Teacher-training students' conception of matter and substances, some results from 31 interviews. Bjørn Håland, University of Stavanger, Norway Background, aims and framework Introductory chemistry seems to be a worldwide problem that researchers have wrestled with for many years (e.g. Andersson, 1990; de Vos & Verdonk, 1985; Johnson, 2000b; Nakhleh, 1992). The particulate view of matter was early singled out as a key problem (e.g. Nakhleh, 1994; Nussbaum & Novick, 1981), in addition to chemical reactions (e.g. Andersson, 1990; de Vos & Verdonk, 1985; Johnson, 2000b), phase changes (e.g. Andersson, 1990; Stavy, 1990; Stavy & Stachel, 1985) and the gaseous state (e.g. Johnson, 1998a; Stavy, 1988). In the last years two, perhaps even more fundamental, concepts have added to the list: the concept of matter (Nakhleh et al., 2005; Stavy, 1991) and the concept of a substance (Johnson, 1996, 2005). An interesting aspect in this connection is Johnson's (1998a; 2002) findings that some understanding of the particulate view of matter seems necessary to understand all these key concepts and vice versa A key concept within human constructivism (Ausubel et al., 1978; Novak, 1993) is meaningful learning. New concepts always have to be tied to existing concepts in the learners' cognitive structure. As a teacher with this view of learning and teaching, one would claim that it is important to know something about the students' prior ideas. Only then one can pick out the "relevant anchoring concepts available in the learner's cognitive structure" (Ausubel et al., 1978, p. 358) and make the teaching meaningful. This investigation brings forward some knowledge about what Norwegian teacher-training students think about matter and substances and to what extent they use a particulate view of matter when they try to explain phenomena. The research question therefore was: To what extent do teacher training students' views of matter and substances differ from the scientific view, and is it possible to find any common relevant anchoring concepts for introductory chemistry education. The concepts of matter and substances are used from day one in chemistry teaching. If one, as a teacher, wrongly assumes that the students understand and use these key concepts the same way as oneself, there is most probably a crucial communication problem (Johnson & Gott, 1996). For students that use these concepts differently, major parts of the chemistry teaching must be a mystery, and they are left with only one option: rote learning. Methods and samples Science is not a compulsory subject in Norwegian teacher education. At University of Stavanger the teacher-training students can optionally choose to include 30 + 30 credits (ECTS) science as part of their education. Introductory chemistry (10 ECTS), is one module within this science education. It is reasonably to assume that it is the students who are more interested (and able) in science, that choose these courses. -1- 31 students were interviewed before they started the introductory chemistry course. The interviews were semi structured and linked to practical tasks. All interviews were tape recorded and some photos were taken. The students were initially introduced to a selection of (29) different objects, and asked to sort them as they wished. They were asked if they could see anything in common to all these objects and comment on that. Later 8 single-substance objects were picked out and they were asked if they could see anything in common to these and comment on that. The students were then asked to explain the results and what had happened to the substances in 5 practical demonstrations: evaporation of water, the missing stearin of half burnt candles, formation of dew (water), melting of stearin and dissolving of coarse salt in a glass of water. The interviewer consistently tried to avoid any expression that could hint what had happened to the material. The interview was finished with a discussion of the meaning of some (Norwegian) words: "stoff" (stuff / matter) and "materiale" (materials), the borderline between "stoff" (stuff / matter) and not "stoff" and the difference between "enkeltstoff (reint stoff)" (a substance) and "stoffblanding" (a mixture). The interesting parts of the interviews were partly transcribed. The results were coded, sorted and analyzed. Results, conclusions and implications As space is restricted only three main aspects will be discussed: the two concepts matter and substance and the particulate view of matter. In classical science, there is a strict borderline between what is material and what is not material (Chi et al., 1994). This investigation revealed several non scientific views of matter. A few examples: Only a few students could see anything in common to all the objects to be sorted. Statements like “different substances” or “built from atoms” were mentioned. Some claimed that the missing stearin had become energy, light or heat, and for some it had just disappeared. Others had no explanation for the dew outside the glass of water with ice cubes. About 1/3 of the students did not conserve mass (weight) during the dissolving process and / or melting process. When asked directly about the borderline between matter ("stoff") and nonmatter ("ikkestoff"), 6 students would (commonly) only think of solids as matter / stuff ("stoff"). Another 4 would include the liquids but not the gases. 3 would not include natural or living things among matter / stuff. Totally 21/31 students had one to several views on matter ("stoff") that did not correspond to the scientific view. It is interesting to notice that these findings correspond well with Stavy's (1991) results for children grade 1-7. In Norwegian the same word, stuff ("stoff"), is used both for the general term matter and for substances. The idea of single substances, with its own identity and its own properties, is, however, crucial for the understanding of chemistry (Johnson, 2000a). It is especially important in connection with reactions. -2- Although all students seem to recognize, use, buy and sell different substances, and are certain they will remain unchanged if treated properly, most students seem to have a rather vague notion of what a substance really is. When asked if they could see anything in common for the 8 single-substance objects, only one eventually recognized that they all were pure substances. When asked directly about the expressions: a (pure) substance (“enkeltstoff / reine stoff”) and mixtures (“stoffblandinger”), 13/31 expressed views that was scientifically (more or less) correct. But when it came to evaporation, burning of candles and formation of dew, only 4 of them gave explanations that show that they had reasonably good notions of substances. 16/31 thought that a single (pure) substance was one that contained only one element (“et grunnstoff”). Their different notions agree well with Johnson's (2000a) work that shows that "children do not 'naturally' have a concept of substance identity …". This investigation had no task or questions that directly probed the students' understanding and use of the particulate view of matter. Instead it was noticed how the students spontaneous used these ideas during the interview. None of the students gave explanations that revealed anything near a complete (basic) particulate view of matter. As many as 25/31 students, however, sporadically mentioned some sort of particle (molecules, atoms, small piece, particles), in a relevant way, one time or more in their explanations. It was most often mentioned in connection with the solution of salt (21/31) and evaporation of water (10/31). Only 4 students used some sort of particles relatively consistently across more tasks (4-8 times). This agrees well with Johnson's (1998b) and Nakhleh's et al. (2005) observations that students only seldom uses particles spontaneously when they talk about substances. Summing up over all the three main aspects discussed, 28/31 students either had some kind of view of matter and / or substances deviating from the scientific one or they used ideas about particles only sporadic. In other words, these basic and fundamental concepts in chemistry seem to be rather poorly understood among our students. As our students most probably are among those who are more interested and able in science, this probably also holds true for all Norwegian teacher-training students. Assuming that these concepts are well understood must generate many problems. For many, their only option must be rot learning (Ausubel et al., 1978) This investigation has uncovered a wide range of non scientific ideas about matter, substances and the use of the basic particulate view of matter. These key concepts therefore clearly need to be addressed as part of introductory courses in chemistry. Common and relevant anchoring concepts, however, seem to be rather limited. As all students recognized solid substances as matter, this could be used as one common starting point. By consolidating and expanding the concept of substances the concept of matter can also gradually be expanded. As some understanding of the particulate view of matter seem necessary to understand these key concepts (Johnson, 1998a, 2002), the particulate view of matter must be taught simultaneously. As many students spontaneously introduced some sort of particles in their explanation of the dissolving of solid matter (salt) in water, this might be used as a starting point. These deviating ideas about matter, substances and the particulate view of matter must also generate problems in part of the biology and ecology education (e.g. physiology, circulation of nutrients etc.). -3- Bibliography Andersson, B. (1990). Pupils' conceptions of matter and its transformations (age 12 - 16). Studies in science education, 18, 53-85. Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D., & Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational psychology. A cognitive view (second ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & Leeuw, N. d. (1994). From things to processes: a theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 27-43. de Vos, W., & Verdonk, A. H. (1985). A new road to reactions. Part 1. Journal of Chemical Education, 62(3), 238-240. Johnson, P. (1996). What is a substance? Education in chemistry, 33(2), 41-42+45. Johnson, P. (1998a). Children's understanding of changes of state involving the gas state, Part 2: Evaporation and condensation below boiling point. International Journal of Science Education, 20(6), 695-709. Johnson, P. (1998b). Progression in children's understanding of a 'basic' particle theory: a longitudinal study. International Journal of Science Education, 20(4), 393-412. Johnson, P. (2000a). Children's understanding of substances, part 1: recognizing chemical change. International Journal of Science Education, 22(7), 719-737. Johnson, P. (2000b). Developing students' understanding of chemical change: what should we be teaching? Chemical education: research and practice in Europe, 1(1), 77-90. Johnson, P. (2002). Children's understanding of substances, Part 2: explaining chemical change. International Journal of Science Education, 24(10), 1037-1054. Johnson, P. (2005). The development of children's concept of a substance: A longitudinal study of interaction between curriculum and learning. Research in Science Education, 35(1), 41-61. Johnson, P., & Gott, R. (1996). Constructivism and evidence from children's ideas. Science Education, 80(5), 561-577. Nakhleh, M. B. (1992). Why Some Students Don't Learn Chemistry - Chemical Misconceptions. Journal Of Chemical Education, 69(3), 191-196. Nakhleh, M. B. (1994). Student's Models of Matter in the Context of Acid-Base Chemistry. Journal of chemical education, 71(6), 495-499. Nakhleh, M. B., Samarapungavan, A., & Saglam, Y. (2005). Middle school students' beliefs about matter. Journal of research in science teaching, 42(5), 581. Novak, J. D. (1993). Human constructivism: A unification of psychological and epistemological phenomena in meaning making. International Journal of Personal Construct Psychology, 6, 167-193. Nussbaum, J., & Novick, S. (1981). Brainstorming in the classroom to invent a model: a case study. School Science Review, 62(221), 771-778. Stavy, R. (1988). Childrens Conception of Gas. International Journal of Science Education, 10(5), 553-560. Stavy, R. (1990). Childrens Conception of Changes in the State of Matter - from Liquid (or Solid) to Gas. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27(3), 247-266. Stavy, R. (1991). Children's Idea About Matter. School science and mathematics, 91(6), 240. Stavy, R., & Stachel, D. (1985). Children's conception of changes in the state of matter: from solid to liquid. Archives de Psychologie, 53, 331-344. -4-