Paper accepted for publication at the European Journal of Developmental Psychology Emotional expressions during early infant-father conversation Theano Kokkinaki Department of Psychology University of Crete, Greece Work address: University of Crete Department of Psychology 74 100 Rethymnon – Gallos Crete – Greece tel:++30 – 28310 - 77536 fax:++30 – 28310 – 77578 e-mail:nina@psy.soc.uoc.gr Home address: 26, Leutheraiou Street, 71 305 Heraklion, Crete – Greece tel:++30 – 2810 – 261188 Keywords: emotional matching, emotional attunement, facial expressions of emotion, infant-father interaction. 1 Notes and Acknowledgements The data of the present study are derived from the Ph.D study of the researcher (Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, under the supervision of Professor Colwyn Trevarthen), for which ethical approval has been granted by the Royal Infirmary of Edinburgh, NHS Trust (8/95). I gratefully acknowledge Professor Giannis Kugiumutzakis (University of Crete) and Dr Tricia Striano (Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences) for their assistance and invaluable advice, and Assistant Professor Vassilis Vasdekis (Athens University of Economics and Business) for his comments on the statistical analysis of this article. Above all, I am deeply indebted to the infants and their families for “sharing” their time, cooperation and patience to participate in this study. 2 Abstract The present longitudinal and naturalistic study aims to investigate fathers’ and infants’ facial expressions of emotions during paternal infant-directed speech. The microanalysis of infant and paternal facial expressions of emotion in the course of the naturalistic interactions of 11 infant-father dyads, from the 2nd to the 6th month, provided evidence that: a) fathers and infants match their emotional states and attune their emotional intensity, b) infants seem to match paternal facial emotional expressions more than vice versa, c) the prevailing emotional states of each partner remain constant in the beginning and at the end of speech, d) the developmental trajectories of infant interest and paternal pleasure change significantly across the age range of 2-6 months and they seem to follow similar courses. These results are interpreted in the frame of the theory of innate intersubjectivity. Introduction The aim of the present longitudinal and naturalistic study was to analyze facial expressions of emotions of fathers and infants in the course of spontaneous paternal infant-directed speech taking place in dyadic infant-father interactions during early infancy. The temporal organization of dyadic emotional states in face-to-face parent - infant interaction has been described through the application of various terms which imply differences in conceptualizing how infants express their understanding by engaging in reciprocal responses to the adult’s social signals (see also Muir, Lee, Hains & Hains, 2005). In “co-regulated” mutual matching both partners are trying to match each other, and since this happens in a continuous way, each partner ends up trying to match an action that is partly the self's and partly the other's reflected back to the first (Fogel, 1993). The mutual and continuous nature of anticipatory action is reflected in 3 the concept of “mutual coordination” (Beebe and Jaffe, 1992, cited by Fogel, 1993). “Mutual regulation” has been defined as the goal to achieve a joint regulation of the interaction with interactive behaviors, or the goal of achieving a joint state of reciprocity. The quality of each partner's display relative to the quality of the other's display is a measure of match existing between their intentionality and affectivity. When synchronization is not achieved, there is a mismatching and lack of cyclic build-up of the behaviors into a smooth flow of phases (Brazelton, Tronick, Adamson, Als & Wise, 1975; Weinberg, Tronick, Cohn and Oslon, 1999). Lately, Tronick (2005) proposed that “dyadic states of consciousness” – the successful regulation of meaning leading to the emergence of a mutually induced dyadic state of meaning and states of consciousness have an intensity and force. The emotion brought into a state of consciousness may be one variable affecting intensity. Dyadic states of consciousness with greater force and intensity are ones that assemble more private meaning from each individual into shared meanings. “Affect attunement” is a form of matching that excludes imitation, in which the referent for the match is not the external action, but the presumed feeling state of the partner (Stern, Hofer, Haft & Dore, 1985). “Intersubjectivity” is the process in which mental activity – including conscious awareness, motives, intentions and emotions – is transferred between minds (Trevarthen, 1993a, 1993b, 2005). Limited evidence on the temporal organization of emotional states in infant-father interactions come from comparisons with mother-infant interaction - within different theoretical frames - and provides evidence of differences and similarities in the intensity of dyadic mutual regulation (Kokkinaki, 2003; Yogman, 1982). In the present study we hypothesized that emotional coordination (emotional matching of dyadic facial expressions or emotional attunement of dyadic emotional 4 intensity) between infants' and fathers' emotional expressions would be evident in the course of natural paternal infant-directed speech. This hypothesis is based on the welldocumented communicative function of infant-directed speech (Papousek, Papousek & Bornstein, 1985; Trevarthen, 2005). Studying facial expressions of emotions during spontaneous paternal infant-directed speech is important due to the suggestion that infant's preference for infant-directed speech cannot be explained solely by the information contained in pitch contours, since male and female speakers use different features to convey the same affective messages to their infants (McCartney, 1997; Slaney & McRoberts, 2003). Method Recruitment and Participants After ethical approval had been obtained (see notes), participants were recruited through the assistance of local obstetricians and pediatricians. Parents were informed with respect to the procedure of the study and were asked to sign the consent form prior to video-recording. Visits at infants’ home and video-recordings were scheduled at a time when the infants were reported by their mothers to be alert and playful and when the father was at home. Eleven infant-father pairs (N=22) participated in this study. The fathers’ mean age was 33.63 years (SD = 5.69, range : 27-47 years). All the 11 infants (5 males and 6 females) were full-term and healthy. The infants’ mean birth weight was 3568 gr (SD = 487.479, range : 2800-4250 gr), and the mean birth height was 52.22 cm (SD = 2.160, range : 48-55cm). Procedure Video-recordings were made at 15-day intervals, starting when the infant was 2 months old until he/she was 6 months old. Each recording lasted 8-10 minutes (8 5 minutes for the younger infants aged 2-4 months and 10 minutes for the older infants aged 4.5 to 6 months). Over the course of the study, nine video-recordings were made for each infant. A total of 99 video-recordings were made for the entire sample while the total duration of video-recorded and analyzed interactions was 880 minutes. The only instruction given to the fathers was: “Play as you normally do with your baby”. The recordings took place in a room and a position chosen by the fathers prohibiting any third-party intervention. If the infant became distressed or either the father or the researcher considered that the visit should be postponed for some reason, it was rescheduled as soon as possible thereafter. All recordings were made with a Panasonic NV-MS4 S-VHS HI-FI STEREO camera. Coding In order to micro-analyze the facial expressions of emotions of each and both partners in the course of spontaneous paternal infant-directed speech in a reliable way, the flow of interaction had to be structured in a well-defined way. This was the reason that paternal infant-directed speech was classified into verbal expression categories and grouped into units and subunits of analysis, independently of the fact that this categorization is not related to the research questions of this paper. Paternal infant-directed speech was transcribed verbatim from the videorecording, verified against the original tapes, and categorized into the following verbal expression categories: 1) infant-focus paternal utterances, described the infant's internal and external state and responded to the infant's non-vocal cues, 2) fatherfocus paternal utterances described the father's own behavior(s), desire(s) and presence, 3) dyad-focus paternal utterances described the father's attempt to: a) communicate with the infant through vocal expression and/or verbal response, b) describe the dyadic emotional / behavioral exchange and, c) express “sharing” 6 (through the use of “we”) of behaviors/physiological states/body parts/gaze direction with the infant, 4) other-focus paternal utterances described an external situation, an object/toy or refers to a third person, 5) nursery songs, repeated vocalization(s), sentence(s) and non-speech sound(s), non-responsive to infant cues, were analyzed as separate no-focus categories. This scheme adapted and extended certain parts of the categorization system developed by Murray and her colleagues (Murray and Trevarthen, 1986; Murray, Kempton, Woolgar and Hooper, 1993). Each of the above categories of paternal utterance and the pauses following them were micro-analysed (in accuracy of 1/25th of a second) in order to be grouped into units and subunits of analysis. The unit of analysis was defined as a temporal period which began at the start of one paternal verbal expression category and it ended at its termination. Each unit of analysis contained one or more categories of verbal expression, that is, subunits of analysis, depending on the pause duration between these categories. In the case that the pause between two categories was shorter or equal to 2 seconds then these categories constituted part of the same unit of analysis and the transcription and analysis of paternal speech was continued within this unit of analysis. If the pause between two categories was longer than 2 seconds then a new unit of analysis began. The 2-second pause has been judged adequate for the change of content of parental utterances (Herrera, Reissland and Shepherd, 2004). Diagram 1 shows an example of categorization of paternal infant-directed speech into units and subunits of analysis, the accompanying paternal and infant facial expressions of emotions and emotional intensity for each sub-unit of analysis, the elapsed time and the video duration in an eight digit number string (these eight digits are four pairs of numbers representing the time in hours, minutes, seconds and frames in twenty fifths of a second, which was then transformed into seconds). 7 (Insert Diagram 1 about here) In the cases of infant physiological need (e.g. infant vomit), micro-analysis was interrupted independently from the pause duration. Paternal utterances that were either inaudible or un-intelligible in meaning, were excluded from the analysis, but this occurred rarely. Paternal whispers were analyzed given that they constitute 14.6% of paternal utterances in early infancy (Papousek, Papousek & Bornstein, 1985), along with physiological sounds, since fathers respond often to these expressions by imitation (Kokkinaki, 1998) and verbal comments. Within each subunit of analysis, the coding scheme described six types of facial expressions [happy (pleasure directed to the partner/external world, interest directed to the partner), neutral (neutral expression, interest directed to the external world) and sad], three qualities of valence (positive, neutral and negative), and four categories for the direction of intensity change (ascending, descending, stable and fluctuating). The pleasure facial expression directed to the partner was coded when the eyes are open, focused and attentive to the partner or they convey bright and animated pleasure, mouth is elongated, that is closed, slightly open or open in the horizontal plane, the corners of the mouth are slightly pulled back and/or drawn upwards, the lips are slightly or more distinctly stretched causing wrinkles on each side of the mouth and the cheeks are slightly drawn upwards or raised. Interest directed to the partner was defined according to eye contact, gaze or orientation to the other partner’s face or body. When one partner is gazing at the other partner’s face or body, interest was signalled by an unsmiling face, with open eyes. The lips are usually open or at other times loosely closed. When the lips are open, the 8 corners of the mouth are slightly downward, with the upper lip in a reversed-U shape, and the lower lip is relaxed or slightly stretched. In addition to gaze behaviour, one or a combination of the following facial or vocal expressions may occur: a) raised eyebrows, b) knitted eyebrows, c) blinking, d) cooing (for the infant) or other vocalizations (infant, father), e) pre-speech mouth movements (infant). Neutral expression was identified when one subject was not alert while he/she was looking passively or orienting himself to own or partner’s body. The expression should be one of an unsmiling face, with no signs of body movement, vocalization or intent to vocalize (pre-speech mouth movements). In another situation, neutral was coded when the infant showed signs of self-absorption (looking passively own body/parts of the body) or sleepiness (yawning, rubbing eyes). Interest/Pleasure directed to the inanimate world was identified when one partner showed signs of interest and/or pleasure addressed not to the other partner, but to external objects. Negative facial expression was signified by a furrowed brow (lines on the forehead), wrinkles around the eyes and the nose, tight and somewhat protruded lips, mouth either open or closed, corners of the mouth slightly downward or pulled downward while the infant is gazing at or away from the parent’s face or trunk. The denotation of parent’s emotional expression as “negative” was extremely rare. This was signified in cases in which the parent showed signs of annoyance that was rather expressed by verbalization than by facial expression. Coding of the infant and paternal facial expressions of emotions was continuous, since the onset time in the expression of an emotion of one partner was also the offset time of the previous emotion of the same partner. In the course of one subunit of analysis in the flow of paternal speech, it was most likely for each partner 9 to express more than one emotional state. In order to obtain a description of the change of intensity over time, each facial expression within each category of emotional valence represented a symbol in the following scale: (a) positive emotional valence consisted of pleasure directed to the partner (+++), pleasure directed to the inanimate world (++), and interest directed to the partner (+), (b) neutral emotional valence consisted of the neutral facial expression and the interest directed to the external world (0), and (c) negative emotional valence consisted of the negative facial expression (-). After the microanalysis of paternal and infant facial expressions of emotions within each subunit of analysis, emotional matching and mismatching, or the sequential combination of them, were determined for each subunit, according to the type and the sequence of facial expression of both partners within this subunit. In the present study, emotional matching was coded when one partner (2) expressed the valence (positive, neutral, negative) of facial expression of emotion of the other partner (1) before the first completes it, independently of the intensity of it (e.g. emotional matching of pleasure was coded when father expressed laughter while the infant expressed grin). The temporal organization of the emotional matching, within each subunit of analysis, showed the direction of it, that is, which partner expressed first an emotion which was then expressed by the other partner. For example, infant emotional matching was coded when the father expressed pleasure in the course of one subunit, while the infant, in the course of the same subunit, expressed self-absorption in the beginning, which was then changed to pleasure. It has to be clarified that the termination of the facial expression of emotion of the second partner could occur either before or after the termination of the facial expression of the first partner (photographs 1a, 1b and 2a, 2b). 10 (Insert photographs 1a, 1b, 2a and 2b about here) In addition, when one subunit of analysis of paternal speech began and ended, or just began, with emotional matching, the direction of it was not coded since the matching was either simultaneous, or it had started during the previous pause of paternal speech. Emotional mismatching was coded when one or either partner was not interested in interacting with the other partner. In the first case, one partner expressed interest or pleasure to the partner, while the other was neutral or negative in emotion. When either partner was not interested in communication, one partner was neutral in emotion while the other was negative. The sequence of the above symbols of each partner in the course of each subunit of paternal infant-directed speech determined the direction identified for the change of emotional intensity as follows: a) Ascending is defined as a sequence in which the intensity of the last emotional state of one partner at the end of the verbal expression category is higher in the scale than the intensity of the first emotional state of the same partner in the beginning of this category, e.g. when the infant changes from interest (+) to pleasure (+++) directed to the partner, b) Descending is defined as a sequence in which the intensity of the last emotional state of one partner at the end of the category is lower in the scale than the intensity of the first emotional state of the same partner in the beginning of this category, e.g. when interest (+) is followed by a negative emotional state (-), c) Fluctuating sequence is defined as one in which the intensity of the first and the last emotional states of one partner is the same in position in the scale, while the intermediate intensity(s) are different, e.g. when pleasure to the partner (+++) is followed by neutral emotional state (0) and this is followed by pleasure again (+++) and, d) Stable category refers to an unchanging 11 intensity of emotional state of one partner in the whole course of a category, e.g. a partner expresses interest throughout the paternal verbal expression. Inter-observer reliability assessments were made for the facial expressions of emotions and the intensity of paternal and infant emotional states. Inter-observer reliability for all categories ranged from 0.76 to 0.86, the mean value of k for all categories being 0.80. Scores obtained for the facial expressions of emotions ranged from 0.76 to 0.85, and those for emotional intensity ranged from 0.76 to 0.86. After the end of inter-observer reliability assessments, the two scorers discussed and corrected each assessment on which there was disagreement. The statistical analysis was carried out after all the corrections were made on the data set. Statistical Analysis This experiment, by its nature, generated data with relatively strong dependencies between them since repeated observations on time are obtained on a relatively small number of individuals. These longitudinal dependencies were not taken into account in the analysis because of their complexity. Therefore, for the statistical analysis, the chi-square test of independence was used to determine possible relationships between pairs of categorical variables. This would tend to increase the number of significant results. For this reason, the significance level was set at 1% (instead of 5%), as a safeguard against false rejections of the null hypothesis, increasing therefore the power of the test. No chi-square value was considered as valid if a minimum expected frequency less than one was found. For convenience, when results are presented in a table, the independent variable will always be presented in the columns. Conclusions based on these tables, will be made as regards the distribution of the dependent variable, for each value of the independent variable. When there was a disagreement between frequencies and percentages, the conclusions will be reached in relation to 12 percentages since inferences on the basis of frequencies can be misleading when different sample sizes are used for different levels of explanatory variables. All analysis were performed using SPSS statistical package (Version 13, 2004). Results In the course of 880 minutes of free infant-father interaction a total of 4386 subunits of analysis of paternal infant-directed speech were recorded. Given that the investigation of the relationship between infant and paternal emotions was the main interest of this analysis, any relationship would be of interest provided that it concerned the common predominant emotions of both partners, that is, paternal pleasure and interest (81.2% and 40%, respectively) and infant pleasure and interest (27.4% and 59.5%, respectively). A relationship between the fathers' and the infants' emotions (χ2 = 137.99, df = 4, p<0.001) (Table 1) showed that when the father was expressing pleasure, the infant was also pleased (22.5%) more than when the father was showing interest (6.6%) or pleasure and interest directed to the father, sequentially (4.5%). When the father was showing interest, the infant was more likely to be interested (89.7%) than when the father was pleased (52.1%) or pleased and interested (44.3%). When the father was expressing pleasure and interest, the infant was more likely to express both emotions (51.1%) than when the father was pleased (25.4%) or interested (3.7%). (Insert Table 1 about here) Descriptive analysis of the emotional matching, mismatching and their combination, provided evidence that emotional matching occurred in 1140 (26%) subunits, emotional mismatching was analysed for 1276 (29.1%) subunits and their combination described the dyadic emotional states of 1970 (44.9%) subunits. 13 As long as the direction of emotional matching is concerned, the following description and the analysis that regards the relationship between the direction of emotional matching and infant’s age is based on the frequencies that represent infant and paternal emotional matching derived from the subunits that contained one matching and from the first emotional matching of the subunits that contained more than one matching. The present data provided evidence that emotional matching by the infant occurred in 1815 (76.7%) subunits of analysis and predominated over emotional matching by the father which was coded in 551 (23.3%) subunits (p<0.0001, two-tailed Binomial test). Independently of the direction of emotional matching, a relationship between infant and paternal emotional intensity (χ2 = 471.06, df = 9, p < 0.001) (Table 2) shows that when the emotions of the father were ascending, the infant’s emotions were mostly ascending (36%) than when the father was descending (17.6%), stable (18%) or fluctuating (22.6%). Similarly, when the father’s emotions were descending, stable or fluctuating, the infant’s emotional intensity changed rather in the same than a different direction. (Insert Table 2 about here) Even, when the direction of emotional matching was taken into consideration, a relationship between infant and paternal emotional intensity, during the course of the subunits in which the infant matched the emotions of the father (χ2 = 52.674, df=4, p<0.0001), revealed that when the emotions of the father were ascending, the infant's emotions were mostly ascending (56.4%) than when paternal emotions were descending (27.7%), or fluctuating (29.2%). When the father's emotions were descending, the infant's emotions were mostly descending (24.6%) than when paternal emotions were ascending (10.7%), or fluctuating (20.2%). Similarly, when paternal 14 emotions were fluctuating, the infant’s emotions were mostly fluctuating (50.2%) than when paternal emotions were ascending (32.9%), or descending (47.7%). Infant and paternal categories of stable emotional intensity were eliminated due to the low frequencies of infant stable category. Similarly, a relationship between infant and paternal emotional intensity, during the course of the subunits in which the father matched the emotions of the infant (χ2 = 52.304, df = 4, p<0.0001), revealed that when the emotions of the infant were ascending, the father's emotions were mostly ascending (66.7%) than when infant emotions were descending (29.5%) or fluctuating (33.5%). Similarly, when the infant's emotions were descending or fluctuating, the father's emotional intensity changed rather in the same (24.1% and 48.1%, respectively) than a different direction. A relationship between paternal predominant emotions in the beginning and at the end of spontaneous paternal speech (χ2 = 1780.86, df = 1, p < 0.001) shows that when the father was pleased in the beginning of free paternal speech, it was more highly probable to remain pleased at the end of paternal infant-speech than when he was interested in the beginning of paternal infant-directed speech. Further, when the father was interested in the beginning of paternal infant-directed speech, he was mostly likely to remain interested at the end of it than when he was pleased in the beginning of paternal infant-directed speech. Similarly, a relationship between infant predominant emotions in the beginning and at the end of spontaneous paternal speech (χ2 = 1573.38, df = 4, p < 0.001) provides evidence that when the infant was pleased, interested to the father or expressed interest/pleasure to the inanimate world in the beginning of paternal speech, he/she was more likely to express the same respective emotional expressions at the end of it. 15 The analysis for the relationship between infant’s age and partners’ emotions was carried out only for the prevailing emotions of infant interest and paternal pleasure. Chi-square analysis showed a significant relationship between infant interest and age (χ2 = 71.520, df = 8, p < 0.001) and paternal pleasure and age (χ2 = 37.88, df = 8, p < 0.001). As seen in Figure 1, the developmental curves of infant interest and paternal pleasure seem to be similar across the age range of 2 to 6 months. (Insert Figure 1 about here) Discussion This study aimed to investigate the facial expressions of emotions of infants and fathers in the course of free paternal infant-directed speech taking place in dyadic naturalistic interactions from the second to the sixth month of life. The present study provided evidence that in the course of spontaneous paternal infant-directed speech, fathers and infants coordinated their emotional expressions, either in the form of emotional matching (one partner expressed the valence of the facial expression of emotion of the other partner), or in the form of emotional attunement (one partner matched the shifts of emotional intensity of the other partner). The evidence of emotional attunement between infant and paternal expressions, is reinforced the quantification of Table 2, which shows the relationship between paternal and infant emotional intensity. One reasonable way to quantify attunement is to see the ratio of the cases in which attunement occurred to the total number of the observed cases. The sum of the frequencies of the central diagonal to the total number of cases (171 + 63 + 1864 + 136 = 2234 / 4386 = 0.50) provides such an index. The evidence of infant and paternal emotional matching and attunement of facial expressions in the course of fathers' speech implies that the emotional context 16 accompanying paternal infant-directed speech constitutes an example of intersubjectivity, that is, the process in which mental activity – including motives and emotions – is transferred between minds (Trevarthen, 1998b; Trevarthen, Kokkinaki & Fiamengi, 1999). In particular, the evidence provided implies that both fathers and infants adjust the timing, form and energy of their emotional facial expressions to obtain inter-synchrony. Timing adjustment is evidenced by the temporal structure of emotional matching implied by the definition of it. This may imply that infants not only are they able to adjust or fit their emotions to the emotions of others, but they participate actively and control to a great extent the exchange. In this way infants may compensate or counter-balance for the predominance of parental over infant imitation of other expressive behaviors. (Kokkinaki, 1998; Kugiumutzakis, 1993). This suggestion may be integrated in the hypothesis that communication is regulated by an integrated system of equivalent expressions, in which most responses are complementary translations of the partners expressions (Trevarthen, 1993a). Despite the fact that in this study, the coding of facial expressions of emotions did not include fine-grained measurements of muscle activity, emotional matching of facial expressions implies, indirectly, adjustment of spatial patterns or forms of muscle activity. Given that the human facial communicative system has a highly differentiated facial anatomy “...attached a bisymmetric set of muscle units that are differentially excitable...” (Trevarthen, 1985, p. 22), and that “...the discrete facial muscle actions visible in the adult can be identified and finely discriminated in newborns” (Oster and Ekman, 1978, cited by Field, 1982, p.284), matching of facial expressions of emotions implies adjustment or fitting of the spatial patterns of father's and infants' facial muscle activities. 17 Energy adjustment is evidenced by emotional attunement of facial expressions of emotions, as implied by the matching of emotional shifts. The present study provided evidence that when the father's emotional intensity was rising, infants were found to attune in a sympathetic ascending way, as if they agreed on rising the emotional intensity of interaction. When the father's emotions were descending, infant's emotional tone fell, as if infants consented on leading the interaction into disruption. When fathers' expressions of emotions fluctuated, infants' emotional tone was also fluctuating, either because of the infant's effort to search for stability, or because infants were trying to subtly attune to all the shifts in the father's emotional tone. Further, when the father's emotions were stable in quality, infants did not change their emotional expressions, implying their intention to facilitate and maintain interaction. These regulating patterns provide evidence that infants can experience both self-awareness and other-awareness as well as awareness of purposeful sharing of emotional states (Trevarthen, 1993a, 1993b). This suggestion is reinforced by the “consistency” and “stability” in infants’ and fathers’ emotional states and emotional intensity in the course of spontaneous paternal infant-directed speech. This kind of consistency suggests that paternal infantdirected speech constitutes an expressive system featured by self-regulatory emotions aimed for coherence in awareness, consciousness and purpose, unified intentionality and control of internal states and processes (Izard, 1992; Trevarthen, 1993b). The evidence of similar courses of infant interest and paternal pleasure and the non-linear developmental course of infant emotions provided by this study implies periodic reorganizations in the infant's motivational system (Kugiumutzakis, 1993; Trevarthen, 1998a), which may give rise to either corresponding motivational changes in fathers, or to changes in his perception of the moods and interests of infants. The 18 similarity of developmental curves imply the dyadic propelling of development by the interaction of motivational changes in both infants and fathers (Trad, 1990). In sum, the present study provided evidence that the emotional context accompanying paternal infant-directed speech during early infancy constitutes an example of intersubjective coordination (evidenced by mutual adjustment of timing, form and energy of emotional expressions) and intra-subjective regulation (evidenced by the “consistency” in infants’ and fathers’ emotional states and emotional intensity). This evidence highlights the need for more intensive research on the emotional aspects of paternal-infant-directed speech given that the focus on facial expressions, as index of emotions, has to be completed by the investigation of other expressive systems that convey affective information (Weinberg and Tronick, 1994). 19 References Brazelton, T.B., Tronick, E., Adamson, L., Als, H. and Wise, S. (1975). Early motherinfant reciprocity. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 33 Amsterdam: Elsevier. Field, T. (1982). Individual differences in the expressivity of neonates and young infants. In S. R. Feldman (Ed.), Development of nonverbal behavior in children (pp. 279-298). New York: Springer-Verlag. Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: origins of communication, self and culture. Great Britain: BPCC Wheatons Ltd. Herrera, E., Reissland, N., & Shepherd, J. (2004). Maternal touch and maternal childdirected speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. Journal of Affective Disorders, 81(1), 29-39. Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99(3), 561-565. Kokkinaki, T. (1998). Emotion and imitation in early infant-parent interaction: A longitudinal and cross-cultural study. PhD thesis, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, UK. Kokkinaki, T. (2003). A longitudinal, naturalistic and cross-cultural study on emotions in early infant-parent imitative interactions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 243-258. Kugiumutzakis, G. (1993). Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction. In J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (Eds), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 22-47). London: Routledge. 20 McCartney, J. (1997). Four-month-olds do not prefer but can discriminate infant directed and adult directed pitch contours. Master of Science in Psychology, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA. Muir, D., Lee, K., Hains, C., & Hains, S. (2005). Infant perception and production of emotions during face-to-face interactions with live and “virtual” adults. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional development: recent research advances (pp. 207233). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Murray, L., Kempton, C., Woolgar, M. & Hooper, R. (1993). Depressed mothers' speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1083-1101. Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant's role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 15-29. Papousek, M., Papousek, H., & Bornstein, M. H. (1985). The naturalistic vocal environment of young infants: On the significance of homogeneity and variability in parental speech. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 269-297). N. J.: Ablex. Slaney, M., & McRoberts, G. (2003). BabyEars: A recognition system for affective vocalizations. Speech Communication, 39, 367-384. Stern, D., Hofer, L., Haft, W. and Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In T. M. Field and N.A. Fox (Eds), Social perception in infants (249-268). Norwood, N.J.:Ablex. Trad, P. V. (1990). Infant previewing: predicting and sharing interpersonal outcome. New York: Springer-Verlag. Trevarthen, C. (1985). Facial expressions of emotion in mother-infant interaction. Human Neurobiology, 4, 21-32. Trevarthen, C. (1993a). The self born in intersubjectivity: the psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: ecological and interpersonal sources of the self-knowledge (pp. 121-173). New York: Cambridge University Press. Trevarthen, C. (1993b). The function of emotions in early infant communication and development. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 48-81). London: Routledge. 21 Trevarthen, C. (1998a). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Braten (Ed), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 15-46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Trevarthen, C. (1998b). Intersubjectivity. Entry for the MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences. General Editors Wilson, R. and Keil, F. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Trevarthen, C. (2005). Action and emotion in development of cultural intelligence: why infants have feelings like ours. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional development: recent research advances (pp. 61-91). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Trevarthen, C., Kokkinaki, T. & Fiameghi, G. (1999). What infants’ imitations communicate: with mothers, with fathers and with peers. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy: Progress and prospects of current research (pp. 127-185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tronick, E. (2005). Why is connection with others so critical? The formation of dyadic states of consciousness and the expansion of individuals' states of consciousness: coherence governed selection and the co-creation of meaning out of messy meaning making. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional development: recent research advances (pp. 293-315). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1994). Beyond the face: an empirical study of infant affective configurations of facial, vocal, gestural, and regulatory behaviors. Child Development, 65, 1503-1515. Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., & Olson, K. L. (1999). Gender differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35, 175-188. Yogman, M. W. (1982). Development of the father-infant relationship. In H. Fitzgerald., B. Lester and M. W. Yogman (Eds), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 1) (pp. 221-279). New York: Plenum. 22 Diagram 1: Microanalysis of paternal and infant facial expressions of emotions during units and subunits of paternal infant - directed speech. p > 2sec Pat. speech Unit 1 Unit 2 Subunit 1 Subunit 2 Subunit 3 Subunit 1 "Shall we do apsiou, apsiou, apsiou, tsiou?" Non speech sounds "Why are you opening your mouth? Why are you opening it? Dad is not food." "Shall we go for a walk? Let' s go for a walk." Pat. emotions pleasure (+++) interest (+) neutral (0) negative (-) Inf. emotions pleasure (+++) interest (+) neutral (0) 23 pat. emot. intensity pleasure (+++) Inf. emot. intensity pleasure (+++) video duration elapsed time negative (-) 0 (sec) descending ascending stable interest (+) stable neutral (0) negative (-) interest (+) stable stable descending stable neutral (0) negative (-) 10:28 pause 1:56 3:16 pause 0:48 pause 5:04 8:36 9:76 0 00:05:26:08 00:05:29:16 00:05:16:01 (h:m:s:f) 00:05:27:22 00:05:30:03 00:05:38:12 00:05:43:13 00:05:53:07 Photograph 1a Matching of interest expression Baby boy two (2) months old: In the course of mutual eye contact, the father starts talking with an unsmiling face with raised eyebrows while the infant is motionless, expressing interest to him. Photograph 2a Matching of pleasure expression Baby boy six (6) months old: Infant and father seem to enjoy paternal singing with mutual expressions of pleasure. 24 Photograph 1b The father ends his question while mutual eye contact, with attentive and unsmiling faces, still feature infant father interaction Photograph 2b The father stops singing, and in the course of eye contact, both partners still express mutual pleasure. Table 1: Relationship between paternal and infant emotions during paternal infantdirected speech _____________________________________________________________________ Paternal facial expressions of emotions _____________________________________________________________________ Infant facial expressions of emotions Pleasure Interest Pleasure/Interest Pleasure 184 (22.5%) 9 (6.6%) 8 (4.5%) Interest 427 (52.1%) 122 (89.7%) 78 (44.3%) Pleasure/Interest 208 (25.4%) 5 (3.7%) 90 (51.1%) _____________________________________________________________________ Table 2: Relationship between paternal and infant emotional intensity during paternal infant-directed speech _____________________________________________________________________ Paternal emotional intensity _______________________________________ Infant emotional intensity Ascending Descending Stable Fluctuating _____________________________________________________________________ Ascending 171 (36%) 40 (17.6%) 603 (18%) 76 (22.6%) Descending 60 (12.6%) 63 (27.8%) 454 (13.6%) 68 (20.2%) Stable 127 (26.7%) 55 (24.2%) 1864 (55.7%) 56 (16.7%) Fluctuating 117 (24.6%) 69 (30.4%) 427 (12.8%) 136 (40.5%) 25 _____________________________________________________________________ Figure 1: Developmental curves of paternal pleasure and infant interest during paternal infant-directed speech across the age range from 2 to 6 months. 26 500 400 Frequency 300 Emotional expression Paternal pleasure Infant interest 200 2,00 3,00 2,50 4,00 3,50 5,00 4,50 Infants's age (in months) 27 6,00 5,50 28