Synesthesia and Number Cognition in Children Jennifer A. K. Green & Usha Goswami Centre for Neuroscience in Education, University of Cambridge Correspondence address: Jennifer Green, Centre for Neuroscience in Education, University of Cambridge, 184 Hills Road, Cambridge, CB2 2PQ, UK. Tel. +44 1223 767600 or +44 1865 794116 E-mail: jakg2@cam.ac.uk WORD COUNT: 3050 excluding references 1 Abstract Grapheme-color synesthesia, when achromatic digits evoke an experience of a specific color (photisms), has been shown to be consistent, involuntary, and linked with number concept in adults, yet there have been no comparable investigations with children. We present a systematic study of grapheme-color synesthesia in children aged between 7 – 15 years. Here we show that such children (but not children with phoneme-color synesthesia) experience involuntary difficulties in numerical tasks when digits are presented in colors incongruent with their photisms. Synesthesia in children may thus have important consequences for certain aspects of numerical cognition. 2 Synesthesia, the involuntary multi-modal perception of objects or events that are usually perceived unimodally, has greatly interested neuroscientists (Rich & Mattingley, 2002). For example, seeing a digit like 5 may automatically elicit a simultaneous color percept or “photism” (grapheme-color synesthesia), whereas a taste may evoke a tactile sensation. Estimates of the prevalence of the condition range from 1 in 25,000 (Cytowic, 1996) to 1 in 20 (Galton, 1883), with grapheme-color synesthesia generally accepted as the most common form. Scientific investigation of synesthesia is expected to yield important clues to the neural mechanisms that underlie the integration of sensory information (Rich & Mattingley, 2002). However the study of synesthesia has so far focused exclusively on adults. This research strategy can only throw light on the end state of sensory integration. It cannot show whether synesthesia affects the development of sensory systems in ways that perturb the end state, or whether synesthesia has effects on cognitive developments that depend on sensory integration. Although widely assumed to be present from early childhood (Rich & Mattingley, 2002), studies examining synesthesia in children are limited to case studies from the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries (Riggs & Karwoski, 1934). It has been suggested that synesthetes are simply reporting idiosyncratic childhood associations between colors and numbers (Calkins, 1895). If it can be established that children experience grapheme-color synesthesia consistently (Baron-Cohen, Harrison, Goldstein, & Wyke, 1993; Baron-Cohen, Wyke, & Binnie, 1987) and automatically (Mattingley, Rich, Yelland, & Bradshaw, 2001; Mills, Boteler, & Oliver, 1999; Odgaard, Flowers, & 3 Bradman, 1999) as adults do, one obvious developmental question concerns potential effects on the acquisition of culturally-acquired cognitive skills such as numeracy and literacy. If grapheme-color photisms for numbers are involuntary, this could affect the development of certain aspects of number cognition. For example, if a young child automatically experiences the digit 1 as having the color green, will this affect her ability to learn that 9 is a larger count number if 9 is printed in green in her maths book? In other words, will color interfere with conceptual understanding of magnitude relations? Somewhat analogous confusions have been noted in the domain of literacy. One adult reported ‘I may call someone “Debbie” when she is really “Paula”, because D and P are more or less the same color green’ (Rich & Mattingley, 2002) Grapheme-color synesthesia has been associated anecdotally with deficient mathematical ability (Cytowic, 2003). In a recent large-scale survey, reports of weakness in the area of mathematics were significantly greater among synesthetes compared to a control group (Rich, Bradshaw, & Mattingley, 2005). Interestingly, however, the percentage of synesthetes who reported mathematics as an area of strength was also significantly greater than that of controls, although the number of synesthetes reporting an advantage for mathematics was much smaller. It is not clear whether and how the mathematical difficulties or advantages experienced by grapheme-color synesthetes might be impacted by the experience of color itself. While it is accepted that photsims are triggered automatically, it is not yet known how synesthetic color might affect the way numerical information is processed in the brain. However research with adult synesthetes suggests that the concept of a number alone can elicit photisms (Dixon, Smilek, Cudahy, & Merikle, 2000; Jansari, Spiller, & Redfern, 2006). Subjects 4 presented with single digit sums (e.g. 5 + 2) followed by color patches were slower to name the color of the patch when it was incongruent to the photism elicited by the solution. Additionally, while synesthesia was at first thought to occur in only one direction (e.g. numbers evoke colors, but colors do not evoke numbers) recent research suggests that co-activation occurs bi-directionally (Brugger, Knoch, Mohr, & Gianotti, 2004; Cohen-Kadosh & Henik, 2006a; Knoch, Gianotti, Mohr, & Brugger, 2005). It has also been demonstrated that color alone can evoke numerical magnitude in adult synesthetes (Cohen-Kadosh & Henik, 2006a, 2006b; Cohen-Kadosh et al., 2005). However, the developmental origins of this 2-way relationship are not known, as there have been no studies of preschool or school-aged children with synesthesia. We therefore recruited a group of children with photisms for digits, and explored whether these photisms were elicited automatically as in adults, and whether they played a role in memory for digits (for participant details, see Table 1). Children were asked to recall digits that were colored either congruently or incongruently with their photisms, or that were neutrally colored. Additionally, we investigated whether photisms can elicit a sense of numerical magnitude in children. Accurate counting involves learning the correspondences between Arabic numerals and semantic representations of set sizes (“numerosity”) (Landerl, Bevan, & Butterworth, 2004). As Arabic numerals are acquired, they are thought to access these semantic representations for numbers, leading to modality- independent size and distance effects (Dehaene, 1997). We adapted Besner and Coltheart’s (1979) size congruity paradigm, in which pairs of digits were presented with one of the digits being physically larger than the other. Participants were asked to ignore the physical size of the digits and to indicate 5 which was numerically larger. Adults were both slower to select the larger digit when it was presented as the physically smaller (incongruent), and faster when it was presented as the physically larger (congruent), than when both digits were of equal size (neutral). We created an analogous color congruity paradigm, in which pairs of digits of equal size were presented in colors that were either congruent with or incongruent with the child’s photisms. We expected participants to be slower to select the larger digit when it was in the “wrong” color, particularly if the incongruent color matched a numerically smaller digit than the comparitor. A similar effect has been documented with an adult synaesthete (Cohen-Kadosh & Henik, 2006a). Methods Participants. Six grapheme-color (GC) synesthetes, four girls and two boys, took part in the study. All of these children reported experiencing color when they saw or thought about numbers and letters. Five (all but G15) also reported color associations for days of the week, with three of these (G9a, G9b, and G13) also having color associations with the months of the year. One participant (G15) experiences color for musical notes. All volunteers were given a version of Baron-Cohen’s ‘Test of Genuineness’ (hereafter TOG) (Baron-Cohen, Harrison, Goldstein, & Wyke, 1993; Baron-Cohen, Wyke, & Binnie, 1987). Children were asked to fill out a color survey which asked them to list the colors they experienced for digits 0 – 9 and ten selected letters of the alphabet. They were then retested on their color assignments after a period of at least two weeks. If the child achieved at least 90% consistency on the TOG, parents were asked for written consent for participation in the main part of the study. Children were then asked 6 to choose the colors that they experienced for digits 0 – 9 on a color wheel to enable creation of the experimental stimuli. Two control groups were studied (see Table 2 for details). Thirty typically-developing control children of average cognitive ability were recruited from local schools and were matched to the GC synesthetes for age and gender. Five control children were matched for each of the six synesthetes in the GC group and received tasks based on that synesthete’s colors. It was envisaged that children would be matched based on IQ measures as well, but some of the synesthetes scored extremely highly on these measures and it was not possible to find absolute matches (see Table 1). A group of phoneme-color synesthetic children, one boy and three girls, formed a further control group. Phoneme-color synesthetes also experience color for numbers and letters but differ from grapheme-color synesthetes in that it is hearing letters or number words spoken that induces color experience rather than seeing them written or printed (BaronCohen, Harrison, Goldstein, & Wyke, 1993). Given the visual presentation format used, phoneme-color synesthetes were not expected to show effects of color congruency in the memory and number comparison tasks. These children were included to control for any general developmental effects of being a synesthete. These children tended to be less consistent on the color re-test of the Test of Genuineness, but all were above 80%. Tasks. Both experimental tasks were designed using EPrime psychological testing software and were presented on a laptop computer. 7 Memory for digits task. We used an adaptation of a matrix memory paradigm used with adults (Smilek, Dixon, Cudahy, & Merikle, 2002) simplified for use with children. Grids of either nine digits (3 x 3) or sixteen digits (4 x 4) were presented on the computer screen for a period of 60 seconds. After piloting with a separate control sample, only the 7 year-olds received the nine-digit grid. Children were then given a blank grid printed on a piece of paper and instructed to fill in as many digits as they could remember in the correct spatial positions. Children received three critical conditions, presented in a fixed order: neutral, congruent, incongruent. In the neutral condition, digits were presented in black on a gray background (or in dark gray on paler gray if the child had a black photism). In the congruent condition, digits were presented in the colors corresponding to the child’s photisms. In the incongruent condition, digits were presented in colors incongruent with the child’s photisms. As in the study by Smilek et al (2002), incongruent color assignments corresponded to digits one greater than the digit presented (e.g. 5 appeared in the color the child associated with 6). Children were scored for the number of correctly recalled digits in the correct locations out of 16 (scores from the 7 year-olds were prorated; child G7 scored 9/9, 9/9 and 5/9 in the neutral, congruent and incongruent conditions, respectively). Magnitude discrimination task. This task required children to decide which was the numerically larger of two same-sized digits presented on the computer screen, by pressing designated keys on the keyboard. The keys ‘A’ and ‘L’ were used, with stickers covering the letters. Children were instructed that ‘A’ should be pressed when the digit on the left was larger and ‘L’ when the digit on the right was larger. Digits were presented in 4 different color conditions: 8 ‘Neutral’ – digits printed in black (or if child had a black photism, gray) ‘Congruent’ – digits printed in colors congruent to the child’s photisms ‘Incongruent consistent’ – incongruent colors were chosen to correspond to the child’s photism for either the digit above (d + 1) or the digit below (d – 1) the target digit. The color assignments for both digits in the pair went in the same direction, so that if the pair 3 and 7 were presented, and 3 was assigned the color for 2 (d – 1), then 7 would be assigned the child’s color for 6 (also d – 1). The colors chosen therefore reflected the same larger-smaller relationship as the digits presented. ‘Incongruent inconsistent’ – here the inconsistent colors assigned were reversed, so that the smaller number (e.g. 3) was assigned the ‘larger’ photism (e.g. the color associated with 6) and the larger number (e.g. 7) was assigned the ‘smaller’ photism (e.g. the color associated with 2). Crucially the larger-smaller relationship was thereby reversed. A practice round of ten trials was administered so that children could become accustomed to pressing buttons and reacting to stimuli. None of the number pairs from the experimental trials appeared in the practice round. There were 64 experimental trials in all, sixteen in each condition, presented in pseudo-random order. Trials were presented in two sets of 32, each with a short rest break after 16. Results and Discussion 9 The number of digits recalled successfully in the memory for digits task in the neutral, congruent and incongruent conditions is shown in Figure 1. A clear effect of congruency was observed for the grapheme-color group only. Phoneme-color synesthetes and control children showed similar performance across conditions. Effects were confirmed by a 3 x 3 analysis of variance taking condition (neutral, congruent, incongruent) as the within-subjects factor and group (grapheme-color synesthesia, phoneme-color synesthesia, controls) as the between-subjects factor. There was a significant effect of condition (F (2, 74) = 3.65, p <.031, p2 = .09) and a significant interaction between condition and group (F (4, 74) = 3.168, p < .018, p2 = .15). Posthoc tests (Newman-Keuls) revealed a significant decrease in memory for digits in the incongruent condition for the grapheme-color synesthetes only (10.83 v 5.65, p < .01). Hence child grapheme-color synesthetes experience detrimental effects on memory for digits when digits appear in the “wrong” colors, just like adults (Smilek, Dixon, Cudahy, & Merikle, 2002). This detrimental effect was also reflected in the individual data, as shown in Table 3. All grapheme-color synesthetes except one (child G9b) showed poorest retrieval in the incongruent condition. Child G9b has particularly poor memory in all conditions. Child P11, a phoneme-color synesthete, also appears to show an effect of color incongruency for digit memory. This child may have performed the task by orally rehearsing the digits that were seen, thereby activating her synesthetic photisms. It may also be of note that this child was the only ‘projector’ – the only synesthete who reported 10 experiencing photisms as projected externally in space1, as opposed to experiencing them internally, or in ‘the mind’s eye’ (Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004)(Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004). Performance in the number comparison task was measured in terms of mean reaction time for the correct trials only. However, whereas most children made only 1 or 2 errors in the number comparison task, two children (G9a and G9b) made 29 and 14 errors respectively and were also very slow in their responses. When these two children were excluded from the data set, a Kruskal-Wallis test revealed no significant effect of group or condition. However, inspection of the data from the correct trials completed by G9a and G9b showed a clear incongruency effect. This is illustrated in Figure 4. Both children were around 400 ms slower in the incongruent inconsistent condition. This pattern suggests that for these children, magnitude relations were still being established: color incongruency interfered with the magnitude judgements required. The effect of color incongruency on judgments of numerical magnitude may thus be characteristic of a certain stage of mathematical development. According to some researchers, the automatic activation of magnitude information from Arabic digits develops around the third grade, when children are 8 or 9 years old (Girelli, Lucangeli, 1 She reports experiencing colour as ‘mists’ before her eyes when she hears letters or numbers. 11 & Butterworth, 2000). Hence the difficulties experienced by the 9-year-olds in this study may reflect their point on a developmental trajectory. This may also explain why the 7-year-old did not show an incongruency effect. Alternatively, given her extremely high score on the non-verbal IQ measure (see Table 1) and her excellent performance in the magnitude judgment task (errors and RTs comparable to the much older children) she may have advanced beyond the 9-year-olds in terms of the developmental trajectory for numerical skills. As will be recalled, G7 also showed excellent memory for numbers, in contrast to G9a and G9b. Note also that whereas in our study the three GC children aged from 11 – 15 did not show a color incongruency effect, Cohen-Kadosh et al. did find a color incongruency effect with an adult in an analogous (although not identical) magnitude judgement task. Clearly, it is not possible to draw firm conclusions regarding possible developmental effects from this small sample. Without testing more synesthetes, it is unclear whether this adult or our teenagers are atypical. Furthermore, studies with synesthetic adults suggest there is a high level of individual variability among synesthetes, even among those with the same form of synesthesia (Dixon, Smilek, & Merikle, 2004; Hubbard, Arman, Ramachandran, & Boynton, 2005). Hubbard and Ramachandran (2005; Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001) suggest that differences observed in behavioural performance on cognitive tasks may reflect differences in the neural mechanisms underlying the individual synesthesiae. Further, they propose that these difference might serve to categorise synaesthetes into two groups: ‘higher’ and ‘lower’, based on when during processing, and perhaps where in the brain, synaesthetic co-activations occur 12 (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001). Further group studies of both children and adults are needed in order to explore possible reasons for individual variability. This is the first study of which we are aware to report on grapheme-color and phonemecolor synesthesia in children. As in adults, synesthetic photisms appear to be consistent and involuntary. Most children with grapheme-color synesthesia showed a significant deterioration in memory for digits when the digits were presented in colors incongruent with their photisms, and children with less mathematical expertise (the 9-year-olds studied here) were also significantly slower to make judgments about the relative magnitude of numbers when colors were incongruent with their photisms. In fact, child G9b remarked, “In my head, some colors are bigger than other colors. I just get the feeling that blue is bigger than red, and green is bigger than orange”. Further developmental studies of synesthesia are required to understand potential effects on the development of different aspects of number cognition. Although synesthetes are frequently believed to have difficulties with mathematics (Cytowic, 2003; Rich et al., 2005), the number of participants in our study was too small to enable strong conclusions. Here, for all synesthetes except one (G9a, male), performance on a standardised test of mathematical reasoning was above the population mean, and sometimes remarkably so (see G13, G15, P11). This does not suggest impaired acquisition of mathematical concepts in these synesthetic children. Similarly, the synesthetes in this sample had good spatial reasoning, with scaled scores on the Block Design test as high as 17 and 19 (such scores are over 4 SD above the population mean). This is interesting with respect to the connections between spatial and 13 mathematical cognition now being documented (Hubbard, Piazza, Pinel, & Dehaene, 2005). However, the only synesthetes affected by color incongruency in the magnitude task were the two male participants, and they showed above-average spatial reasoning (scoring 1 – 2 S.D. above the population mean on the WISC block design subscale). Given that children as young as 7 years show consistent and involuntary graphemecolor synesthesia, studies with larger groups are clearly required to document fully the potential effects of synesthesia on the development of number cognition. 14 References Baron-Cohen, S., Harrison, J., Goldstein, L. H., & Wyke, M. (1993). Coloured speech perception: Is synaesthesia what happens when modularity breaks down? Perception, 22, 419 - 426. Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M., & Binnie, C. (1987). Hearing Words and Seeing Colours. Perception, 16(6), 761-767. Besner, D., & Coltheart, M. (1979). Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English Neuropsychologia, 17 467-472. Brugger, P., Knoch, D., Mohr, C., & Gianotti, L. R. R. (2004). Is digit-colour synaesthesia strictly unidirectional? Preliminary evidence for an implicity colored number space in three synaesthetes. Acta Neuropsychologica, 2(3), 252258. Calkins, M. W. (1895). Synaesthesia. American Journal of Psychology, 7, 90-107. Cohen-Kadosh, R., & Henik, A. (2006a). Color Congruity Effect: where do colors and numbers interact in synaesthesia? Cortex, 42, 259-263. Cohen-Kadosh, R., & Henik, A. (2006b). When a line is a number: color yields magnitude information in a digit-color synesthete. Neuroscience, 137, 3-5. Cohen-Kadosh, R., Sagiv, N., Linden, D. E. J., Robertson, L. C., Elinger, G., & Henik, A. (2005). When Blue is Larger than Red: Colors Influence Numerical Cognition in Synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1766-1733. Cytowic, R. (1996). Synesthesia: Phenomenology and Neuropsychology - a review of current knowledge. PSYCHE, 2(10). Cytowic, R. (2003). Synaesthesia: A Union of the Senses (2nd Edition ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press. Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. London: Penguin Books. Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Cudahy, C., & Merikle, P. (2000). Five Plus Two Equals Yellow. Nature, 406(27 July), 365. Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. (2004). Not all synaesthetes are created equal: Projector versus associator synaesthetes. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 335-343. Galton, S. F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London: Macmillan. Girelli, L., Lucangeli, D., & Butterworth, B. (2000). The Development of Automaticity in Accessing Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76(2), 104-122. Hubbard, E. M., Arman, A. C., Ramachandran, V. S., & Boynton, G. M. (2005). Individual Differences among Grapheme-Color Synesthetes: Brain-Behavior Correlations. Neuron, 45, 975-985. Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435-448. Hubbard, E. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2005). Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia. Neuron, 48, 5009-5520. Jansari, A. S., Spiller, M. J., & Redfern, S. (2006). Number Synaesthesia: When Hearing 'Four Plus Five' Looks Like Gold. Cortex. 15 Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., Mohr, C., & Brugger, P. (2005). Synesthesia: When colors count. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 372-374. Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of 8-9 year old students. Cognition, 93, 99-125. Mattingley, J. B., Rich, A. N., Yelland, G., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2001). Unconscious priming eliminates automatic binding of colour and alphanumeric form in synaesthesia. Nature(6828), 580-582. Mills, C. B., Boteler, E. H., & Oliver, G. K. (1999). Digit Synaesthesia: A Case Study Using a Stroop-Type Test. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16(2), 181-191(111). Odgaard, E. C., Flowers, J. H., & Bradman, H. L. (1999). An investigation of the cognitive and perceptual dynamics of a colour - digit synaesthete. Perception, 28(5), 651-664. Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia: A Window Into Perception, Thought and Language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 334. Rich, A. N., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2005). A Systematic, Large-scale Study of Synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexicalcolour associations. Cognition, 98(1), 53-84. Rich, A. N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2002). Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 43-52. Riggs, L. A., & Karwoski, T. (1934). Synaesthesia. British Journal of Psychology, 25(19), 29-41. Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., Cudahy, C., & Merikle, P. M. (2002). Synesthetic Color Experiences Influence Memory. Psychological Science, 13(6). 16 Table 1 – Characteristics of the grapheme-color (G) and phoneme-color (P) synesthetes. Synesthete Age Gender (years) BPVS WISC Blocks WISC Arithmetic G7 7.33 female 92a 19 11 G9a 9.83 male 104 14 8 G9b 9.33 male 125 12 G11 11.66 female 100 10 10 G13 13.58 female 160+ 10 18 G15 15.33 female 160+ 17 17 P9 9.33 male 101 14 P11 11.08 female 119 12 16 b P14a 14.16 female 131 10 10 P14b b 14.16 female 119 11 12 Note. Numerals represent age in years, dashes represent unavailable data. British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS) scores are standardised (population mean = 100, SD = 15) and Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (WISC) subtest scores are scaled (population mean = 10, SD = 1.5). a note that English is not this child’s first language b these two participants are twins Table 2 – Characteristics of the control groups (matched to grapheme-color synesthetes only). Control Group Age Gender BPVS WISC WISC (years) Blocks Arithmetic 7 year olds 7.50 female 116 14 (0.33) (16.01) (1.15) 9 year-olds (a) 9.45 male 114 12 (0.17) (10.37) (1.58) 9 year-olds (b) 9.18 male 118 10 (0.21) (15.94) (3.90) 11 year-olds 11.80 female 106 12 (0.08) (4.60) (2.59) 13 year-olds 13.29 female 114 10 (0.16) (6.19) (2.30) 15 year-olds 15.13 female 122 13 (0.25) (19.40) (2.95) Note. Ages and scores are presented as means with standard deviations in parentheses. N = 5 for each group. 17 Figure 1 – Mean number correct (out of 16) in the memory for digits task. 16 number correct out of 16 14 12 GC PC Controls 10 8 6 4 2 0 neutral congruent incongruent Figure 2 – Mean reaction times in ms for each group in each of the 4 color conditions in the Magnitude Discrimination task. 1200 mean reaction time 1000 GC PC Controls 800 600 400 Neutral Congruent Incongrent Consistent 18 Incongruent Inconsistent Table 3 – Individual scores for each synesthetic child on the Memory for Digits task out of 16. Neutral Congruent Incongruent 16 16 8.88 G7* 5 8 5 G9a 5 3 5 G9b 9 14 5 G11 14 12 4 G13 16 12 6 G15 12 12 10 P9 16 14 10 P11 11 10 15 P14a 10 9 10 P14b * scores for the 7 year old were prorated from the 9-digit grid 19 Figure 4 – Mean reaction times (in ms) of individual grapheme-color synesthetes in the magnitude discrimination task. Bars represent each of the four color congruency condition. G9a G7 G9b 2000 2000 2000 1800 1800 1800 1600 1600 1600 1400 1400 1400 1200 1200 1200 1000 1000 1000 800 800 800 600 600 600 400 400 400 200 200 200 0 0 0 G13 G11 G15 2000 2000 2000 1800 1800 1800 1600 1600 1600 1400 1400 1400 1200 1200 1200 1000 1000 1000 800 800 800 600 600 600 400 400 400 200 200 200 0 0 0 Neutral Congruent Incongruent Consistent 20 Incongruent Inconsistent