Acta Physiol Scand 1990, 139, 371 385 Change in the pattern of behavioural specialization of neurons in the motor cortex of the rabbit following lesion of the visual cortex YU. I. ALEXANDROV, YU. V. GRI NCHENKO and T. JARVILEHTO* Institute of Psychology, Laboratory of Neurophysiological Basis of Mental Activity, USSR Academy of Sciences, Moscow, USSR, and *Department of Behavioural Sciences, University of Oulu, Finland ALEXANDROV, YU. I., GRINCHENKO, YU. V. & JARVILEHTO, T. 1990. Change in the pattern of behavioural specialization of neurons in the motor cortex of the rabbit following lesion of the visual cortex. Actµa Physiol Scand 138, 371-385. Received 4 January 1989, accepted 29 November 1989. ISSN 0001-6772. Institute of Psychology, Laboratory of Neurophysiological Basis of Mental Activity, USSR Academy of Sciences, Moscow, USSR, and Department of Behavioural Sciences, University of Oulu, Finland. In order to find out whether damage of the visual cortex (area 17) of the brain results in a functional reorganization of the motor cortex, experiments were carried out with freely moving rabbits performing a food acquisition task in an experimental cage. Two rabbits served as controls, while in three rabbits the visual cortex was bilaterally damaged. Analysis of the activity of 575 neurons in the control and operated rabbits after the recovery of the original instrumental food acquisition behaviour revealed a marked difference in the behavioural specialization of the neurons in the motor cortex of two operated rabbits compared with the control animals. Although the same types of units as in the control rabbits could be found in the operated rabbits (M neurons activated in relation to body and limb movements, S neurons activated in relation to food seizure and L neurons activated in relation to learned food acquisition task), the number of S units was about half of that in the controls and the number of L units about double. The relative number of activations of the neurons in the operated rabbits was significantly less frequent during the food seizure and more frequent during the learned behaviour. This difference indicates a change in the pattern of behavioural specialization of the neurons in the motor cortex due to the damage of the visual cortex. In this reorganization, the motor cortex became more like (but not identical to) visual and limbic cortices that normally contain noticeably more L neurons than the motor cortex. The number of neurons activated in relation to the behaviour in the operated rabbits, as compared with the control animals, was smaller in the upper and larger in the lower layers of the motor cortex. This may indicate recruitment of new neurons from the lower cortical layers. Key words: damage of the visual cortex, motor cortex neurons, single-unit activity in behaviour, rabbits, recovery from brain damage. Investigations of the effects of local lesions of various brain structures are widely used for increasing our understanding (1) of the role of a Correspondence: Yu. I. Alexandrov, Institute of Psychology, USSR Academy of Sciences, Yaroslavskaya 13, 129366 Moscow, USSR. given brain structure in the normal behaviour of the organism and (2) of recovery mechanisms after brain damage. These goals are interde pendent; in order to understand normal func tioning of the brain we must use knowledge about recovery processes in the experiments with brain damage and, on the other hand, our 371 372 Yu. I. Alexandrov et al. understanding of recovery mechanisms is itself determined by the prevailing views on the normal functioning of the brain (Laurence & Stein 1978). From the point of view of the functional systems theory (Anokhin 1974), recovery from brain damage means regaining the ability to achieve useful behavioural results in the organism—environment interaction which is realized by coordinated activity of neural elements belonging to different brain structures. This implies that local brain damage is followed by a general reorganization of neural activity that embraces not only the structures closely connected, both morphologically and functionally, with the lesioned structure, but also distant structures belonging to other 'morphofunctional' systems. This reorganization enables recovery of the ability to achieve the same adaptive effect as that before the lesion. In contrast to the above position, it is held that the brain (of an adult at least) is incapable of functional reorganization; recovery of the behaviour altered by brain damage is thought to be based on neuronal processes that were spared in the damage (Spear & Baumann 1979, Le Vere 1980). The general task of the present work was to determine whether or not functional reorganization follows brain damage. From our point of view, the role played by a certain structure in behavioural acts is determined by the behavioural specialization pattern of its neurons (Alexandrov 1989), i.e. by the particular set of the types of the neurons, and the relative number of the neurons belonging to these types, whose activity is related in a specific manner to certain behaviour. The activity of these neurons reflects realization of particular sets of functional systems necessary for given behaviour (Shvyrkov 1986). The aim of the present study was to find out what, if any, changes occur in the pattern of behavioural specialization of the neurons of such a ' distant' structure as motor cortex after damage of the visual cortex of the freely moving rabbit carrying out a food acquisition task. The present work is the first one in which changes in the functional characteristics of neurons were studied after brain damage in freely moving animals. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects. Five experimentally naive adult rabbits of both sexes (weight 23 kg) were used in the study and kept in separate cages in the vivarium with a 12-h light-dark cycle. Experimental procedure. Freely moving animals were taught to acquire food by pressing one of the two pedals in the experimental cage (60 x 60 cm, see Fig. 1). Pressing a pedal activated a feeder at the same wall of the cage. The training procedure (2 weeks) ended when a rabbit's behaviour at both the front and the rear walls of the cage had become repeatedly cyclic with the following behavioural phases: the rabbit pressed a pedal, turned to the corresponding feeder, lowered the head, seized the food, lifted the head, turned to the pedal, approached the pedal, pressed it, etc. Only one pedal was effective at any instant. After completion of the training the striate cortex was bilaterally lesioned (see later) in three rabbits (N 1, 2 and 3). Two intact rabbits (N 4 and 5) served as controls. After the operated rabbits had recovered (4-5 days after the operation), i.e. they regained the ability to obtain food, and the duration and variability of duration of successive phases of the behavioural cycle returned to the preoperative level, unit activity was recorded (see later) for 4 days in the motor cortex during the recovered food acquisition behaviour. The data yielded by the operated rabbits were compared with those obtained from the same region of the control animals during the same behaviour. The choice of the two brain areas was based on the following considerations: (1) These areas of the rabbit may be clearly distinguished and with a high degree of certainty correlated with Brodman's areas 4 and 17 (Kappers et al. 1936, Blinkov et al. 1973). (2) In our earlier studies we gathered many data on characteristics of the neuronal activity in these areas (especially in the motor cortex) in a similar ex perimental situation as in the present study (Alex androv et al. 1984, Shevshenko et al. 1986, Alexandrov 1989). (3) The motor cortex may be considered 'distant' with respect to the visual cortex not only in the spatial sense, but also in relation to the anatomical connections between these two cortical areas; ablations of the striate cortex do not result in any significant de generation in the motor cortex (Shkolnik-Yaros 1965, Montero & Murphy 1976, Towns et al. 1977). Cortical lesions. All surgery was performed under sterile conditions. The rabbits were anaesthetized with Nembutal (40 mg kg l). Cortical tissue was aspirated from the striate cortex region VI, determined according to Rose & Rose (1933) and Moore & Murphy (1976), with a surgical aspirator after retraction of the overlying meninges. Recording techniques. Activity of the neurons was recorded from the anterolateral part of the motor cortex (A 4.0; L 4.0) during a microelectrode penetration. Stimulation of this region resulted in marked lower jaw movements. Glass microelectrodes with 2.5м КС1, tips of 1-3 µm diameter and im- Neural reorganization and brain damage 373 Fig. i. Experimental cage and actographic marks of food acquisition behaviour. (A) Rabbit in the behavioural cycle at the rear wall; (B) scheme of the cage: i, 4, pedal and feeder of the front wall; 2, 3, pedal and feeder of the rear wall; 5, 8, light indicators of head lowering into a feeder and pedal pressing respectively; 6, 7, digital displays of the timer and neuronal discharge counter respectively. (C) Diagram of the relation of the segments of an actogram to phases of the behavioural cycle (for abbreviations, see text). pedance of 15 MQ. at 1.5 kHz were used and driven by a micromanipulator. Electrical activity of m. masseter pars profundus was recorded bipolarly by wire electrodes implanted in the muscle. Pedals and feeders were equipped with photoelectric devices. The changes of their potential served as actographic marks of the phases of the behaviour (see Fig. 1). Unit discharges, EMG and actographic marks of the behaviour at the front and the rear walls (two separate channels) were tape-recorded. Rabbits' behaviour was simultaneously video-recorded (the audio channel of the videotape recorder was used to record simultaneously neuronal spike activity). In the front wall of the cage (Fig. 1) were light indicators of the pedal pressing (8) and head lowering (5), and two counters, one (7) triggered by the spikes from the recorded unit and the other (6) by a 50-Hz pulse generator (timer). Timer impulses were also taperecorded, which enabled synchronization of the unit activity with the video recording. Behavioural and neural analysis. The following behavioural phases were defined in the food acquisition cycle (see Fig. 1): approach to the pedal (AP), pedal pressing (PP), approach to the feeder (AF), lowering the head into the feeder (LIF), food seizure (FS), food grinding (FG), lifting the head from the feeder (LFF) and regular chewing (RC). Behavioural deficits after the lesions were evaluated on the basis of the ability to obtain food from the feeder by pressing the pedal and on the basis of the changes (compared with the preoperative period) in the duration and variability of duration of the behavioural phases. Differences in the latter two indexes were estimated by t- and F-tests, respectively, and were considered significant if P < 0.05. The number of neurons whose activity could be detected was monitored in each penetration. Unit activity was analysed by plotting rasters and histograms with reference to different behavioural phases determined by the actographic marks and EMG. In addition, we calculated, by means of the readings of the timer and impulse counter in the stop-frame mode of the video recorder, the number of impulses during each of the successive 20-ms intervals with reference to the different phases of the behaviour of an animal. The data obtained from the successive realizations of the behavioural cycles were then summed, thus yielding histograms plotted in relation to the behavioural events which were not indicated by the actographic marks. The vertical localization of the recorded neurons was determined within one of the three parts (А, В, С) of a track that could be related to the higher, intermediate and lower layers of the cortex respectively. Because of the variability of the vertical structure in the cortex we treated only A and С parts of a track as definitely different in respect of their relation to the cortical layers: the superficial and deeper layers. For each unit the background frequency and the 374 Yu. I. Alexandrov et al. ratio of the background frequency to the frequency of the discharge during an activation was calculated. Differences in the distribution of these parameters were tested by the Kolmogorov Smirnov test. Activations were estimated during the different phases of the behaviour by relating increases of the firing frequency to the background discharge. Activation was considered to be present if a discharge appeared (in a unit without background activity) or there was a marked increase in the firing frequency (in the units with background activity) during a behavioural phase in all repeated realizations of the behaviour. A change in the firing frequency was considered to be an activation when it deviated by more than two standard deviations from the mean background frequency of a given unit. The first step in the classification of the units was based on the relation of their activation to the behavioural phases: units related and non-related to the different phases of the behavioural cycle. The first group was further divided into three subgroups which were activated in relation to the different phases of the behavioural cycle (see Results). This classification was possible on the basis of the two cycles of the food acquisition behaviour and by the use of some additional tests. The two cycles of the food acquisition behaviour enabled a comparison of the unit activity related to the achievement of a given goal (e.g. 'contact with the pedal', 'contact with the food') in the different environments (at the front vs rear wall) and by means of opponent movements (turns to the right vs to the left). On the other hand, behavioural acts in the different behavioural cycles that pursued different goals could be characterized by similar movements (e.g. a rabbit's movement to the left in the cycle at the front wall during its approach to the pedal). The following additional tests were used: a passive displacement of the rabbit's body, movements subsumed under the defence behaviour, orienting and searching, exploring the empty feeders and taking the food out of the experimenter's hand from above (to take the food the animal had to lift its head) at different positions in the cage. The significance of the differences in the number of units belonging to a classification group and the significance of the differences in the number of units belonging to different groups was estimated by the /\'2test. Differences were considered significant if P < 0.05. Morphological control. Upon the completion of the experimental series, the rabbits were killed with an overdose of Nembutal and perfused with 0.9% saline and 9% formalin. Forty-micrometre serial frozen sections were stained according to Nissl and sketched with the help of a microscope. The degree and borders of the lesions were determined according to the atlases of Rose & Rose (1933), Blinkov et al. (1973) and McBride & Klemm (1968). RESULTS Extent of lesions The morphological control demonstrated substantial lesions of the striate cortex in all three rabbits (see Fig. 2, right). The area peristriata and the occipital cortex (in rabbit N 3, right hemisphere) were also partly damaged. The depth and all the other characteristics of the lesions were similar in all operated rabbits. Some representative sections of the brain of the rabbit N 1 are presented in Fig. 2 (left column). All the layers of the cortex appeared to be lesioned, the damage affecting even the central white matter. The lesion produced a cavity which remained open in the lower cortical layers, compensatory gliosis scar tissue forming only on the surface. Recovery of the food acquisition behaviour There were no statistically significant differences in the preoperative behaviour (duration and variability of the duration of the different behavioural phases) between the operated and control rabbits. In spite of some variations in the amount of the damage, behavioural disorders and the dynamics of the disappearance of the behavioural deficits were similar in all the operated animals. Throughout the first post-operative day, no instrumental food acquisition behaviour was observed in the operated rabbits. On day 2 all the rabbits started to perform the food acquisition task, but the duration of the phases of the behavioural cycle and the variability of the duration of these phases significantly exceeded the preoperative level. Also, the mean duration and the variability of the duration of the approach to a pedal, pressing a pedal and the approach to the feeder significantly exceeded the preoperative level during day 3, but on day 4 both returned to the preoperative level (data not shown). After the recovery the sequence of movements was the same as before the operation. Neurophysiological data Activity of 575 neurons was analysed (274 in the control and 301 in the operated animals). The mean number of the neurons that could be detected in a microelectrode track was similar in both groups: 27.9 ± 7.1 and 24.1±8.0 respectively. Neural reorganization and brain damage Fig. 2. The extent of the cortical lesion in operated rabbits. Right: borders of the damaged zones in rabbits N i, N 2 and N 3, reconstructed on the basis of serial sections plotted on the schematic drawing of the rabbit cerebral cortex (dorsal view). Cytoarchitectonic fields are shown according to Rose & Rose (1933): Rsg, regio retrosplenialis granularis: Pstr, area peristriata; Str, area striata; 375 376 Yu. I. Alexandrov et al. The relation between the number of behaviourally related and non-related neurons was not different in the operated animals as compared with the control rabbits. The latter had 130 units (47%) ш tne behaviourally related group and the rest 144 (53%) in the behaviourally nonrelated group. In the operated rabbits these groups numbered 138 (46%) and 163 (54%) cells, respectively. Histograms of the distribution of the background frequency and of the ratios of the discharge frequency during activation to the background frequency in the operated rabbits did not differ significantly (χ²-test) from those obtained from the control animals. Types of behavioural specialization of neurons in the control and operated rabbits The first subgroup of the behaviourally related units consisted of neurons displaying activation in relation to a particular movement of the body, head or lower jaw, irrespective of the result sought or of the environment of the behavioural act involving these movements (M neurons). These neurons were found in both the control and operated animals (Fig. 3). Some units showed activations related to horizontal or vertical movements of the body and/or the head, others could fire bursts unselectively in relation to any movement. The M subgroup also included those neurons that were activated in relation to rhythmic lower jaw chewing movements. These neurons were observed only in the control rabbits. The second subgroup (S neurons) consisted of neurons which were activated in relation to seizing and/or grinding the food irrespective of the movements and the environment, but showed no activation related to chewing movements of the lower jaw or to head movements other than those involved in the seizure of the food. Also, these neurons were found in both the control and operated rabbits (Fig. 4). The unit presented above in Fig. 4 displayed activation during the food seizure in the behavioural cycle at the front wall (A), at the rear wall (B) and during the food seizure from the experimenter's hand (C). No other lower jaw movements were accompanied by an activation. The unit presented below was activated in relation to the food seizure and food grinding up to the regular chewing in the food acquisition cycle at the front wall (A), at the rear wall (B) and with the food seizure from above, from the experimenter's hand (C). The third subgroup (L) consisted of neurons which displayed activations only in the behavioural phases formed in the learning process in the experimental cage. These acts included the approach to and/or pressing of the pedal and the approach to a given feeder. The activations were independent of the means of the realization (e.g. the act could involve opposing movements; its environment could vary, etc.). Moreover, these units did not display any activation related to the same movements when these were observed during any other behaviour. Figure 5 presents examples of the subgroup L neurons of the control and operated rabbits, which were activated in relation to the approach towards and pressing of a pedal in the both behavioural cycles. Figure 6 shows a neuron which displayed activation during one behavioural cycle only. Differences in the pattern of behavioural specialization of neurons in the control and operated rabbits The most characteristic feature of behavioural specialization pattern observed in the motor cortex of both control rabbits was the great prevalence of S units. For the operated rabbits N 1 and 2, the distribution of the number of behaviourally related neurons was different from that in the control animals. The number of S units in the operated rabbits N 1 and 2 was about half of that seen in the control animals, while the number of L units was about double (Fig. 7). Number of activations related to the different behavioural phases in the control and operated rabbits Differences in the behavioural specialization pattern of the neurons of the control and operated rabbits were reflected also in the number of activations associated with the successive phases of the food acquisition behaviour (see Fig. 1). Figure 8 presents the 'overall picture' of the activations of the neurons during the behavioural cycle, which is based on the calculated percentage of the activations occurring during each of the successive behavioural phases in relation to the total number of activations observed in the control and operated rabbits. The relative number Fig. 3. Examples of the activity of M subgroup neurons, activated during leftward body movement. Neuronal activations related to the leftward movement in the front-wall (A) and rear-wall (C) cycles, during the transition from the front-wall to the rear-wall cycle (B) and from the rear-wall to the front-wall cycle (D-CONTR). D-OPER, activation accompanying the animal's shift to the left by experimenter (marked by oblique arrows). On the histograms, plotted by means of videotape analysis (see Methods): abscissa, time in seconds; ordinate, number of impulses; bin, 80 ms. Arrows mark the onset of the leftward body movement that served as a reference point for histogram plotting. Here and in other figures: CONTR, neuron of control rabbit; OPER, neuron of operated rabbit; EMG, electromyogram of m.masseter; ACTOGR. FRONT, actogram of behaviour in the front-wall cycle; ACTOGR. REAR, in the rear-wall cycle; IAN, impulse activity of neuron. Actogram symbols as in Fig. 1. Fig. 4. Examples of the activity of S subgroup neurons that were activated in relation to the act of food taking. Above: neuron of control rabbit that was activated only in relation to actual food seizure. (A) Activity in the front-wall cycle followed by probing an empty feeder. (B) Activity in the rearwall cycle. (C) Activity during the food seizure from experimenter's hand. Below: neuron of control rabbit that was activated in relation to food seizure and grinding. (A) Activity in the front-wall cycle. (B) Activity in the rear-wall cycle followed by probing an empty feeder. (C) Activity during the food seizure from experimenter's hand. All rasters below unit activity (each line of points shows one trial) are plotted with reference to the onset of the EMG burst in the m.masseter that corresponded to the first mouth closing during the food seizure. Fig. 5. Examples of the activity of L subgroup neurons in control (above) and operated rabbits (below). Units were activated during an approach to and pressing of the pedal in the front-wall (A) and rear-wall (B) cycles. Rasters are plotted with reference to the instant of releasing the pedal. Fig. 6. Examples of the activity of L subgroup neurons that were activated in only one behavioural cycle. Above: neuron of a control rabbit that was activated during an approach to and pressing of the pedal in the front wall cycle; below: neuron of an operated rabbit that was activated during an approach to and head lowering into the feeder in the front wall cycle. (A) Activity in the front wall cycle; activity in the rear-wall cycle. C-CONTR, orienting behaviour (both pedals are inoperative), crossing from the rear-wall cycle to the front wall of the cage and, after pedal pressing, food seizure from experimenter's hand. Neural reorganization and brain damage 381 DISCUSSION Fig. 7. Relative number of the M, S and L neurons in control and operated rabbits. Ordinate: percentage of the units belonging to the given specialization type, expressed as a percentage of all units analysed in control animals N 4 and 5 (n = 274), in operated rabbits N 1 and 2 (n = 183) and in operated rabbit N 3 (n = 118). Significance of the difference in relative number of units of a given type in operated rabbits compared to that in the control animals: * P < 0.05, **P< 0.001. of the activations during the food seizure and food-grinding phases was significantly smaller in the operated rabbits N 1 and 2 as compared with the control rabbits. Conversely, the number of activations at the learned phases of the behavioural cycle was significantly larger. Modifications of the specialization pattern in the operated rabbit N 3 were limited to a smaller number of units that were activated during the chewing. The activity observed in the rabbits N 1 and 2 at this phase is due to tonic activations of S neurons during the chewing. Distribution of behaviourally related and nonrelated neurons along the vertical axis of the cortex The control rabbits showed no significant difference between the number of behaviourally related and non-related units in the parts of the microelectrode track or in the lower and upper layers of the cortex (about 30% neurons in each part). The operated rabbits displayed a significant difference between the number of behaviourally related and non-related neurons in both the upper (A; P < 0.01) and lower (С; P < 0.05) cortical layers. This difference was mostly due to a smaller number of behaviourally related neurons in the A segment of a track (about 20%, on average) and to a larger number of behaviourally related neurons in the С segment (about 45 %, on average). The difference in the number of the behaviourally related units in these layers was statistically significant (P < 0.001). Significant differences in the pattern of behavioural specialization of neurons in the motor cortex were observed in the operated rabbits N i and 2 as compared with the control rabbits. These differences in the specialization pattern cannot be accounted for by changes in the motor structure of behaviour, because it was not changed after the operation (as compared to the controls). Neither can they be explained by changes of any parameters of movements or environmental information available. Our earlier results show that the behaviourally related neurons of the motor cortex are activated at a given phase of the behaviour in spite of the variability of its actual motor realization that is observed even in the simplest acts and in spite of the marked differences in the available environmental information (Alexandrov & Alexandrov 1982, Alexandrov et al. 1984, Alexandrov 1989). The operated rabbit N 3 showed no significant difference to the control rabbits in the number of units belonging to M, S and L subgroups. An analysis of the data obtained in the earlier studies on the neuronal activity of the central and peripheral structures correlated with the history of formation of the behaviour indicates that the neurons participate in the realization of functional systems that have been formed at successive stages of the phylo- and ontogeny: the realization of a set of systems of different ' ages' that subserve the achievement of the useful behavioural results (Alexandrov & Alexandrov 1982, Shvyrkov 1986, Alexandrov 1987, 1989). According to the criteria developed and described in the latter articles for the selection of units featuring different behavioural specialization, units can be classified as belonging to functional systems of different 'ages'. The neurons of the L subgroup belong to the new systems, those of the S subgroup to the older systems (formed at early stages of the individual development) and the neurons of the M subgroup to the oldest systems. In comparison with the control rabbits, in the operated rabbits a smaller number of S units and a larger number of L neurons was found. In spite of a certain variability of the functional morphology of the cerebral cortex, unit activity recordings from the anterolateral part of the motor cortex during various forms of food acquisition behaviour always yielded in our 382 Yu. I. Alexandrov et al. Front —w a l l c y c l e R e a r w a ll c y c l e Fig. 8. 'Overall picture' of activations of units in control and operated rabbits in the front- and rear-wall cycles. Ordinate: number of activations expressed as a percentage of the total number of activations observed in behaviourally related neurons in a given behavioural cycle in control rabbits N 4 and 5 (front-wall cycle n = 202, rear-wall cycle n = 203), in operated rabbits N 1 and 2 (front-wall cycle n = 101, rear-wall cycle n = 97), in operated rabbit N 3 (front-wall cycle n = 103, rear-wall cycle n = 104). Significance of the difference in the relative number of activations related to a given behavioural stage in operated rabbits as compared with control rabbits: *P< 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.005. earlier studies a similar pattern of behavioural specialization that was characterized by the same quantitative dominance of S neurons (Alexandrov 1989). The same number of neurons detected in the microelectrode track, the same frequency of the background activity and the constant frequency observed within activation bursts demonstrated in the operated rabbits, together with the literature showing insignificant morphological changes in the motor cortex after ablation of the visual cortex (see Methods), would suggest the following hypothesis: The differences found in the pattern of behavioural specialization of the neurons of the motor cortex between the control and operated rabbits are not due to a massive cell death or degenerative supersensitivity in the operated rabbits. Rather they are due to a functional reorganization of the pool of the neurons studied, Since the quantitative relation between the behaviourally related and non-related neurons was the same, while the number of behaviourally Neural reorganization and brain damage related neurons was smaller in the upper and larger in the lower layers of the cortex, we may assume that the differences observed in the specialization pattern were not due simply to selective exclusion of the behaviourally related neurons within this group. The difference was probably due to the fact that new cells obtained behavioural specialization and mostly in the lower cortical layers. The neurons of the motor and visual cortex belong to the different, but partly overlapping, sets of functional systems. Hierarchically coordinated action of these systems subserves the achievement of useful behavioural results (Alexandrov 1989). After the damage of the visual cortex the same result as before the damage may be achieved through a reorganization of the hierarchy of functional systems. This reorganization manifests itself as a change of the pattern of the behavioural specialization of the neurons in the motor cortex which lost a considerable portion of its specificity — the marked prevalence of S neurons in the normal rabbits above all other specialization types - and became more like (but not identical to) other cortical areas, such as the visual and limbic cortex, that normally contain significantly more neurons belonging to the new systems than the motor cortex (Alexandrov 1989, Shevskenko et al. 1986). Recovery processes following brain damage can be compared with those occurring during learning (Laurence & Stein 1978). The present results indicate certain neuronal aspects which are similar between learning (Gorkin 1987) and recovery: in both the behavioural recovery and learning, changes in the pattern of the initial specialization entail a relative decrease in the number of S subgroup neurons, as well as the recruitment of new nerve cells. Where recruited new neurons come from is not clear. Are they ' silent' neurons brought up from the reserve (as is the case with learning a new act), behaviourally non-related neurons or neurons having some other behavioural specialization but not activated during the food acquisition behaviour in the normal animals? All that can be stated now is that this reserve of new behaviourally related neurons may be situated in the lower layers of the motor cortex. The fact that the involvement of the motor cortex neurons in the subserving behaviour actually change is clearly demonstrated by the 383 differences in the 'overall picture' of the activations of the neurons, which shows an increase of the activity during the newly acquired behavioural acts and a decrease during the food seizure in the operated as compared with the control animals. The change in the role played by a distant structure - the motor cortex - in the subserving behaviour may be considered to represent at least one of the possible bases of the recovery, i.e. recovery of the initial behavioural pattern observed after the lesion of the striate cortex. This change is based on the reorganization of the unit activity, including a change in the behavioural specialization pattern of the neurons, as well as of the localization of the neurons involved in the behaviour. This conclusion would appear to contradict the results obtained by Spear & Baumann (1979), who demonstrated that the receptive fields of the neurons in the suprasylvian visual area were modified immediately after the ablation of fields 17-19 and did not exhibit any other change during the recovery of the visual discrimination behaviour. From these results Spear & Baumann (1979) concluded that functional reorganization plays at most a negligible role in the recovery after the lesion of the visual cortex of the cat. The authors noted, however, that they could not rule out entirely the possibility that changes might have occurred in some parameters of unit activity that were not investigated. We believe that, as far as the behavioural recovery is concerned, these as yet uninvestigated parameters must include the behavioural relation of the activity of the neurons examined. Our belief is based on the established fact that the specific features of the behavioural relation of this activity cannot be predicted on the basis of the properties of their receptive fields alone (Chapin & Woodward 1982, Alexandrov & Grinchenko 1984, Alexandrov 1989). Furthermore, even if we restrict ourselves to the criterion of the unchanged receptive fields, Spear & Baumann's (1979) conclusion is not true for all types of lesions, structures and behavioural patterns tested. Several studies have revealed gradual changes in the receptive fields of the neurons in the intact structures, changes that underlie the recovery of specific behavioural patterns after a brain damage varying in location and in the method of the lesion (Senatorov & Silakov 1985). Neuropsychologists and physiologists are well 384 Yu. I. Alexandrov el al. aware of the broad interindividual variability of the recovery mechanisms after one brain area has been damaged. The principal factor determining interindividual variability of the effects of brain damage is thought to be the specificity of the individual development (Luria 1973, Stein et al. 1983), which determines premorbid characteristics of the neuronal specialization. Individual specificity may account for the noticeably fewer changes in the pattern of behavioural specialization observed in the operated rabbit N 3, as compared to the operated rabbits N 1 and 2, though the dynamics of the behavioural recovery (according to the analysed indices) was similar in all three operated rabbits, and the extent of the lesion in the rabbit N 3 was at least not smaller than in the rabbits N 1 and 2. While the results of the present study clearly point to the unique reorganization processes in the rabbit N 3, there is not enough evidence to account for this anomaly. Three explanations, no one of which excludes the others, may be suggested: (1) More marked changes in other structures occurred in the operated rabbit N 3 than in the rabbits N 1 and 2. (2) Some functional differences existed in the preoperative state of the motor cortex studied. (3) The change in the behavioural role of the motor cortex units in the operated rabbit N 3 was different in some other parameters not investigated in our study. In any case the fact that changes in the behavioural specialization of the units in the motor cortex were found in two rabbits shows that functional reorganization of the brain is possible after cortical damage in freely moving rabbits. The authors are grateful to Dr V. N. Matz for her valuable assistance in morphological control. The help of Dr L. I. Alexandrov in the preparation of the English version of the manuscript is also gratefully acknowledged. REFERENCES ALEXANDROV, YU.I. 1987. Systemic specialization of the neurons of various cortical areas and its phylogenetic changes. In: V. J. A. Novak & J. Mlikovsky (eds.) Towards a New Synthesis in Evolutionary Biology, pp. 266—267. Czeckoslovak Academy of Science, Praha. ALEXANDROV, YU.I. 1989. Psychophysiologkal Significance of the Activity of Central and Peripheral Neurons in Behaviour. Science Press, Moscow (in Russian). ALEXANDROV, YU.I. & ALEXANDROV I.O. 1982. Specificity of visual and motor cortex neurons activity in behavior. Ada Neurobiol Exp 42, 457-468. ALEXANDROV, YU.I. & GRINCHENKO, YU.V. 1984. Activity of somatosensory and visual cortex neurons related to the testing of their receptive fields and to the realization of food acquisition behaviour. Neurophysiology 16, 254-268 (in Russian). ALEXANDROV, YU.I., GRINCHENKO, YU.V., SHVYRKOV, V.B., SAMS, M. & JARVILEHTO, T. 1984. Behavioral specificity of motor cortex units in freely moving rabbits. Ada Psychol Fenn X, 3-15. ANOKHIN, P.K. 1974. Biology and Neurophysiology of Conditioned Reflex and its Role in Adaptive Behaviour. Pergamon Press, Oxford. BLINKOV, S.M., BRAZOVSKAYA, F.A. & PUCILLO, M.V. 1973. Atlas of the Rabbit Brain. Medicine Press, Moscow (in Russian). CHAPIN, J.K. & WOODWARD, D.S. 1982. Somatosensory transmission to the cortex during movement : gating of single cell responses to touch. Exp Neurol 78, 654-669. GORKIN, A.G. 1987. Behavioral specialization of cortical neurons at different stages of learning. In: V. B. Shvyrkov, V. M. Rusalov & D. G. Shevshenko (eds.) EEG and Neuronal Activity in Psychophysiologkal Studies, pp. 78-81. Science Press, Moscow (in Russian). KAPPERS, C.U.R., HUBER, G.C. & CROSBY, E.C. 1936. The Comparative Anatomy of the Nervous System of Vertebrates Including Man. vol. 11. Macmillan, New York. LAURENCE, S. & STEIN, D.G. 1978. Recovery after brain damage and the concept of localization of function. In: S. Finger (ed.) Recovery from Brain Damage: Research and Theory, pp. 369-407. Plenum Press, New York. LE VERE, Т.Е. 1980. Recovery of function after brain damage: a theory of the behavioral deficit. Physiol Psychol 8, 297-308. LURIA, A.R. 1973. Basics of Neuropsychology. Moscow University Press, Moscow (in Russian). MCBRIDE, R.L. & KLEMM, W.R. 1968. Stereotaxic atlas of the rabbit brain based on the rapid method of photography of frozen, unstained sections. Commun in Behav Biol Part A 2, 179-215. MONTERO, V.M. & MURPHY, E.N. 1976. Corticocortical connections from the striate cortex in the rabbit. Anat Rec 184, 486-493. MOORE, D.T. & MURPHY, E.N. 1976. Differential effects of two visual cortical lesions in the rabbit. Exp Neurol 53, 21-30. ROSE, M. & ROSE, S. 1933. Die Topographie der architectonischen Felder der Grosshirnrinde am Kaninchenschaddel. J Psychol Neurol 45, 264-276. SENATOROV, V.V. & SILAKOV, V.L. 1985. Long-lasting Neural reorganization and brain damage reorganization of neuronal activity in neocortical field 7 after unilateral section of the visual radiation in the cat. Neurophysiology 17, 50—57 (in Russian). SHEVSHENKO, D.G., ALEXANDROV, YU.I., GAVRILOV, V.V., GORKIN, A.G. & GRINCHENKO, YU.V. 1986. Comparison of unit activity in different cortical areas in behavior. In: V. B. Shvyrkov (ed.) Neurons in Behavior: Systemic Aspects pp. 23-25. Science Press, Moscow (in Russian). SHKOLNIK-YAROS, E.G. 1965. Neurons and Interneuronal Connections. Visual Analyser. Medicine Press, Leningrad (in Russian). SHVYRKOV, V.B. 1986. Behavioral specialization of neurons and the system-selection hypothesis of learning. In: Human Memory and Cognitive Capa- 385 bilities, pp. 599-611. Elsevier Publishers, Amsterdam. SPEAR, P.D. & BAUMANN, T.P. 1979. Neurophysiological mechanisms of recovery from visual cortex damage in cats: properties of lateral suprasylvian visual area neurons following behavioral recovery. Exp Brain Res 35, 177-192. STEIN, D.G., FINGER, S. & HART, T. 1983. Brain damage and recovery: problems and perspectives. Behav Neural Biol 37, 185-222. TOWNS, L.C., GIOLLI, R.A. & HASTE, D.A. 1977. Cortico-cortical fiber connections in the rabbit visual cortex: a fiber degeneration study, J. Comp Neurol 173, 560-573.