Broad versus Narrow Traits Broad versus Narrow Traits: Assessing the Bandwidth-Fidelity Tradeoff at the Team-level Thomas A. O’Neill Natalie J. Allen The TeamWork Lab Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario, Canada Corresponding Author: Thomas O’Neill Email:toneill7@uwo.ca 1 Broad versus Narrow Traits 2 Broad versus Narrow Traits: Assessing the Bandwidth-Fidelity Tradeoff at the Team-level It has long been known that personality is a consistent predictor of job-related criteria (Tett, Jackson, & Rothsein, 1991). Researchers have struggled, however, to find robust relations between personality and team performance (Kroeck & Brown, 2004). One potential limitation in the current literature is an over-emphasis on the “Big Five” framework of personality. Indeed, research has found that more precisely defined “narrow traits” can account for as much or more variance in general human behavior (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001), workplace criteria (e.g., Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina, 2006), and team outcomes (e.g., LePine, 2003). Extending this thinking to the team level, LePine (2003) found that the narrower facets of conscientiousness (i.e., dependability and achievement) were related to team adaptability in the predicted directions. That is, teams higher on dependability were less able to adapt to unanticipated changes in the task context, while teams that were more achievement-oriented were more adaptable. These findings highlight the potential value of using clearly defined narrow traits in the prediction of teamwork instead of relying solely on the Big Five. Although LePine’s research hints at the usefulness of narrow traits in predicting teamwork, these more specific traits have not been explicitly compared to broad factors in a team context. The purpose of the present study, therefore, is to examine the relative contribution of narrow to broader traits to the prediction of team process and outcome variables. Broad versus Narrow Traits 3 Methodology and Hypotheses Data were collected from 69 student teams enrolled in a four-month engineering course. We systematically selected and measured five relationship-oriented (i.e., sociability, affiliation, cooperation, aggression, defendence) and task-oriented narrow traits (i.e., endurance, achievement, cognitive structure, organization, and innovation), in addition to assessing the Big Five. We also measured two relationship-oriented emergent states (i.e., team cohesion, relationship conflict) and one task-oriented emergent state (i.e., group potency). Finally, we collected project grades assigned to teams as an indicator of team performance. The project was a 10-page engineering design report that described a prototype each team generated. Note that all of our criterion variables, with the exception of team performance, were aggregated to the team level. Statistics were calculated to justify aggregation (i.e., we calculated rwg(j), ICC(1), and ICC(2) indices). Their respective references, reliabilities, mean rwgs, ICC(1), ICC(2), and number of teams dropped from a given analysis due to low rwg values are in Table 1. We strictly adhered to the .70 cutoff for rwg(j) values, which resulted in 1 team being removed from the group potency analyses, 1 for team cohesion, and 3 for relationship conflict. The number of teams included in each analysis also varied as a function of missing data. Fourteen teams were missing data on emergent states; two teams were missing data on team performance; and one team was missing personality data. Taking both missing data and low rwg values in account, the n for analyses involving team performance was 66; the n for analyses involving team cohesion and potency was 53; and the n for analyses involving relationship conflict was 51. Our general hypothesis was that the five traits we selected as relationship-oriented would predict relationship-oriented emergent states (i.e., team cohesion, relationship conflict) as well, Broad versus Narrow Traits 4 or better, than the Big Five. Similarly, we hypothesized that the five traits we selected as taskoriented would predict the task-oriented emergent state (i.e., group potency) and outcome (i.e., team performance) as well, or better, than the Big Five. Four operationalizations of personality at the group level are commonly used in the literature: mean, variance, maximum, minimum (for example, see Barrick et al., 1998). For relationship-oriented narrow-trait predictors, operationalizations of personality involved only the mean. As for the task-oriented narrow-trait predictors, we used maximum organization, maximum cognitive structure, mean achievement, mean endurance, and maximum innovation. In terms of the Big Five, we hypothesized that mean conscientiousness, variance of neuroticism, variance of extraversion, and maximum openness would predict task-related criteria. On the relationship side, we used variance of conscientiousness, mean neuroticism, mean agreeableness, and mean extraversion for the hypotheses. Analyses and Results The analyses consisted of hierarchical multiple regressions (see Table 2 - 9). For each criterion, we first entered narrow traits in Block 1, followed by broad factors in Block 2. That procedure tests the predictive validity of broad factors over and above narrow traits. To assess the incremental validity of narrow traits, we also included analyses where we entered the Big Five in Block 1, followed by the narrow traits in Block 2. Finally, because the Big Five were operationalized according to our general hypothesis (e.g., using mean versus maximum or some other conceptualization), we wanted to assess the maximum predictive validity of the Big Five by selecting the “best Big Five” predictors of each criterion. Specifically, the Big Five factor operationalizations that had the strongest correlations with the criterion were entered. To be included in the analysis, the relation between each Big Five factor and criterion needed to be in a Broad versus Narrow Traits 5 theoretically meaningful direction, and only one Big Five trait operationalization could be selected for each trait (e.g., both mean conscientiousness and maximum conscientiousness could not be selected). The results in Tables 2 – 9 warrant at least two observations. First, the narrow traits accounted for significant variance when entered in Block 1 for all four criteria. The Big Five, however, were significant for two of the four criteria (i.e., for hypothesized Big Five) and three of the four criteria (i.e., for the best Big Five). Second, the narrow traits accounted for significant incremental variance in four of the eight analyses, whereas the Big Five never accounted for incremental variance. Discussion and Conclusion The results of this study support the notion that narrow traits can account for as much, or more, variance in team-related variables than the Big Five. These findings build on previous research that found conscientiousness, as a broad factor, to actually obscure meaningful relationships compared to a more fine-grained, narrow trait level of analysis (LePine, Colquitt, & Erez, 2000; LePine, 2003). Our findings show that narrow personality traits that are clearly defined and conceptually meaningful can compete with the Big Five. Note, however, that these narrow traits must be selected systematically, perhaps by using what Schneider, Hough, and Dunnette (1996) refer to as the construct-oriented approach. Stated simply, that approach stipulates that predictor traits should be selected based on their conceptual and theoretical link to the criterion variables. Indeed, our results suggest that this approach may be well suited, if not imperative, for aligning predictors with criterion at the team level in a meaningful, replicable, and theoretically sound way. Based on our review of the literature and the results reported in this study, we noted the following: Broad versus Narrow Traits 1) Broad personality factors, such as the Big Five, are not likely to consistently predict criteria unless the dimensions of each can be theoretically linked; 2) Precisely-defined, narrow personality traits are easier to interpret, as are their relations with team outcomes, primarily because theoretical linkages are tighter; 3) Narrow personality traits can predict team-level criteria equally as well as, or better than, the Big Five when the narrow traits are selected carefully. 6 Broad versus Narrow Traits 7 Table 1 Criterion variables requiring statistical justification for aggregation, and their respective scale references, reliability, mean rwg, ICC(1), ICC(2), and the number of teams dropped from the analyses due to low rwg. Variable Cohesion Reference Waldman et al., 2004 Potency Guzzo et al., 1993 Relationship Jehn, 1995 Conflict Reliability Mean rwg ICC(1) ICC(2) .85 .93 .43 .75 Number of teams where rwg > .70 1 .90 .92 .29 .61 1 .94 .94 .45 .76 3 Broad versus Narrow Traits 8 Table 2 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Potency Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F Hypothesized Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Maximum cognitive structure .25 Maximum organization .12 Mean achievement -.05 Mean endurance .26 Maximum innovation .08 Block 2 Mean conscientiousness SD extraversion SD neuroticism Maximum openness Best Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation Block 2 Maximum conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism .23 .15 5, 47 2.83* .08 .02 4, 43 1.28 .19 .15 5, 47 2.83* 5, 42 2.13 -.01 .10 -.32* .22 .25 .12 -.05 .26 .08 -.07 .16 .17 -.34* .16 *p < .05, **p < .01 .09 Broad versus Narrow Traits 9 Table 3 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Potency Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F Narrow traits over hypothesized Big Five Block 1 Mean conscientiousness .16 SD extraversion .08 SD neuroticism -.32* Maximum openness .29* Block 2 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation Narrow traits over best Big Five Block 1 Maximum conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism Block 2 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation *p < .05, **p < .01 .18 .11 4, 48 2.65* .13 .06 5, 43 1.66 .28 .21 5, 47 3.65** .11 .04 5, 42 1.45 .27 .01 -.09 .28 -.02 .12 .09 .19 -.32* .33* -.03 -.02 .17 -.11 Broad versus Narrow Traits 10 Table 4 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Relationship Conflict Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F .32 .24 5, 45 4.22** .10 .05 4, 41 1.78 .32 .24 5, 45 4.22** .11 .04 5, 40 1.52 Hypothesized Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Mean affiliation -.62** Mean sociability .25 Mean cooperativeness -.13 Mean defendence .02 Mean aggression .25 Block 2 Mean conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Mean extraversion Mean neuroticism Best Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression Block 2 SD conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism *p < .05, **p < .01 -.24 .01 .20 .33* -.62** .25 -.13 .02 -.08 -.08 .01 .00 .29* Broad versus Narrow Traits 11 Table 5 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Relationship Conflict Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F .25 .18 4, 46 3.76** Narrow traits over hypothesized Big Five Block 1 Mean conscientiousness -.28* Mean agreeableness -.27* Mean extraversion -.01 Mean neuroticism .29* Block 2 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression -.48* .08 .00 .16 .28 .2.45 Narrow traits over best Big Five Block 1 SD conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism Block 2 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression *p < .05, **p < .01 .11 5, 41 2.45* -.166 -.27 -.10 .26* .28 .20 5, 45 3.48** .15 .09 5, 40 2.01 -.47* .20 -.12 .08 .27 Broad versus Narrow Traits 12 Table 6 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Team Performance Personality Dimensions β R 2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F Hypothesized Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Maximum cognitive structure .23 Maximum organization .07 Mean achievement .36** Mean endurance -.35* Maximum innovation .10 Block 2 Mean conscientiousness SD extraversion SD neuroticism Maximum openness Best Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation Block 2 Maximum conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Variance extraversion Minimum neuroticism *p < .05, **p < .01 .19 .13 5, 60 2.90* .04 -.01 4, 56 .76 .19 .13 5, 60 2.90* .10 .05 4, 56 1.90 -.04 .19 -.02 .08 .23 .07 .36** -.35* .10 .09 .11 .14 -.21 Broad versus Narrow Traits 13 Table 7 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Team Performance Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F Narrow traits over hypothesized Big Five Block 1 Mean conscientiousness .06 Mean agreeableness .21 Mean extraversion -.06 Mean neuroticism .13 Block 2 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation Narrow traits over best Big Five Block 1 Maximum conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism Block 2 Maximum cognitive structure Maximum organization Mean achievement Mean endurance Maximum innovation *p < .05, **p < .01 .07 .01 4, 61 1.15 .17 .10 5, 56 2.43* .13 .07 4, 61 2.30 .16 .10 5, 56 2.52* .22 .09 .35* -.32* .02 .13 .17 .16 -.13 .19 .07 .33* -.36* -.11 Broad versus Narrow Traits 14 Table 8 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Team Cohesion Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F .24 .24 5, 45 2.84* .06 -.01 4, 41 .91 .24 .155 5, 45 2.84* .13 .07 4, 41 2.03 Hypothesized Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Mean affiliation .65** Mean sociability -.30 Mean cooperativeness .09 Mean defendence -.14 Mean aggression -.08 Block 2 Maximum conscientiousness Mean agreeableness Mean extraversion Mean neuroticism Best Big Five over narrow traits Block 1 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression Block 2 Mean agreeableness SD neuroticism Mean extraversion SD conscientiousness *p < .05, **p < .01 -.24 .24 .03 -.14 .65** -.30 .09 -.14 -.08 .30 -.19 -.37* .11 Broad versus Narrow Traits 15 Table 9 Multiple Regression Analyses for the Incremental Prediction of Team Cohesion Personality Dimensions β R2∆ Adj. R2∆ df F Narrow traits over hypothesized Big Five Block 1 SD conscientiousness .11 Mean agreeableness .32* Mean extraversion -.11 Mean neuroticism -.19* .15 Block 2 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression Block 2 Mean affiliation Mean sociability Mean cooperativeness Mean defendence Mean aggression *p < .05, **p < .01 4, 46 2.02 5, 41 2.68* .79** -.31 .05 -.14 .09 .21 Narrow traits over best Big Five Block 1 Mean agreeableness Maximum conscientiousness Maximum extraversion Maximum neuroticism .08 .14 .26 .11 .05 -.18 .17 .10 5, 46 2.32* .13 .05 5, 41 1.55 .63** -.38 .11 -.09 .05 Broad versus Narrow Traits 16 References Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., Neubert, M. J., & Mount, M. K. (1998). Relating member ability and personality to work-team processes and team effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 377-391. Dudley, N. M., Orvis, K. A., Lebiecki, & Cortina, J. M. (2006). A meta-analytic investigation of conscientiousness in the prediction of job performance: Examining the intercorrelations and the incremental validity of narrow traits. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 40-57. Kroeck, K. G., & Brown, K. W. (2004). Work applications of the Big Five model of personality. In J. C. Thomas (Vol. Ed.) & M. Hersen (Ed. in Chief), Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment: Industrial and organizational assessment (Vol. 4, pp. 109129). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Lepine, J. A. (2003). Team adaptation and postchange performance: Effects of team composition in terms of members’ cognitive ability and personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 27-39. Lepine, J. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Erez, A. (2000). Adaptability to changing task contexts: Effects of general cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and openness to experience. Personnel Psychology, 53, 563-593. Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big five factors and facets and the prediction of behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 411-424. Schneider, R. J., Hough, L. M., & Dunnette, M. D. (1996). Broadsided by broad traits: How to sink science in five dimensions or less. Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 17(6), 639655. Broad versus Narrow Traits Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703-742. 17