution 9 28. Cultural evolution / Darwinian and Evolutionary Psychology 11 29. Darwinian medicine Final: Saturday, December 21, 8:00-10:00AM, Rm 1117 PLS (No early exams) 4 Discussing and critiquing scientific papers A major portion of this course deals with the discussion and criticism of scientific papers. In an ideal world all scientific papers would be clearly and concisely written, consider and effectively test all reasonable alternative hypotheses, have a large data base on which to draw conclusions, use appropriate statistics, be free of errors and, most importantly, state conclusions that are supported by the results. While many papers do meet these criteria many others do not sp scientific papers must be read (and written) from a critical viewpoint if they are to accomplish their goal of advancing our knowledge of biology. Almost all scientific papers undergo some review before they are published but the level of scrutiny is highly variable so even though they have undergone review papers with major errors are often published. No study is perfect but we can establish reasonable standards of what to expect. Standards change and almost always in the direction of increased stringency as a field develops. These may occur for a variety of reasons including a willingness to accept more questionable information when a field is new, the development of new and more accurate (and usually more complicated) techniques for analysis as a field develops, and increased competition for journal space. Regrettably many studies are considered important because of they were early in the development of a field, or because of who did them and not how well they are done. It is difficult to do good science and present scientific results papers well so we need not assume that papers with weaknesses are done with bad intent. It may be physically and economically impossible to collect all the desired data, and there are limits to the amount of time that can be devoted to doing analyses and writing the paper. What is amazing is the number of quality papers that are written despite these and other limitations. We should, however, require that authors meet reasonable standards and are straightforward about the limitations of their work. Part of this evaluation process comes from your experience in reading papers so that you can appreciate the range of variation in the quality of papers. Below are some questions you should consider in evaluating the papers you read. What questions does the publication address? Is there a significant question being addressed? There is a nearly endless number of questions that can be studied so it is important to ask if the questions addressed are of general interest. How do we know if a question is important/general? Does the study tests hypotheses that address important theoretical issues? We develop major hypotheses or theories to provide and understanding of how the world works. Generally, the more the hypothesis explains the greater its importance, e.g. does it explain or improve our understanding of some critical process in animals and/or humans? In this course we will value papers based on their ability to clarify issues important to understanding evolutionary and behavioral models. Papers that simply describe a particular kind of animal behavior are considered less valuable than those that use information about behaviors to develop and test general hypotheses about why that behavior occurs, particularly if that behavior is unexpected based on existing theory or if a there is new theory that has not previously been supported. Does the issue raised address a real problem or is it a result of unclear thinking? Statements may seem important because they have been linked to important processes but closer inspection shows this linkage is in error. Is the question falsifiable? Will any result support a particular conclusion or are the alternatives spelled out clearly. Does the publication address the question in a reasonable way? 5 Does the study make reasonable assumptions? No study can start from scratch and work must be done in the context of is already known. Does what it assumes as a starting point generally fit within this knowledge? This can be a problem for truly revolutionary studies because they often don't fit. The assumption of a paradigm for a field has the advantage of helping you sort through lots of information but if it is not reliable important discoveries that do not fit the conventional wisdom may be ignored. Every scientist must deal with what they believe to be realistic and not and this is often only obtained with experience. Are all reasonable hypotheses considered and tested in some way? There are often many possible explanations for a set of observations. We must make assumptions about what hypotheses we reasonably need to test. We must ask if an author has been fair in choosing what hypotheses to consider. Often not all hypotheses can be considered. Has the author been forthright in explaining what has not been done. Is the general question that is presented actually addressed by the experiments and or observations? Our ability to judge the significance of a study depends on the author’s ability to properly set the context for existing theoretical and empirical information. Does the author give a clear and fair (not inflated) picture of the meaning of the study and what has been done by others? For theoretical papers does the model accurately reflect natural systems? In biology, as opposed to mathematics, models are not of interest because of their intrinsic properties but because they provide accurate representations of the real world. Often reality is complex and it is necessary to simplify. Do the assumptions the model makes allow it to still provide a fair representation (one that can be tested empirically) of the real world. Are the simplifications and the domain over which the model is robust clearly stated? Data collection and treatment Most studies are done to make inferences about a larger set of organisms than were actually studied and to help resolve problems beyond those addressed in a particular experiment. Are means used to make these inferences reasonable? For example, are the target organisms of the study appropriate to the issues being considered? Are the study populations reasonable? Are they representative of the population they are supposed to be making inferences about or are they in marginal or disturbed habitats? This is particularly important for evolutionary studies because it is often assumed that current patterns are representative of past conditions (when the traits under study evolved). Are experiments reasonable? Do they ask the animal to do something it can't do? Are they properly controlled? Do they ignore factors like experience that isn't or can't be controlled. Do the variables measured actually measure what they claim (or what their name suggests)? Are there results not presented that can alter conclusions? It is important to present negative as well as positive results. Is there data that should have been collected that could clarify or alter conclusions? Do the conclusions follow from the data? Are all reasonable interpretations considered? Is there too much effort directed at explaining away inconvenient facts? Are there hidden biases in the data collection. Is observer bias considered and dealt with? Are the results of multiple tests "factored in" the assessment of statistical significance? Scientists can go “fishing” and if they do enough tests some will be statistically significant by chance. Do the statistics procedures support the rules of statistical inference? (Unless you have had a statistics course or two you probably wouldn't know too much about this!). Especially important are the issues of independence, unintended/hidden correlations, small sample size, 6 and use of appropriate tests (including meeting assumptions of tests). Are graphs and other figures understandable and properly labeled? Are all symbols defined? Do the summary and/or conclusions accurately reflect what was found in the body of the paper? 7