JS: Lect 4: Ethnomethodology II HAROLD GARFINKEL (1967) CHAPTER 3: COMMON SENSE KNOWLEDGE OF SOCIAL STRUCTURES: THE DOCUMENTARY METHOD OF INTERPRETATION IN LAY AND PROFESSIONAL FACT FINDING, pp.76-103 “Ethnomethodological studies analyze everyday activities as members methods for making those same activities visibly-rational-and-reportable-for-all-practical-purposes, i.e., ‘accountable’, as organizations of commonplace everyday activities” (1967, p.vii). “Sociologically speaking, ‘common culture’ refers to the socially sanctioned grounds of inference and action that people use in their everyday affairs and which they assume that others use in the same way... The discovery of common culture consists of the discovery from within the society by social scientists of the existence of common sense knowledge of social structures... [The research] is directed to a description of the work whereby decisions of meaning and fact are managed, and how a body of [seemingly] factual knowledge of social structures is assembled in common sense situations of choice. (pp.76-77). __________________________________ “There are innumerable situations of sociological inquiry in which the investigator – whether he be a professional sociologist or a person undertaking an inquiry about social structures in the interests of managing his practical everyday affairs – can assign witnessed actual appearances to the status of an event of conduct only by imputing biography and prospects to the appearances. This he does by embedding the appearances in his presupposed knowledge of social structures. This it frequently happens that in order for the investigator to decide what he is now looking at he must wait for future developments, only to find that these futures in turn are informed by their history and future” (p.77). “It therefore occurs that the investigator frequently must elect among alternative courses of interpretation and inquiry to the end of deciding matters of fact, hypothesis, conjecture, fancy, and the rest, despite the fact that in the calculable sense of the term ‘know’, he does not and cannot ‘know’ what he is doing prior to or while he is doing it” (pp.77-78). ___________________________________ “How is it done by an investigator that from replies to a questionnaire he finds the respondent’s ‘attitude’; that via interviews with office personnel he reports their ‘bureaucratically organized activities’.... [etc.]. What is the work whereby the investigator sets the observed occurrence and the intended occurrence into a correspondence of meaning, such that the investigator finds it reasonable to treat witnessed actual appearances as evidences of the event he means to be studying?” (p.79). “To answer these questions it is necessary to detail the work of the documentary method. To this end a demonstration of the documentary method was designed to exaggerate the features of this method in use and to catch the work of ‘fact production’ in flight” (p.79). But what is the ‘documentary method’?: “The method consists of taking an actual appearance as ‘the document of’, as ‘pointing to’, as ‘standing on behalf of’ a presupposed underlying pattern. Not only is the underlying patten derived from its documentary evidences, but the individual documentary evidences, in their turn, are interpreted on the basis of ‘what is known’ about the underlying pattern” (p.78). _______________________________________ Experiment: S S S S S Ten undergraduates Dept of Psychiatry - to explore alternative “ways of giving people advice about their personal problems” First discuss background, and then ask ten questions, to receive a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answer. After each answer, to remove wall plug, and record some comments on it. Final comments... (Example transcript) Nine findings: Finding 1. (p.89): “Typically the subjects heard the experimenter’s answers as answers-to-the questions. Perceptually, the experimenter’s answers were motivated by the questions.” And one cannot say here ‘Of course they were’, because the simple fact to bear in mind is that the ‘answers’ were not in fact answers to the questions. People had been ‘set up’ to deal creatively and artfully (Garfinkel would say) with these random utterances - and they do so! People begin to create a set of ‘meanings’ which will ‘make sense’ of what the experimenter (or pseudo-advisor) says. These ‘meanings’ acquire their content, not from any information provided within the advisor’s replies, but from something much more complex - from, what for the moment, we can just call the background in terms of which people routinely make sense of one another’s actions in their society. Finding 2. (p.89): “All [subjects] reported ‘the advice they had been given’ and addressed their appreciation and criticism to that advice.” In other words, although the ‘advisor’ had said only ‘yes’ or ‘no’, subjects had ‘lent’ or ‘given’ those answers a meaning which were not within the answers themselves - “I felt his answers were helpful..,” “The conversation and the answers given I believe had a lot of meaning to me” - The subjects ‘gave to’, ‘lent to’, ‘made for’ the responses of the advisor a meaning in terms of their own conceptions. Subjects then, in attempted to sustain a meaningful social relationship, then responded to that meaning (which they themselves had created). Finding 3. (p.89): “Over the course of the exchange the assumption seemed to operate that there was an answer to be obtained, and that if the answer was not obvious, that its meaning could be determined by active search, one part of which involved asking another question so as to discover what the advisor ‘had in mind’.” When a person embarks upon a social transaction, the general ‘theme’ of the engagement - in this case: advice-giving - is established by the making of certain initial commitments, which continue to determine how each step in the interaction should be interpreted throughout its whole course. The overall meaning of the transaction is sustained by the actor’s (the student’s) capacity to continue to interpret each of the ‘advisor’s’ responses as relevant to that meaning - even when they are not! In this finding Garfinkel begins to display the extent to which an everyday social exchange relies upon factors which are far removed from the current situation, but are ‘brought in’ from the background to ‘fill it in’, and to supply it with a sense of continuity. Finding 4. (p.90): “The identical utterance was capable of answering several different questions simultaneously, and of constituting an answer to a compound question that in terms of the strict logic of propositions did not permit a single yes or no [answer].” People are not logical in much of their social transactions; this does not mean, however, that they are unconcerned with rationality in their everyday behaviour. Clearly, individuals attempt to find support for the particular position they have adopted: calling on evidence; giving reasons; etc. But the fact is, being rational in day-to-day discourse is not the same as strictly scientific discourse (see Garfinkel, 1967, Ch.8 discussed in next lecture - where he shows ‘experimentally’ how attempts to speak scientifically, destroy to possibility of everyday communication). Finding 5. (p.91): “More subjects entertained the possibility of a trick than tested this possibility. All suspicious subjects were reluctant to act under the belief that there was a trick involved. Suspicions were reduced if the advisor’s answers made ‘good sense’. Suspicions were least likely to continue if the answers accorded with the subject’s previous thought about the matter and with his preferred decisions.” One of the major features of social behaviour is the extent to which people are unwilling to call into question what is going on; rather than questioning it, people tend to wait to see whether ‘they can work it out’ in the normal course of the interaction. Many rules operate in social exchange, says Garfinkel, not because they have withstood the test of time, but the opposite: they simply have not been tested. “Indeed,” he says (p.70), “the more important the rule, the greater is the likelihood that knowledge is based upon avoided tests” - e.g., the attribution of meaning to people’s statements - tests which would reveal uncertainties and ambiguities in some of our basic assumptions. Finding 6. (p.91): “Throughout there was a concern and search for pattern. Pattern was likely to be seen in the first evidence of the ‘advice’.” It seems inconceivable that “there is no order in the universe” (Eco). People seem to come into social relationships with a pre-established sense of pattern, of events as fitting into a whole in relation to one another. He points out that when subjects were told that they had received no advice at all, just random answers, meaningless in themselves, they found it difficult to cope with that fact; they could make little sense of it. At best they shifted their interpretation of the counsellor’s comments from ‘advice giving’ to ‘deceit’. While the subjects had been naive, they had accepted the ‘advice’ as advice. It was not the character of the ‘advice’ itself which determined whether it was accepted as advice. Some other condition or background factor determined that. So, suggests Garkinkel, the character of the advice being received is not a function of the advice being given; rather, it is a conceptualization of the advice by the recipient, and as such, the recipient needs to ‘check it out’ to see if they have conceptualized it aright. The random responses, seen as an event, provide what Garfinkel calls the documentation for an already established pattern or theme. Finding 7. (p.92) “ Subjects assigned to the advisor, as his advice, the thought formulated in the subject’s questions. For example, when a subject asked ‘Should I come to school every night after supper to do my studying?’ and the experimenter said “My answer is no?’, the subject in his comments said “he said I shouldn’t come to school and study’. This was very common.” This observation has a great number of implications; it suggests among other things: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Messages that carry no meaning in themselves can, within the process of communication, work to contribute towards the ‘making’ of a meaning (indexical vs. objective expressions); a message does not exist as an isolated event and can be given no meaning as such on its own (an event signifies in terms of its ‘place’ or ‘position’ within a whole background schemes of things a form of life); understanding the significance of a message within a group (by an outside observer) calls for a comprehensive knowledge of the social and cultural backgrounds of the group members, including knowledge of how those backgrounds are brought to bear in the interpretation of messages; the meaning of a message for sender and receiver cannot be the same i) unless the backgrounds of both are the same, and ii) the receiver takes the trouble to establish the sameness of meaning what are the criteria of this? distortion is a common occurrence in communication. It is a maxim of social psychology that the individual and society are two sides of the same coin. Garfinkel reaffirms this, but not in terms of people possessing shared values or shared beliefs, but in terms of them possessing shared “means’, shared methods, i.e., ways of ‘making sense’ of occurrences in their form of life. Especially in Garfinkel’s view, individuals may be unique individuals. For people involved in social acts may shape and influence the situation which is also shaping and influencing them; the common systems for the use of signs does not limit them to a single, common, point of view. When subjects were told, after the ‘counselling’ session that they had contributed a great deal to the ‘advice’ they had received, they were amazed. Finding 8. (pp.92-93): “Subjects made specific reference to various social structures in deciding the sensible and warranted character of the advisor’s advice... References the subject supplied were to social structures which [the subject] treated as actually of potentially known in common with the adviser. And then, not to any social structures known in common, but to normatively valued social structures which the subject accepted as the conditions that his decisions, with respect to his own... grasp of his circumstances and the adviser’s advice, had to satisfy. These social structures consisted of normative features seen from within which, for the subject, were definitive of his membership of the various collectivities that were referred to.” In other words, when the subject says “I happen to be of the Jewish faith and I have been dating a Gentile girl..’ he knows that various directives are associated with the social institutions involved, and that his own action has to be a particular instance of these directions. The subject is trying to define for himself a new ‘Jewish-Gentile institution’ and attempting to establish the reality of that institution in his conversation with the adviser. Thus the conversation takes the form of a game in which one player (the subject) is uncertain of the rules, and seems to be testing out the other (the adviser) to discover what they are - the subject uses the experimenter ass the already briefed upholder of the rules. This is a conception of people as creative agents who fashion between themselves the central institutional structures within which they see themselves as significantly involved - institutions fashioned for the moment only - but which are none the less used as a context for rendering their own behaviour meaningful. Finding 9. (p.94): “Through the work of documenting - i.e., by searching for and determining pattern, by treating the adviser’s answers as motivated by the intended sense of the question, by waiting for later answers to clarify the sense of the previous ones, by finding answers to unasked questions - the perceivedly normal values of what was being advised were established, tested, reviewed, retained, restored; in word, managed. It is misleading therefore to think of the documentary method as a procedure whereby a propositions are accorded membership of a scientific corpus. Rather the documentary method developed the advice so as to be continually ‘membershipping’ it. In the course of the conversations and other kinds of social exchange, people behave in terms of, i.e., use as resources, social norms and manners (some of which are improvised for the immediate occasion to hand). But people do not respond in a simple mechanical manner to pre-given socio-cultural norms, nor do they merely express pre-established personality characteristics. Individuals, from an ethnomethodological perspective, are continually in need of other people as agents through which they can continuously confirm their own uncertain and anxious sense of what and who they are within the community.