Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 University of Plymouth Early Childhood Studies BA (Hons) Year 3 Is the Bookstart Programme an effective way to encourage shared reading experiences between parents and children? Catherine Hunn Email: ahunn@kankucho.freeserve.co.uk 1 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Acknowledgements Firstly I would like to thank my brother Christopher Whyley. He gave me the initial encouragement and helped me to establish self belief. My gratitude also goes to Claire Brown - student and friend - who gave me endless support and advice. Thank you also to the tutors and support staff at Plymouth University. Lastly, my biggest appreciation is for my husband Andrew and my three daughters Ellie, Abi and Chloe, this is a family achievement. 2 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Abstract: Bookstart, a joint initiative, is funded by multiple charities, and includes Government support through the Sure Start programme. Free children’s books are gifted to families with children less than 5 years of age. The Bookstart mission is to encourage shared reading experiences between parents and children with the vision of embedding a positive approach towards books and reading. This small scale research study focuses on the Bookstart Programme. My research shows that shared reading experiences with babies as young as 6 months old will help to expand early language, literacy, and emotional development and additionally offers parents opportunities to spend quality time with their children. Findings from my participant families indicate that all the parents appreciate how important books and shared reading experiences are for children’s development and I have confirmed that the Bookstart Programme is effective in offering such valuable opportunities for families. 3 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Is the Bookstart Programme an effective way to encourage shared reading experiences between parents and children? Introduction The Bookstart Programme is an initiative that began in the early 1990’s which offers free books to families with pre-school age children in order to encourage shared reading experiences at home. This dissertation outlines the origin and concept of the Bookstart Programme and examines how effective the Bookstart Programme is in encouraging shared reading experiences between parents and pre-school children. I compare opinions and data collected from Bookstart and non-Bookstart families and from one professional Bookstart co-ordinator. My literature review includes current findings and theories from key books and journal articles which outline the significance of the early introduction of books to pre-school children. My research in this area was driven by my desire to understand why some young children seemed to dislike reading. I have worked for several years as a teaching assistant and have supported many children with severe reading difficulties who were unmotivated or uninterested in books. I have often questioned why these difficulties and negative attitudes occur, especially when it is clear that the children have no underlying learning disability such as dyslexia. My own children are all avid readers, to the point where I have on 4 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 occasions had to take their books away from them so that they could get some sleep. I always read stories to my children from a very early age and encouraged them to share their stories and ideas. But why do some children enjoy reading, whilst others dislike it so much? Do early experiences in the home affect children’s attitudes toward books? Owing to the proposed research being short term, I am not aiming to establish whether the Bookstart Programme has long term beneficial results. However, I am hoping that the data collected will expand the debate on the value of shared book experiences. Is the Bookstart Programme effective in expanding children’s literacy, language and enhancing their social and emotional development? For the purpose of this dissertation, data has been collected from children, parents and one Bookstart Co-ordinator. The methodology used was a combined approach of semi-structured questionnaires and non-direct participant observations of children during shared reading sessions. Literature Review The Bookstart programme was initiated in Birmingham in 1992 by a national independent charity called Booktrust. Designed as an intervention programme, it offered free books for babies. Central to the aims of the Bookstart Programme was the desire to encourage shared reading experiences between parents and pre-school children. 5 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 The concept of Bookstart was to provide a universal gift to every child: a love of books, which would become a gift for life, and would benefit every child, culturally, educationally, socially and emotionally. (Collins et al 2005: 4) The Bookstart Programme, delivered by health visitors, librarians and project co-ordinators, sought to enable parents and children to share the pleasures and satisfaction that books offer, enabling parents to help build the foundations of their children’s literacy. Booktrust commissioned Moore and Wade (1993) to research the effects of early book sharing between children and parents and how this impacted upon child development. In Moore and Wade’s view, children’s interest in books and their literacy development is very much influenced by family members. They suggest that, in families where books and reading are promoted as an activity of fun and enjoyment, there is a higher chance that these children will adopt similar attachments and inquisitiveness about books. Moore and Wade emphasise in their Bookstart Programme evaluation that structured intervention and training in a pre-school home teaching programme - where parents felt supported and encouraged - would foster feelings of confidence in parental abilities to support children’s reading. …where access to books is difficult or attitudes to reading are not positive or where reading with very young children is not considered important, children may experience difficulties in beginning reading because they lack the ability to view a text as a focus of interest. (Moore & Wade 2003: 5) To overcome the problematic area of family access to books, the Bookstart Programme carefully selected and delivered books into communities via 6 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 libraries, health visitors and early childhood settings such as Sure Start - a Government aided programme which aims to achieve better outcomes for children, parents and communities. One of the ways to help achieve these aims was to introduce Bookstart In order to assess the potential effectiveness of the proposed Bookstart Programme, Moore and Wade undertook an empirical study of 300 children aged 2-3 years and observed family reading practices and its impact upon childhood development. The cohort families were represented by a culturally ethnic mix and included a cross section of professional and manual workers and unemployed groups. The families were given free children’s books. After an initial six month period, results from questionnaires indicated that ‘71% of parents bought more books for their children, 28% spent more time sharing books with their children, 57% of parents had book club membership and 29% had enrolled as library members for themselves and for their babies’ (Moore and Wade 1993 cited in Moore and Wade 2003: 5). Furthermore, in a followup study, which matched a select group from the original 300 families against a non-Bookstart cohort, analysis showed that the Bookstart children were more likely to look at books compared to the non-Bookstart children; parents gave a higher priority to purchasing books as presents; visited the library more frequently; more parents shared books with their children and there was increased frequency of parents sharing the whole text, talking about the story and encouraging language development by linking reading with other experiences. Also, the children were observed focusing on books, turning the pages, making comments about the text, pointing to text and illustrations, 7 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 making predictions and joining in with the reading, asking and answering questions. All results from the study showed that Bookstart families were advantaged over non-Bookstart families by a statistically significant level of between 5% and 0.1% levels (Moore and Wade 1993 cited in Moore and Wade 2003: 6). Moore and Wade summarized their findings by observing that Bookstart children were ‘consistently superior in all observed elements at pre-school’ (Moore and Wade 1993 cited in Moore and Wade 2003: 8). Roehampton University was commissioned to evaluate the Bookstart Programme between 1993 and 1994. Researchers, Collins, Svenson and Mahoney interviewed families, health visitors, librarians and Bookstart Coordinators. Their findings showed conclusively that all professionals were supportive of the programme. However, organisation of the Bookstart scheme and partnerships between professionals varied across the three regions. Recommendations include further training of some professionals was necessary for the effective delivery of Bookstart. Findings indicated that parents showed positive responses to the Bookstart scheme and agreed that the books had had a positive effect on their young children’s literacy development. Furthermore, the evaluation indicated that where children had accelerated reading skills, they ‘engaged in a diverse range of reading related activities’ (Collins et al 2005: 2). A further positive outcome of the study also concluded that, where parents highly valued books, this had a ‘positive impact on children’s reading’ (Collins et al 2005: 2). The 8 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 evaluation showed however that in some cases, for reading to be embedded within the family, parents would need more than the gift of free books before appreciating the significance of shared book experiences. Additionally, the Roehampton evaluation observed some negative feedback from families who did not have English as their first language. Despite the translation of the Bookstart literature into some community languages, the supporting leaflets from the packs were not always readily available to these parents in their relevant languages. Consequently some families did not understand the importance of the Bookstart packs and the Bookstart philosophy (Collins et al 2005). The Bookstart evaluation by Roehampton University (Collins et al 2005) concluded that shared reading experiences did have a multiple purpose. Language acquisition and early literacy skills increased when shared reading took place regularly. Parents too, benefited from having such close and often treasured moments with their children. Moore and Wades’ (1993) research on early literacy confirms that there are considerable documented links between language development and book sharing. For example, the frequency of reading aloud in the pre-school period is positively correlated with concurrent language skill and reading readiness and with later language and reading abilities in the infant school. (Wells 1985 cited in Moore and Wade 2003: 4) 9 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Wells (1985) observes that parents modify their speech when they are talking to young children; their pitch, exaggeration of tone and rising intonation all succeed in attracting the attention of the child listener. This can be seen to an even greater degree when parents read books to or with their children. Wells emphasises the close connection between talking and learning. And stresses ‘the most important feature of a child’s language experience is that it is conversational in nature’ (Wells 1985: 1) Moreover, Wells believes that when children listen to stories, before they are able to read, they gain experience and organisational structures of language -‘…the child is beginning to come to grips with the symbolic potential of language’ (Wells 1985: 134). Wells suggests that stories have a fundamental role in helping children to attach meaning to other events and experiences and that when children want to make sense of an experience, they need to construct a story out of it. The adult helps to make links and bring shape to the child’s experiences through narratives and ordering of events - ‘storying is one of the most fundamental means whereby human beings gain control over the world around them’ (Wells 1987: 197). Stories provide a real purpose for extending control over language, all the more effective because they also tap one of the child’s most powerful ways of understanding, enlarging, and working on experience (Wells 1987: 203) Evans (1997) reinforces the suggestion that reading to children assists in the development and refinement of language and communication skills. When adults model the correct use of language and surround children with 10 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 opportunities to speak and listen, such as story time and nursery rhymes, they should also respond readily to children’s attempts at words. In this way their emerging communication skills will be consolidated. Create an environment which promotes active listening and productive talking (Evans, 1997: 29). As children experiment with sounds and rhythms, the words and structures of their native language will develop into recognisable communicative sentences. Children will begin to recognise that language can be used as a useful tool for social interaction and become increasingly more confident in their use of words. A common view is that, in the first instance, it is the attitude of parents or significant others in encouraging inquisitiveness, enjoyment and sharing books that may inevitably lead the child to becoming an able reader. Using Bruner’s Scaffolding Theory (cited in Smith et al 2003: 503l) as a framework to develop reading through informal experiences, we can observe how Bruner’s ideas are consistent with the development of reading skills. …scaffolding does not imply a rigid structure or didactic teaching method but rather a flexible and child-centred strategy, which supports the child in learning new things and which enables the child to have a sounding board for action. (Smith et al, 2003: 503) 11 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 The Bookstart Programme can be said to support a scaffolding approach to learning. Through the resultant social interaction - the pleasure and intimacy the child is motivated by the relationship and thereby encouraged to develop an interest in books. As the parent acts as an enabler - pointing out initial sounds, letting the child fill in the missing words, encouraging discussion such as “what do you think will happen next?” - and reinforcing the sequential patterns of the story, the child begins to recognise that stories have a beginning, middle and an ending. As the adult gradually withdraws, the child becomes increasingly more independent and capable, will have a positive disposition and will be at a point of reading readiness. In this way considerable meaningful ‘tuition’ takes place, but without formal teaching (Miller 1996: 34). Children’s interest in their environment is motivated by the engaging experiences and enjoyment that they have at home with family members. There appears to be little doubt that babies and young children can be introduced to books and their interest further developed by encouragement from parents or caregivers. This can take many forms: contributing to shopping lists, looking through television guides, pointing at road signs on car journeys and looking at recipes in cookery books. Young children ‘play’ literacy games such as putting magnetic letters on fridge doors and activities like these sow the seeds of communication through the written word. 12 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Williams and Lewis (1999:1) point out that ‘Young children may not have made the connection that the little squiggles on the page actually mean something, but they watch us engaging in this mysterious activity called reading on a daily basis’. They also note that ‘You will be able to tell when a child is interested’ (Williams & Lewis 1999: 5) by the fact that children will point at books, turn the pages over and make noises about the pictures, listening intently to the words being read to them even if they do not have the cognitive ability to understand the meanings of the words. The baby will love hearing your voice… They will probably be curious about the activity and notice your interest. You will be sharing your confidence in, and your enjoyment of, written materials with them. This is an important experience for any young child. (Williams and Lewis 1999: 4) Miller (1996), in a study of children’s reading development describes the home environment as being the place where literacy development in young children is nurtured. She emphasises that the key to children acquiring successful literacy development is primarily down to: Parents who enjoy interacting with their children in ways which foster literacy development Children who either initiate or are responsive to parental interactions An environment in which such experiences are part of every day life (Miller 1996: 39). 13 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Miller (1992) conducted a study of 31 children between 3 and 4 years of age who attended a playgroup. She aimed to encourage parents and children to regularly share books together. What surprised Miller from the outset however was the knowledge of books that children had already gained before they started their pre-school settings. She observed that many of the children could orientate a book correctly. Of the studied children, 28 could correctly identify pictures in the books and 22 of the children were able to identify writing. Whilst Miller was encouraged by her findings, she did acknowledge that all the children in her study were from families where most parents had professional jobs and none of the parents were unemployed. Miller added that in her study group the parents involved had already established regular reading patterns with their children and were members of libraries. Miller pointed out, that due to the keen interest that the parents displayed about reading themselves, some excellent attitudes towards books were already present. What these children knew about written language and books had been learned from their experiences at home with their parents and other significant adults. (Miller 1996: 11) This highlights the unsurprising results from her study. The children were from predominantly “middle class” families where parents are perhaps more likely to have higher expectations and aspirations for their children and model good attitudes towards books. However, this is not to say that “working class” parents do not establish successful reading patterns with their children. 14 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Furthermore, children with parents that have unreasonably high expectations of their children and promote premature reading development may encounter some reluctance from their children. Whitehead (1999) issues a warning when she discusses the potential dangers of inappropriate pressure put upon children to become literate, suggesting that unreasonable amounts of coercion may ‘make reading and writing narrow and pointless. This will undermine our children’s true literacy interests and strategies’ (Whitehead 1999: 49). Whitehead emphasises that early book reading should be a shared experience where both participants are active, the child pointing at pictures, choosing books, joining in the patterns and rhythms of the stories. Early reading is most successful when children and adults share the pleasures of looking at books, reading them together and talking about them on many occasions. (Whitehead 1999: 60) Whitehead (1999), cautions against parents using books solely for the purpose of making their children more able readers. Whitehead suggests that a parent who exerts excessive pressure on a young child to learn to read may create negative feelings in the child which in time may interfere with establishing successful reading skills. Opinion is divided about the right time to start formally teaching children. ‘Parents [in Scandinavian countries] are actively discouraged from engaging their pre-school children in early reading acquisition’ (McGuiness1998: 335). In a study concerning the appropriate time to start formal education with regard to the relevance of compulsory school starting age (NFER, 2002), over 15 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 half of 33 European countries have starting ages of 6 years. In 8 of the countries school starting age is 7 years. England is only 1 of 5 countries where obligatory schooling starts at 5 years. The study focused primarily on the advantages and disadvantages of school starting ages and how this impacted upon academic development suggesting that, whilst the teaching of more formal skills at an earlier age had initial academic advantages, such advantages were not sustained. The report concluded that children exposed to books at home, amongst adults who enjoy reading, tend to read earlier, but formal teaching of reading skills at an early age does not appear to give children a lasting advantage (Sharp, NFER, 2002: 20). This view echoes the Rudolph Steiner (1861-1925) approach to child development, the philosophy of which taught that formal learning is inadvisable if undertaken prematurely, before the age of 6 or 7 years. Before this age a child is still at a formative stage of physical growth and development, with structures in the brain still being refined and elaborated. A young child's primary mode of learning should be through physical experiences and focussed on play and language maturity. A Word-rich Experience as a Foundation for Literacy Every person or group of persons who move into literacy build a foundation for reading and writing in the world of orality. Orality supports literacy, provides the impetus for shaping it. The skills one learns in orality are crucial because literacy is more than a series of words on paper. (Oldfield 2001: 114) 16 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Steiner emphasises the whole development of the child, which should concentrate on the spiritual, moral and physical growth of the young child. It views education as a journey not a race. Whilst the Steiner approach endorses a ‘free to learn’ attitude, (Oldfield 2001), it does not disparage the use of books for children under the age of 7 years but suggests careful judgement when selecting appropriate reading material for this age group. Illustrations should be emphasised rather than the text and books aimed at ‘educating’ children should be avoided - books that ask questions such as “What shape is it?” have no reality for the child (Salter 1987: 101). They neither fire his imagination nor engage his fantasy. For the child they present what is only deadly dull meaningless information (Salter 1987: 101). Steiner philosophy also suggests that quality books should be beautifully illustrated and hold true representations of the characters in the stories animals should look realistic, Kings should look regal and ‘a man be a true representation of humanity’ (Salter 1987: 100). Furthermore, it is the telling of a story by the parent to the child rather than ‘reading the story’ that will herald a meaningful experience. Traditional tales, nursery rhymes and songs will appeal to a child’s sense of imagery and rhythm and are the exact opposite of educational books, they ‘speak not to the head but to the knowing heart, and as such they are the birthright of every child’ (Salter 1987: 105). To conclude this literature review, it is quite clear from the work of Salter (1987), Wells (1987) Miller (1996), Whitehead (1999) Oldfield (2001) and 17 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Moore and Wade (2003) that shared reading experiences are multi purpose. Children benefit not only from extended reading and social skills but, most significantly, their language development improves. These milestones do not stand alone as one being more important than the other. Story-telling to children helps children to make sense of their world. It brings sequence and order to their lives and gives context to their experiences. Parents and children can enjoy treasured moments from these shared experiences and will develop a lifelong relationship with books. Methods of Enquiry I collected qualitative data from 3 sources (triangulation). This qualitative method of data collection was a combined approach of (1) observed shared reading activities between parents and children. (2) semi-structured questionnaires with parents and (3) a semi-structured interview with the County Bookstart Co-ordinator. I chose to use unstructured interviews and questionnaires since my research is in people’s opinions not in statistic collection. Bell (2005), points out that the advantages of this type of data collection are its adaptability. Ideas can be expanded and followed up and participant’s feelings, motivations and ideas can be developed. ‘Questionnaire responses have to be taken at face value, but a response in an interview can be developed and clarified (Bell 2005: 157). However, Bell points out that interviews can be time consuming and their analysis problematic as they can be liable to misinterpretation. For this 18 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 reason the number of participants in this type of data collection is usually significantly fewer than those involved in quantitative questionnaires. I used the Collins, Svenson, and Mahony (2005) ‘Individual Parent Questionnaire’ and the ‘Librarian/Health Visitor conversation/Questionnaire’ as a framework for my own data collection, adapting its format as appropriate for my specific investigation. In my non-participant observations I examined parent and child shared reading experience. Roberts-Holmes (2005) suggests that researchers use such methods frequently: ‘Researchers are always looking and listening for information and behaviour which will help them to answer their research questions’ (2005: 92). However Roberts-Holmes does caution that this can be widely interpreted or misinterpreted. It is for that reason that I tried to maintain a non-judgemental attitude and cautioned myself against any preconceived ideas that I previously held about families and their attitudes to children’s reading. This was particularly pertinent to me when I met with a family who have adopted a Steiner approach to child development. Before my research I possibly had some judgemental attitudes towards child development practices that challenge normal convention. It is about being open as to why children, practitioners and parents are doing things in a different way from that which you might expect (Roberts-Holmes 2005: 95). 19 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 I used the Moore and Wade ‘Home Activities Survey and Observational Study’ (1996) as a framework for my observations and all my observations were conducted in the child’s home so that the child and parent could be as natural as possible, hopefully adding validation to the data. I made some preobservation visits to the families discussing the purpose of my research with them and making myself familiar to the children. Ecological validity can be enhanced by doing more naturalistic research, in natural settings such as homes, schools, playgrounds… …and using familiar people such as parents, peers and professional in the research design (Greig & Taylor 1999: 74). There have been weaknesses to my methodology. My pilot study (family 1) showed my observation chart and questionnaires to be lacking. This became apparent when I noticed discrepancies in the data collected. One of my questions (appendix I, 4.f) asked parents whether their child points to words and letters during shared reading, to which the parents answered “no”. However, when I subsequently observed the shared reading, the child regularly pointed to the pictures - a significant contribution, particularly in the light of the Steiner approach to books - acknowledging this as an oversight on my part, I subsequently changed the wording on both observation charts and questionnaires to ’points to words, letters and pictures’. A pilot study is crucial at the beginning of your study because it will alert you as to whether or not your research questions, approaches and proposed research methods are specific, ethical and actually doable. (Roberts-Holmes 2005: 35). 20 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 When I reflect upon my methodology it is possible that quantitative data collection may have produced more conclusive results. Methods of research can be divided into two main categories: primary research and secondary research. (Green 2000: 2) Primary research is the process of carrying out one’s own research project and producing your own findings. Secondary research is when the researcher is using material gathered by somebody else or has been written by another person. My small scale project whilst not original by nature is nevertheless called primary research as data gathered and analysed was undertaken by me. Research data is usually collected by using either qualitative or quantitative methods. Quantitative research produced results which can be expressed using numbers or statistics. (Green, 2000: 3). Qualitative data is not so concerned with statistics. Qualitative data can also be described as an interpretive approach. ‘Qualitative research obtains viewpoints and personal feelings from its participants…’(Green 2000: 3). It is important therefore to take into account the opinions of all persons involved in the topic area. In this instance this included parents and the Bookstart 21 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Co-ordinator. This method of combined data collection is referred to as triangulation. Triangulation is a way of comparing and validating evidence. The authenticity of the research participants’ responses can be demonstrated by cross-checking or triangulating their responses with other people. Through this process of data triangulation it is possible to see if the participants’ responses are consistent. (Roberts-Holmes 2005: 40) …validity can be enhanced by the use of triangulation. Broadly speaking this can be defined as supporting your findings in more than one way. (Green 2000: 83) By using the triangulation method of enquiry I believe that my findings have shown a range of opinions and ideas, but are not conclusive. Findings and Discussion My findings on the value of the Bookstart Programme are based on four observations of parent-and-child shared reading experiences, six semistructured questionnaires from parents and one semi-structured interview with the County Bookstart Co-ordinator. I have presented my findings of parent questionnaires and observations of shared reading experiences first. Secondly, I have presented a discussion outlining key aspects of my interview with the Bookstart Co-ordinator including several transcriptions from the taped conversation. I have also demonstrated how the Bookstart Programme has expanded into the wider community – linking with family learning. Lastly I have summarised my key findings. 22 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Family 1: Family 1 have a boy who is just over 3 ½ years old. Both parents are British and professionally employed. They received the baby Bookstart Pack but were disappointed not to have received the Bookstart + set, although their son does attend a pre-school setting. Due to this distribution difficulty the chosen book was not from the Bookstart range. Shared reading happens every night as part of the child’s bed time routine and both mother and father read a book to their child. The marks on the chart indicate how often the child performed the task. Non participant observations of family 1 (recording 1) Child Parent Location Boy Age: 3:8 Mother, story 1 Child’s bedroom Length of activity Date 18:33 27.3.07 Father, story 2 Story1: ‘Dig, dig Digging’ by Margaret Mayo Asks parent questions about the story Joins in with telling the story Makes conversations about the pictures Turns the pages Makes predictions about the story Points to the pictures, words or letters Story 2: ‘Emergency’ by Margaret Mayo / / / / / / / / (8) / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / (17) (0) (0) / / / / (4) / / / / / / / / / (9) / / (2) (0) (0) (0) / / / / / / (6) / / / / / / / / / / / / / / (14) 23 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Discussion: Consistent with the parent questionnaire (appendix i), child 1 often asked questions about the story, often made conversations about the story and on two occasions turned the pages. There was a discrepancy however between the parent answers (4.f) and my observations during the shared reading session. This issue I addressed for subsequent observations (see methodology (pg: 18). I did not observe the child joining in with the story telling, whereas the parent questionnaire suggested that occasionally this happened. There may be a variety of reasons for this, perhaps due to the book choice or because I was in the room making the situation unusual, perhaps inhibiting his normal responses. I found it interesting that the child participated and contributed more to the shared reading experience with his father than with his mother. This could be a gender issue i.e. the books were ‘male orientated’ and perhaps brought about a father and son union. This could make an alternative interesting research study – looking at the difference between mother and fathers reading to their children, their relationships and male/female stereotypical books. Answers 1-5 of the parent questionnaire indicated that this family had already established good reading practices prior to Bookstart and, whilst they felt the programme did not encourage them to do anything new or different, they still very much valued its worth. 24 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 They are a vital part of his language development. By listening to stories he is gaining an understanding of story/non-fiction features and this will prepare him to become a writer (Mother of 3:8 year old). This response to the parent questionnaire (answer 6) echoes the recognised link between story telling and language development, Evans (1997), Wells (1985). An additional comment of ‘very useful bag’ was made and, perhaps whilst not meant to be taken too seriously, I think it is worth commenting on. The Bookstart baby pack, Bookstart + and the treasure chest box are all made to a very high standard, with good quality materials. I think that this suggests to parents that Bookstart, the supporting charitable agencies and the government are giving a very clear message to parents, that is to say, parents and children are highly valued and reading is regarded as an essential life acquisition. The books are of excellent of quality, carefully chosen by a select committee of professionals and the books include established children’s authors including; Jill Murphy, Helen Oxenbury, Quentin Blake and Allan Ahlberg (appendix ii). …We valued the opportunity to receive books and know that they are valued enough to send out to all parents. (Mother of 3:8 year old). 25 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Family 2: Family 2 have a daughter who is 2 years and 3 months old. Her mother is English and her father is Danish. Their daughter speaks both English and Danish. Her father is a business developer and her mother, an ex primary school teacher, is now a full time parent. The family shared books prior to being gifted books from Bookstart and say that Bookstart did not initiate anything new. The child has only received the Baby Bookstart pack. The book chosen from the observation was not from the Bookstart range. Non participant observations of family 2 (recording 2) Child Parent Girl Age: 2:3 Mother Location Length of activity Family sitting 6:09 room Book: Each Peach Pear Plum By Allan & Janet Ahlberg Asks parent questions about the story Joins in with telling the story Makes conversations about the pictures Turns the pages Makes predictions about the story Points to the pictures, words or letters (0) / (1) / / / / / / / / / / / / (12) (0) (0) / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / / (17) 26 Date 4.4.07 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Discussion: My observations of this book sharing experience are that the child did not respond consistently with the answers given in her mother’s questionnaire (appendix iii). In the parental questionnaire ‘often’ was ticked as a response to ‘Asks questions about the story’ and ‘joins in with telling the story’. There was only one occasion when the child joined in with the story telling. The child never attempted to turn the page in spite of an ‘always’ answer on the questionnaire. This again could be to do with book choice or my presence. It is not possible to be conclusive about these discrepancies and my results suggest that using such data collection methods is too variable and exact measurements are, in these circumstances not possible. Consistent with parental answers the child made 12 conversational contributions about the story and pointed to pictures and words 17 times. Child 2 is does not attend any day care setting. This has highlighted the problems of Bookstart distribution and whilst this family have good reading patterns established, it perhaps suggests a gap in the system where the ‘hard to reach’ families are potentially missing out on the Bookstart opportunities. ‘More regular contact, more events…’ (Mother of 2 year old). 27 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Family 3: Family 3 have a daughter who is 3 years old. Both parents are English. Her father is an engineer and her mother is a full time parent. The child has received both the Baby Bookstart pack and Bookstart + for toddlers. However, the child did not choose one of the Bookstart books to share with her mother during my observation. Wishing to adhere to the rights of the child, I felt it inappropriate to attempt to dissuade her from her book choice for the purpose of my research. Non participant observations of family 3 (recording 3) Child Parent Location Girl Age: 3 Mother Family Kitchen Story book: The Other Ark By Lynley Dodd Asks parent questions about the story / / / / / / / / / / (10) Joins in with telling the story / / / / / (5) Makes conversations about the pictures / / / / / / / (7) Turns the pages (0) Makes predictions about the story Points to the words letters & pictures (0) / / / / / / / / / / / / / / (14) 28 Length of activity 6:13 Date 14.04.07 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Discussion: The child contributed well to the book sharing experience, echoing her mother’s questionnaire responses (appendix iv). She asked many questions, joined in with telling the story, and pointed to the pictures on 14 occasions. She made no attempt to turn the pages and made no predictions about the story; her mother indicated on her questionnaire that she occasionally did this. The child’s mother felt that the Bookstart Programme ‘hasn’t changed our behaviour but it’s reinforced our feeling that reading is a valuable activity’. Furthermore it has been recognised by the family (answer 6) that shared reading activities are a valuable and special part of the parent child family relationship. Very good idea, part of our bedtime routine – the only time we sit down alone. A valuable activity that is not just about reading, but talking about ideas and pictures (Mother of 3 year old). Family 4: Family 4 follow the Waldorf Steiner approach to childcare. Their twin five-year olds do not go to school and have never experienced a state run pre-school day care setting. In the past year they have been attending a Steiner playgroup, started by their mother. The twin’s mother did believe that in some circumstances Bookstart may have a purpose: …can be a useful tool for encouraging children’s language skills for parents who may not be aware of, or confident employing other techniques’ (Mother of 5 year old twins). 29 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 As the family have chosen not to pursue the Bookstart Programme the book chosen was not from the Bookstart range. Non participant observations of family 4 Child Parent Location Boy Age: 5 Mother Family back garden Length of activity 10:27 Date 10.4.07 Book: Kate, the Cat and the Moon By David Almond & Stephen Lambert Asks parent questions about the story Joins in with telling the story Makes conversations about the pictures Turns the pages Makes predictions about the story Points to the pictures, words or letters / / / / / / / (7) (0) / / / / / / / / / / / / / (13) / (1) (0) / / / / / / / / / (9) Discussion: My observations of this shared reading experience echoes answers from the parent questionnaire (appendix v). My impression was that they regularly shared books. The child was very much a participant in the activity and not a passive observer. He initiated mature conversations about the story and there was an abundance of discussion between the two of them. His conversation explored and examined the characters. His use of language was well established. Caution should be exercised, however, about 30 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 interpreting and comparing this maturity of conversation against that of other children as the study child was over 5 years old. The second oldest child in my observation group was 4 and the youngest 1:5 so there is an extensive range on the language development spectrum. Their Mother (recording 4 & 5), a former primary school teacher, has chosen to opt out of the English state education system as she feels that the National Curriculum and its ensuing restrictions emphasises academic achievements and is not in the best interests of her children. There’s a lot of children who just don’t have a sound basis for language development and in the first seven years that’s really what should be going on (Mother of 5 year old twins). The twins’ mother held strong views about children’s books and felt that British attitudes towards shared reading emphasise an educational/academic approach rather than the perspective of language development (recording 4&5). I think it’s more about the language experience rather than specifically sharing books. I think children who have had an opportunity to talk to an interested adult and to hear conversations where they are allowed to join in are at a distinct advantage (Mother of 5 year old twins). 31 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Family 5 & 6 Family 5 have a daughter who is 4 years old. Both parents are English. Her father is a flight safety manager and her mother is currently a full time student. The child has received all three Bookstart packs. Analysing the questionnaire (appendix vi) it is quite clear that the Bookstart Programme has enhanced shared reading practices within this family. Whilst the parents had already established good reading habits with their child, it was felt that since receiving the Bookstart packs their reading practices had improved and they are now sharing books with their child daily. The child takes an immense interest in books and ‘always’ was indicated on the questionnaire in answer to; asks questions, contributes and joins in with telling the story, makes predictions about stories and points to words, letters and pictures. This all indicates that shared reading is a common occurrence in this household. It would appear that the child feels confident with books, enjoys shared reading experiences and, since receiving the free books, her mother feels that she takes a lot more interest in books. The book box belongs solely to my daughter and is very precious to her. She brings her box and chooses a book for us to read (Mother of 4 year old). Family 6 have a boy of 17 months. Both parents are English. His father is a full time parent and his mother a primary school teacher. The child has received the baby Bookstart pack and the Bookstart + set. Within this family there is a regular reading routine which had been established prior to the free gift of books. The child’s mother who completed 32 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 the questionnaire (appendix vii) was pleased to be introduced to some different books from Bookstart - books that she may not have chosen herself. Her child was the youngest of all my participants so unfortunately question 4, based on language was not so relevant. However the child’s mother was very keen to tell me that her son is fantastic at imitating reading and loves to hold a book and babble. She enthusiastically advocates encouragement about books ‘rather than formal approach at early age’ (mother of 17 month old). The Bookstart Co-ordinators perspective This section outlines the perspective of the Bookstart Co-ordinator whom I interviewed in March (recording 6). She was very positive about the Bookstart Programme. Her job as coordinator is a full time post suggesting that the programme is well funded and wholly supports the raising of literacy skills in children. It’s a huge amount of funding going into this programme, if you consider it’s National…that message in itself is saying that there’s something valuable in this (Bookstart Co-ordinator). She expressed appreciation towards the large financial commitment from private and public charities, sponsorship from over 25 children’s publishers and funding from Sure Start, amounting to approximately £60 per child for three sets of books. 33 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 The Bookstart Co-ordinator especially wished to have it noted that funding for Bookstart Cornwall came from the European Social Funding and the Learning Skills Council. Part of the reason for this funding is that Cornwall is a designated Objective One area. Objective One (2006) has been set up to help to reduce some of the social and economic differences within the European Union, primarily aimed at removing disparities in wealth across the European Union. Goals include the restructuring of deteriorating industrial areas and the diversity of rural areas where there is declining agriculture We met at the baby bounce and rhyme time session in a local library (appendix viii). I have not gone into detail about this session; however the activity, held for any-aged pre-school child, was supported by a library assistant who read stories to the children and held a singing session. 5 children attended. The afternoon included a visit from Bookstart Bear - a giant life-size teddy bear invited to befriend the children and promote the Bookstart Programme. My brief impression was that it was an enjoyable experience for the children that attended; I observed them listening attentively to the stories, asking for additional readings and joined in enthusiastically with the music. Parents observed high quality modelling of shared book reading, echoing part of the vision of the Bookstart Programme. My subsequent interview with the Bookstart Co-ordinator started with a brief history of Bookstart in Cornwall. Once funding came directly to Bookstart Cornwall, the Bookstart team took over the responsibility and the coordination of the programme from the library service. There has been a 34 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 steady increase in active library users in Cornwall (appendix ix). County library statistics show that active children (under 16) who have borrowed at least one library item within the financial year, have risen from 24,920 in 20052006, to a figure of 25,637 in 2006-2007 - an increase of 717 (06/07 figures are un-audited therefore are provisional figures only and subject to change). From 06/07, the library will be keeping figures of active library users in the 0-4s age group and this year’s (pre-audit) figures are 3,697. However there is no evidence to conclude that the increase in active library users is solely due to the Bookstart Programme. Bookstart maintain a positive partnership with county libraries, linking library sessions such as the baby bounce and rhyme time and story sack workshops. The coordinator felt that the involvement with the library was a significant part of the project and put a lot of emphasis on the story sack courses. ‘Part of our funding enables us to offer every parent a chance to attend a story sacks course ‘(Bookstart Co-ordinator). The story sack course gives parents the opportunity to have further involvement in the Bookstart Programme, such as craft activities, making resources to supplement the story books and workshops to learn different story telling techniques. Story sack courses also offer adult learning advice to parents offering opportunities to enhance their own learning opportunities, linking with Cornwall County Councils promotion of adult continued learning programmes (appendix x). 35 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Link into Learning: Link into learning offers free, friendly, flexible help with the essential skills of adult literacy, numeracy and English for speakers of other languages Family Learning: Family learning is for parents and carers who want to become more involved in their children’s education. Additionally it offers improved job prospects as parents can improve, gain new skills and continue with their own learning. Parents often overcome negative learning experiences and confidence improves (Cornwall County Council 2007). The Co-ordinator expressed total commitment to the programme and stated that, in her opinion, the free books for children had made big contributions towards children’s reading and language development ‘It enhances language development and it enhances early literacy skills’ (Bookstart Co-ordinator). An area of difficulty touched upon by the co-ordinator was in regard to the distribution of the packs. There appear to be limited National guidelines about this, and ambiguity about the ages at which children should receive the packs. Contributing to the problem is that many agencies gift the packs and therefore tracking which families had received the packs is logistically problematic. Consequently, Cornwall, where the co-ordinator is based, has devised its own system and categorises children in line with the school academic years, i.e. Bookstart + was gifted to children in 2006/7 if their birthdates fall between September 2003-August 2004 and Treasure Chests were gifted to children born in the months of September 2002-August 2003. This system has been much more efficient (appendix xi). 36 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 I asked the co-ordinator about any problematic aspects of the Bookstart Programme and she felt that in families where parents had literacy difficulties, some of the guidance materials (appendices xii & xiii), particularly Bookstart + and Treasure Chests had overwhelmed some parents. Where this had been acknowledged, literature from the baby packs was included with the older books and support offered. Care is also taken with families whose English is not their first language; here dual language books are gifted (appendix xiv). Twins were also acknowledged as individual children and each would receive a book pack containing different books. The Co-ordinator shared the fact that the books are carefully selected and reflect a socio-economic mix, including ethnic minority groups, disabilities and diverse belief systems, (appendix xv) and emphasised that the books were a gift to every child and were not limited to deprived families and minority groups. She held a very strong view that the programme was not just about a delivery service and that her team offered an all round service; ‘The packs are no good on their own’ (Bookstart co-ordinator). These comments very much reflect the vision of the Bookstart Programme. Her emphasis was that the love of books should be embedded in every child across the country and due to the success of the Bookstart programme a fourth delivery of books is likely to be introduced to 5 and 11 year olds. 37 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 At no time had the Co-ordinator experienced a parent not wishing to accept the free gift of books. However, she commented that a parent of a young baby wished to bypass the baby books and had requested a set of toddler books instead. The co-ordinator reassured the parent that the young child concerned would be sure to enjoy the baby books and in time would receive the toddler books as well. There is therefore a danger that in some cases parents may view the gift of books as an educational tool and not fully appreciate that shared reading experiences is more than teaching a child to read. From my impression, Bookstart Cornwall appears to a collaborative partnership with libraries, children centres and families. ‘We’re giving them [the parents] opportunities and it’s about whether they take these opportunities or not…They [the parents] get excited about it cause we’re all excited about it’ (Bookstart Coordinator). 38 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Summary of key findings Analysis drawn from; (1) observations of shared reading experiences with 4 families, (2) semi-structured questionnaires from 6 families and (3) an interview with the County Bookstart Co-ordinator. Analysis of semi-structured parent questionnaires Nationality Age and sex of child At least 1 professional working parent Bookstart packs received from Health Visitors Bookstart packs received at day care settings All eligible book packs received Did Bookstart help the family to do any new things? Families that daily share books with their children Since Bookstart, does the child have the same interest in books? Since Bookstart, does the child have a lot more interest in books? Family 1 Family 2 Family 3 Family 5 Family 6 British Dual British British British 3:8 Boy 2:3 girl 3 girl 4 girl 1:5months boy √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ X X √ √ √ X X X √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ √ Family 4 are not accounted for in this table as they did not pursue the Bookstart Programme due to personal choice regarding their belief in the Steiner philosophy. However, I think the following comment from this family adds weight to the importance of book sharing, regardless of one’s belief systems: 39 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 I read to my children daily but do not use this time to teach them to read – that will come later at their appropriate developmental stage – somewhere between their 6th + 7th year. The children enjoy sharing books but I also tell stories with no books as I think this is a better way of engaging their imaginations, developing listening skills… (Mother of 5 year old twins). The Bookstart Co-ordinator and families 1,2,3,5 and 6 unequivocally supported the Bookstart Programme as being an effective way of encouraging shared reading experiences. All were aware of the Bookstart message and the importance of shared reading. Conclusion My research project has been a small scale study looking at evidence to support early book sharing experiences at home. I believe that I have corroborated research - Wells (1987), Miller (1996), Whitehead (1999), Oldfield (2001) and (Evans 1997) - signifying that families who value and share books with their children are helping them to develop good literacy skills, language advancement and help to advance social and emotional maturity. The Bookstart Programme, according to my findings, is fulfilling its vision by offering these opportunities to families through the gifting of free books. My findings and conclusion echo the research (Moore & Wade 1993) showing that the Bookstart Programme was demonstrably successful in promoting the importance of early book-sharing experiences within families. All participants in my research study contributed positive attitudes about Bookstart and 100% of the interviewed families read daily with their children. One of my 40 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 participants (Family 4) held strong views about children’s books but was nevertheless able to recognise that in some cases the Bookstart Programme could have positive effects upon early childhood development; however she felt that books used as a tool for formally teaching children the skills of reading should be reserved until after the age of 6 years. A weakness in my research has been the difficulty in finding a socio-economic mix of families. For this reason it has been hard to compare data between families who have received the gifted books against those who have not. Given the opportunity to develop my research further, it would have been interesting to extend the research in this way. I think my research would have been enhanced if I had gathered data from the so called ‘hard to reach’ families who have not taken advantage of the Bookstart Programme either intentionally or unintentionally and for a diverse mix of reasons do not access day care settings, libraries, health visitors. Families in these circumstances may miss out on the support available by the Bookstart Programme by the occasional distribution difficulties. Distribution issues were highlighted in my data where 2 out of 5 (40%) of eligible families had not been gifted their second Bookstart pack. My views have broadened. Before I researched this topic I perhaps had a narrow-minded view of children’s reading development and viewed it primarily as an academic process. Since undertaking this research I am more able to appreciate that the process of shared family reading has multi purposes, much broader than I initially understood. My perspective of children’s 41 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 development has altered, particularly in light of the Steiner philosophy whose distinctive approach to child development provides a balance of academic, social and artistic elements. I more firmly believe that educational professionals and policy makers, who focus on curricula, academic achievements, targets and results, may achieve only short term or limited success. I have become more sympathetic towards the Steiner movement which is child centred and does not concern itself by government economic forces. Whilst it is clear that some children are able to be numerate and literate before the age of 6 years, this may have long term negative consequences, such as feelings of inadequacy or underachievement if the child is pressurised earlier than he or she is physically and/or emotionally ready. I have learned that the nature of early learning is self-motivated, through active feeling, touching, exploring and imitating, allowing the child to come to know the world in the way most appropriate to his or her age and ability. Bookstart is instrumental in this and supports families in meeting the developmental stages of children, offering language, social, sensory, emotional and academic nourishment. I fell that I am now closer to understanding why some children are unmotivated to read. It does seem very clear that positive parental attitudes towards books and families who enjoy, encourage and share stories together have a profound effect on children’s long term attitude towards reading. If my research was larger scale and a longer time scale I would have liked to 42 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 expand my study to include attitudes, achievements and perspectives from teachers, parents and children at primary schools. It would be interesting to compare how home literacy experiences impact upon children at school. Initiative such as Bookstart seeks to target family learning and values effective partnerships between parents and children. I have seen how the Bookstart Programme fosters and develops these opportunities for families. I believe that through my readings and broadened understanding, I can now appreciate more widely the significance of developing children’s interest and motivation towards books. I value much more the impact books have particularly how extensively they are linked with language development, something that I had previously not fully grasped. Moreover, this research project has helped me to realise that story telling helps children put their life’s experiences into context thereby helping them to make sense of the world. As my first piece of university research I have found my choice of topic very interesting and value the support that I have received from all my participants. I am also very satisfied with how much I have learned through this process. 43 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 References Allberg, A and Allberg, J (1999) Each Peach Pear Plum. Viking Kestrel Picture Books: New York Almond, D and Lambert, S (2004) Kate, The Cat and the Moon. Hodder Children’s Books: London Bell, J (2005) Doing your Research Project. Open University Press: Berkshire Collins, F., Svenson, C., Mahony, P (2005) ‘Bookstart: Planting a seed for life Roehampton University, London, 1-123 Cornwall County Council [online] http://www.cornwall.gov.uk/index.cfm?articleid=455 [28 March 2007) DfES. ‘What Sure Start Is Here to Do [Online] http://www.surestart.gov.uk/aboutsurestart/ [13th February 2007] Dodd, L (2006) The Other Ark, Puffin Books: London Evans, J (1997), Oral Language, Developmental Continuum, Western Australia, Rigby Heinemann Green, S (2000) Research Methods in Health, Social and Early Years Care. Stanley Thornes Ltd: Cheltenham Grieg, A and Taylor, J (1999) Doing Research with Children. Sage Publications Ltd: London Hayes, N and Orrell, S (1993) Psychology An Introduction. Third edition, England: Addison Wesley Longman Ltd 55, 1, 1-12 Hunt, H (2007) Bookstart Co-ordinator, Conversation with author, 14th March. Mayo, M (2001) Dig Dig Digging, London: Orchard Books Mayo, M (2002) Emergency!, Orchard Books: London Mc Guinness, D. (1998) Why Children Can’t Read. Penguin Books: Great Britain Moore, M and Wade, B (2003) Bookstart: a qualitative evaluation. Educational Review, 55 (1) Miller, L (1996) Towards Reading. Open University Press: Buckingham 44 Catherine Hunn: EEC 315 Objective One [online] http://www.objectiveone.com/O1htm/whatis.htm [28March 2007] Oldfield, L (2001) Free to Learn: Introducing Steiner Waldorf Early Childhood Education, Gloucestershire: Hawthorn Press Roberts-Holmes, G (2000) Doing Your Early Years Research Project. Paul Chapman Publishing: London Salter J (1987) The Incarnating Child, Gloucestershire: Hawthorn Press Sharp, C (2002) ‘School Starting Age: European Policy and Recent Research’ [Online] http://www.nfer.ac.uk/publications/other-publications/conferencepapers/school-starting-age-european-policy-and-recent-research.cfm [14th April 2007] Smith et al (1993) Understanding Children’s Development. Fourth edition, United Kingdom: Blackwell Publishing Smith, P., Cowie, H., Blades, M. (2003) Understanding Children’s Development, Fourth Edition, Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd Smith, T (1980) Parents and Preschool, London: Grant McIntyre Ltd Wade, B and Moore, M (1996b) Home Activities: The Advent of Literacy, European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 4, pp. 63-76 Weinberger, J (1996) A Longitudinal study of children’s early literacy experiences at home and later literacy development at home and school. Journal of Research in reading, 19 (1), 14-24. Wells, G (1985) Language, Learning and Education, Berkshire: NFERNELSON Publishing Company Wells, G (1987) The Meaning Makers. Children Learning Language and Using Language to Learn, Portsmouth: Heinemann Educational Books Inc Whitehead, M (1999) Supporting Language and Literacy Development in the early years, Buckingham: Open University Press Williams, S and Lewis, T (1999) Helping Young Children to Read, Hodder & Stoughton: Great Britain 45