(Transcript of the email in a readable font) Please find my attached proposal for the 2010 ISRA conference. My preference would to present an Oral Paper, however I would do a poster if necessary. If any other information is required feel free to email me or reach me via phone at 914 629 0193. Thank you, Shanah Einzig Title: Difference in gender prevalence rates in psychopathy: Do the constructs of overt and covert aggression provide answers? Authors: Shanah Einzig, MA John Jay College of Criminal Justice & Dr. Diana M Falkenback, John Jay College of Criminal Justice Abstract: Current research has identified two types of aggression, overt (physical or verbal) and covert (relational), with men displaying more overt aggression and women displaying more covert aggression (Bjorqvist, Osterman, & Lagerspetz, 1994; Little, Jones, Henrich, & Hawley, 2003). Aggression is often linked with psychopathy, a set of characteristics that are typically divided into interpersonal traits (F1; Factor 1) such as charm, grandiosity, narcissism and lack of empathy, and behavioral traits (F2; Factor 2) such as impulsivity, antisocial behavior and delinquency. Closer examination of psychopathy indicates that aggression is more specifically linked with F2 traits. The literature suggests that there are no sex differences in the traits of F1, however sex differences appear in F2 (Strand & Belfrage, 2005) psychopathic traits, with women scoring lower on assessments of these traits. The current study aims to determine if: 1) sex differences exist in psychopathy total and factor scores using the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (Lilienfield & Andrews, 1996); 2) sex difference exist in overt and covert aggression as measured by the Little Aggression Inventory (Little, Jones, Henrich, & Hawley, 2003); and 3) these differences in overt/covert aggression explain the sex differences on the PPIII. Preliminary findings with 100 undergraduate psychology students showed that men and women did not differ significantly on total or F2 psychopathy scores, but women scored higher on F1. Analyses revealed that there was not a correlation between sex and overt/covert aggression. The research did not support the hypothesis that overt/covert aggression explains the sex differences in psychopathy. There are potentially important clinical implications, particularly in terms of the assessment of psychopathy in women, where findings suggest that sex differences in the expression of aggression need to be considered when assessing women. Summary: General Aggression Aggression, a construct that has been defined and studied in numerous ways, continues to be better understood in men than women. Less is known about female aggression, including the motivational factors that cause women to be aggressive, how women express aggression, and the developmental course of aggression in woman (Penny & Morett, 2007). Most research suggests that men are more aggressive (e.g., Baron and Richardson, 1994), however, those results may be due to differences in form of aggression used. Men tend to employ more obvious, or overt aggression (e.g., kicking, hitting, punching), described as verbal or physical aggression where there is harm, or threat of harm caused to others (Craig, 1998; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernbert, 2001). Women use more subtle, or covert, forms of aggression (e.g., gossip, refusal of friendship, ostracize, manipulating a relationship) (Bjorkqvist, et al., 1994; Little et al., 2003), which tend to be more non-verbal and indirect (Crick, 1996; Cullerton-Sen & Crick, 2005; Paquette & Underwood, 1999). Psychopathy Psychopathy is a disorder incorporating the characteristics of several personality disorders. Cleckley (1941) and Hare (1994) described the psychopath as manipulative, egocentric, impulsive and lacking empathy. Psychopathic traits are typically clustered into two factors; F1 corresponds to interpersonal and affective traits such as lying, grandiosity and lack of remorse and empathy, and F2 consists of social deviance and antisocial behaviors including impulsivity, juvenile delinquency, and poor behavioral controls (Berardino, Meloy, Sherman, & Jacobs, 2005; Hare, 1991). While research supports the construct validity and assessment of psychopathy in males, there is some controversy about psychopathy in females (Jackson et al., 2002; Strand & Belfrage, 2005). Vitale and colleagues (2002) found that the base rates of psychopathy in female offenders range from 9 to 23 percent compared to 15 to 30 percent in men. Additioanlly, females receive lower scores than males on the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003; Jackson et al., 2002; Vitale & Newman, 2001), leading some to implement a cutoff score adjustment in order to diagnose psychopathy in females (Jackson, Rodgers, Neumann, & Lambert, 2002; Vitale, Smith, Brinkley, & Newmann, 2002). Individual psychopathic traits may also be demonstrated differently in women (Bolt, Hare, Vistale, & Newman, 2004; Chapman, Gremore, & Farmer, 2003; Hamburger et al., 1996; Rutherford et at., 1995; Salekin et al., 1997). For example, research suggests impulsiveness in women is shown through self-harm, running away and manipulation, whereas men demonstrate impulsivity through violent behavior (Forouzan & Cooke, 2005). Additionally, some items on the PCL-R load on the factors differently across sex; one study found that three items (poor behavioral control, lack of realistic goals, and impulsivity) loaded on both F1 and F2 for female offenders, whereas these items loaded on only F2 for men (Salekin and colleagues, 1997). Some research also indicates that the sex differences on psychopathy are specific to the behaviorally based items of F2, while men and women tend to score similarly on the personality based items of F1 (Bolt, Hare, Vitale, & Newman, 2004; Krueger et al, 1994). It is unclear if the lower psychopathy scores and base rates for women are due to less expression of these traits, a difference of construct or inaccurate assessment of psychopathy in women. Psychopathy and Aggression Although there are various definitions of aggression used in the literature, research has linked various forms (how it is manifested) and functions (the intention) of aggression to the construct of psychopathy (Cornell et al., 1996; Harris, Rice, Cormier, 1991; Patrick & Zempolich, 1998). There are sex differences in the type of aggression displayed, with males displaying more overt aggression, and there are sex differences specific to F2 psychoapthic traits, with males displaying more of these traits (Forth et al. 1996). Given that F2 psychopathic traits are typically associated with overt aggression (Bolt, Hare, Vitale, & Newman, 2004; Hicks et al., 2004; Kennealy et al., 2007), perhaps a more specific assessment of the type of aggression and the type of psychopathic traits by sex can explain the rate discrepancies in psychopathy across the sexes. Current Study The current study investigated psychopathic traits and aggression across sex. For psychopathy, it was hypothesized that total psychopathy scores would be lower for females than males. Additionally, it was speculated that there would be no gender differences on F1 scores and that men would have higher F2 scores. Secondly, sex differences in overt and covert aggression were assessed, with the expectation that women would score lower on the overt scale and there would be no significant sex differences on the covert scale. Finally, this study investigated the interelations between psychopathy, aggression and sex. It was hypothesized that that only F2 would correlate with aggression, and specifically more to overt aggression than covert aggression, and sex differences in the specific types of aggression would account for the differences in F2 psychopathy scores. Participants 100 male and female undergraduate students from an urban college. Results Contrary to the hypothesis, independent t-tests showed that although men’s PPI Total scores (M = 456.04, SD = 28.7) were higher than females’ PPI Total scores (M = 439.83, SD = 40.43), the difference between the two groups only approached significance, (t (97), p = .08). Additional independent t-tests showed that there was no significant difference between men (M = 185.61, SD = 27.1) and women (M = 182.84, SD = 25.487), on the PPI-II (t (97) = .45, p = .65). However, contrary to hypotheses, there were differences between men (M = 150.65, SD = 16.38) and women (M = 140.42, SD = 22.01) on PPI-I scores (t (97) = 2.06, p = .04), with men scoring higher. Further investigation of aggression found that the difference in men and women’s scores (M= 6.68, SD = 2.75; M = 6.68, SD = 2.72, respectively) on the Little Aggression Inventory (Little, et al., 2003) subscale measuring overt aggression was non-significant (t (96) = -.004, p = .99). Independent t-tests showed that men’s LAI Covert scores (M = 8, SD = 3.28) were not significantly different than women’s (M = 7.58, SD = 2.99) scores (t (96) = .57, p = .57). Next, a significant positive correlation was observed between psychopathy and overt aggression as well as covet aggression (r(97) = .25, p < .005) (r(97) = .36, p <.001, respectively). Bivariate correlation analyses investigating the relationship between F1 and overt and covert aggression (r(97) = -.03, p =ns); r(97) = .03, p = ns, respectively)were not significant. However, F2 was significantly correlated to both overt aggression (r(97) = .33, p < .01) and covert aggression (r(97) = .42, p < .01). Discussion The present study demonstrates no sex differences in overt and covert aggression, psychopathy total and F2, however women in this sample scored lower on F1 psychopathy traits. When aggression was teased apart as either overt or covert aggression, the results showed that men and women did not differ significantly in use of overt aggression or covert aggression. The dichotomy of overt-covert aggression used in this study is in the initial phases of investigation and in need for more validation research; it is possible it lacks discriminative validity as the two scales were significantly correlated (r = .74, p < .01). Further, although the majority of literature indicates the idea that men use overt aggression more than women, more current research supports that men decrease their amount of overt aggression as they age. Males develop later than females, making them more likely to use physical aggression because it does not rely much on cognitive thinking (Hay, 2007). As boys age, it becomes less socially acceptable for them to use physical aggression and therefore the energy that was once used for physical overt aggression is transformed into covert aggression. Adult males may be as capable of using indirect aggressive strategies because they have acquired verbal and social skills equivalent to those of adult females (Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & Kaukiainen, 1992; Kaukiainen et al., 1999). As the current sample is of undergraduates aged 18 to 33 years, the findings are supported by the developmental theory that at a certain age, typically in adolescence (Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz & Kaukiainen, 1992), men start to use covert instead of overt aggression. Almost all studies, regardless of the measures used, find that men have a higher scores on psychopathy compared to women (Forth et al., 1996; Hare, 1991, 2003; Rutherford et al., 1995; Salekin et al., 1997; Strand & Belfrage, 2005). Contrary to previous findings, results from this study did not reveal any significant differences in total psychopathy. However the sex difference approached significance, suggesting the possibility that with a larger sample size a significant difference, similar to those found in the literature, might be found. Because women in this sample had similar scores on the overall PPI to men’s scores, a finding dissimilar to most research displaying men scoring significantly higher than women on overall PPI (Forth, Brown, Hart, & Hare, 1996; Hare, 1991; Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1997 Zagon & Jackson, 1994), and similar aggression scores, it is understandable that the sex differences in correlation with the two factors of psychopathy would not be evidenced. Men should be scoring higher on F2 and should be the same on F1 compared to women, yet in this sample women scored equal on F2 and higher on F1, keeping the predicted proportions correct. This finding is consistent with findings from Levenson and colleagues (1995) as they found that men scored higher on F1. The current study found that F2 was correlated to both overt and covert aggression, consistent with the results by Schmeelk and colleagues (2008). F1, which measures interpersonal features, and thought to be correlated to covert aggression, was not correlated to either overt or covert aggression. Perhaps theory is wrong in expecting different types of aggression to be correlated to each factor (Skeem et al, 2003; Warren & Clabour, 2009) as aggression as a whole is associated with F2 (Walters, 2003), a similar finding to Schmeelk and colleagues’s (2008) results that relational aggression is correlated with F2 even though the PPI’s F2 does not contain any precise aggression items Contrary to theoretical understandings, the connection between the interpersonal and behavioral aspects defining psychopathy and the present dependent variables of overt and covert aggression were comparable across the sexes, similar to findings by Penny and colleagues (2007). Limitations Although this study provides additional understanding of psychoapthic traits and aggression in females, there are limitations to the interpretation of the conclusions. First, generalizability of the results may be limited because of the use of an undergraduate sample. Additionally, the sample size was 100 participants, therefore there may not have been enough power to find real differences. This lack of power is particularly important because a number of analyses were approaching significance. Second, although many research studies utilize self-report measures, there is an ongoing discussion as to the use of such measures for the assessment of psychopathic traits (Hare, 1993). The use of selfreport measures may influence how honest an individual is in answering the questions. Future research should validate a non-self-report measure that reliably assesses psychopathic-like traits in a non-forensic population. Research has started to define psychopathy in women differently from the terms used to describe psychopathy in men. Men and women were shown to have similar scores on the psychopathy measures and measures assessing different types of aggression. As many of the hypotheses predicted were not supported, further research is necessary in order to understand female psychopathy and aggression. If the current findings are indeed replicated in future studies, this means that clinicians have become attuned to the female psychopath and therefore research should start to provide additional support showing the limited differences between the psychopathic traits of men and women.