Assessment, Analysis, and Cross-Cultural Validation of Autonomous and Related Self Construals Cigdem Kagitcibasi Nazli Baydar Zeynep Cemalcilar Koc University, Istanbul, Turkey 23.04.2010 Author’s note: We want to acknowledge the important contributions of Ayse AycicegiDinn, Derya Gungor, Pelin Kesebir, Echo Liao, Ozlem Otyakmaz, and So-wan Wong for data collection in Turkey, Belgium, the U.S.A, Hong Kong, Germany, and the U.K., respectively. Kagitcibasi’s work on this article was partially supported by the Turkish Academy of Sciences. 1 Autonomy and relatedness have long been considered basic needs by several theoretical perspectives in psychology (Kagitcibasi, 2005)). They are also viewed as aspects of independent and interdependent self, respectively. In the last two decadescultural/cross-cultural social psychological contributions, mostly conducted within the individualism-collectivism framework (e.g. Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2004, Kitayama, 2002; Kitayama, Duffy & Uchida, 2007; Oyserman, Kemmelmeier & Coon, 2002; Schimmack, Oishi & Diener, 2005; Triandis, 1995; Triandis, Bontempo, Villareal, Asai & Lucca, 1988), and developmental perspectives (e.g. Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni & Maynard, 2003) have been influential. However, the meanings attributed to independence-interdependence have been rather diffuse. . They mirror those of individualism and collectivism, often the two dichotomous sets of constructs being used interchangeably. The present paper aims to bring clarification to the conceptual and operational definitions of autonomy and relatedness and offers a cross-cultural validation of these constructs. Autonomy and Relatedness Kagitcibasi proposed ‘dependence-independence’ as a basic dimension, ‘having to do with human merging and separation’ (1990, p. 154). This can be seen as the dimension of ‘interpersonal distance’, reflecting the degree to which the self is connected with others, and the extent to which the self boundaries are well defined or diffuse and permeable (Kagitcibasi, 1996). Both individual and cultural differences exist in this dimension. Subsequent conceptualizations, particularly by Markus and Kitayama (1991) and others following suit (e.g. Gudykunst, Matsumoto, Ting-Toomey & Nishida, 1996; Singelis, 1994) attributed further meaning to these constructs so that independent self came to imply a combination of separateness from others (uniqueness) together with autonomy, and interdependent self came to indicate a blending of relatedness with lack of autonomy. These more inclusive 2 conceptualizations are common in cross-cultural social psychological research (for an extensive review see Kagitcibasi, 2005, 2007). A Autonomy and relatedness have been construed as conflicting in much theory and research despite their acceptance as basic needs. Thus separation from others is seen as a requisite for autonomy. The theoretical background of this view can be traced to psychoanalytic thinking, especially to “separation-individuation hypothesis” in infancy (Blos, 1979; Mahler, Pine & Bergman, 1975). The construal of autonomy as separateness also influenced adolescent research stressing the importance of the separation of the adolescent from parents as a second ‘separation-individuation process’ (Hoffman, 1984; Kroger, 1998; Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986). utonomy has been prioritized in psychology, even in cross-cultural psychology, reflecting the Western individualistic world view. Yet autonomy concerns human agency, not relatedness. To clarify the issue, Kagitcibasi (1996, 2005, 2007) proposed ‘agency’ and ‘interpersonal distance’ as two distinct underlying dimensions of self. Agency spans autonomy to heteronomy, and interpersonal distance spans separateness to relatedness. A similar conceptualization was put forward by the ‘selfdetermination theory’ which proposed autonomy and relatedness as distinct needs, also adding competence (Chirkov, Kim, Ryan & Kaplan, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Ryan, Deci & Grolnick, 1995). The joint operationalization of agency and interpersonal distance has been rather problematic. Some reviews questioned the empirical support, even the validity, of the independentinterdependent self construal (Matsumoto, 1999; Takano & Osaka, 1999). Several critics 3 challenged the dichotomous conceptualization of independence-interdependence as well as individualism-collectivism (Killen, 1997; Raeff, 2004). Others examined the structure of self construal scales, revealing multifactorial structures rather than single factor structures (Beyers, Goossens, Vansant & Moors, 2003; Hardin, Leong & Bhagwat, 2004; Kashima & Hardie, 2000; Uleman, Rhee, Bardoliwalla, Semin & Toyama, 2000). were made at conceptual clarification for example by proposing two types of relatedness (Rothbaum & Trommsdorff, 2006) and two types of agency (Markus & Kitayama, 2004). The prevailing conceptualization of autonomy and relatedness as two poles of a single dimension has also influenced the measurement of these self construals. Many individualism/ independent self scales tap separateness but also autonomy, and collectivism/ interdependent self scales tap relatedness but also heteronomy (Hui & Yee, 1994; Gudykunst, et al., 1996; Kitayama, Markus, Kurokawa, Tummala & Kato, 1991; Matsumoto, Weissman, Preston, Brown & Kupperbusch, 1997; Singelis, 1994; Triandis, et al.,1988). Given the way these self construals have been measured, relatedness does not figure in an independent self, and autonomy in an interdependent self. By implication, the importance, even the existence of autonomy in closelyknit collectivistic cultures has been questioned (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Oishi, 2000; Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, Miyake & Weisz, 2000). Yet autonomy concerns human agency, not relatedness. To clarify the issue, Kagitcibasi (1996, 2005, 2007) proposed ‘agency’ and ‘interpersonal distance’ as two distinct underlying dimensions of self. Agency spans autonomy to heteronomy, and interpersonal distance spans separateness to relatedness. A similar conceptualization was put forward by the ‘selfdetermination theory’ which proposed autonomy and relatedness as distinct needs, also adding competence (Chirkov, Kim, Ryan & Kaplan, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Ryan, Deci & Grolnick, 4 1995).Pursuing the distinction of the two underlying dimensions of agency and interpersonal distance and considering their four-fold combination, Kagitcibasi (1996, 2005, 2007) proposed four prototypical self configurations, the ‘autonomous-separate’, ‘autonomous-related’, ‘heteronomous-separate’, and ‘heteronomous-related’ selves. Research provides support to the distinctness and compatibility of autonomy and relatedness, confirming the validity of Kagitcibasi’s conceptualization (Beyers et al., 2003; Celenk, 2007; Huiberts, Oosterwegel, van der Valk, Volleberg & Meeus, 2006; Kim, Butzel & Ryan,1998; Kwak, 2003; Meeus, Oosterwegel and Vollebergh, 2002). In the light of the above considerations, autonomy is defined here as willful agency. In other words, it involves an agentic self, acting willfully. This definition is akin to Self Determination Theory’s point that ‘true agency requires autonomy (Ryan et al., 1995, p. 624) as well as to other conceptualizations (Beyers, Goossens, Vansant & Moors, 2003). It is to be noted that this definition of autonomy does not imply more or less relatedness. Thus, autonomy can coexist with both separateness and relatedness. This is because, relatedness refers to the other dimension, i.e. ‘interpersonal distance’. While autonomy refers to one pole of the agency dimension, relatedness refers to one pole of the interpersonal distance dimension. It connotes a self construal with closely knit interpersonal ties.Given the growing recognition of the importance of autonomy and relatedness constructs and their combination, there is a great need for the empirical analysis and assessment of these constructs. In particular, devising an autonomy scale that does not tap relatedness (separateness) and a relatedness scale that does not tap autonomy (heteronomy) is called for. The present paper pursues four aims: (1) to present autonomous and related self scales that are conceptually distinct with established psychometric properties; (2) to establish the cross-cultural measurement invariance of Autonomous Self and 5 Related Self scales; (3) to examine the association of these two constructs empirically across cultures; and, (4) to explore whether there exist ideal types of self construals that are seen in different cultures with varying frequencies. In the first study, we describe the scale construction process and report the validation analysis of the newly developed scales using two different validation samples composed of Turkish youth. In the second study, we report cross-cultural analyses of these scales, with data from seven different cultural groups. Study 1: Construction and Validation of Autonomous-Related Self Scales Autonomous and Related Self Scales were developed and validated in two phases. The first phase was devoted to item selection and scale construction and the second phase was devoted to the validation of the scales. Phase 1: Scale Construction A group of experts generated a large set of items to assess the autonomous, related, and autonomous-related self items based on Kagitcibasi’s theory (1996). This set was reduced to a 40-item pool and was administered to 117 university students in Istanbul, Turkey. Mean age of the sample was 21.8 years (SD=3.08) and 72% were females. Participants indicated their agreement with each item on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Following a principal component analysis, items with factor loadings above .45 were selected to be in the final scale. The resulting 27 items constituted the Autonomous, Related and the AutonomousRelated Self scales, each consisting of 9 items and collectively referred to as the ARS scales. These are balanced scales, with some of the items negatively worded, to minimize acquiescent response bias. The items, item factor loadings and reliabilities of the scales are presented in Table 1. Parallel scales of Autonomous, Related and Autonomous-Related self for measuring the construal of self specifically in the family context were also devised. These latter scales may 6 be used in studies that focus on self-construal from a developmental perspective (not given here; see, Kagitcibasi, 2007, pages 194-197). The association between the three scales are discussed in Phase 2, the validation study. --------------- Insert Table 1 about here --------------------Phase 2: Validation study Validation of the ARS Scales was done using two emergent adult samples that differed by socio-economic status. The samples were administered the same set of measures pertaining to attitudinal, personality and family attributes that were hypothesized to be related to their self construals. The predictions regarding the association of the ARS scales with conceptually related constructs were similar for both validation samples because the construct validity of the ARS scales should not vary across samples. Validation samples: The first sample was composed of university students, similar to the sample in Phase 1. The second sample consisted of young adults who were at the same age group as the first sample but had less than university education. The latter sample was dissimilar to the sample that was used for the development of the scales in Phase 1 and was used to further test the validity of the ARS. Having two samples with divergent characteristics provided insights into the variations in self construals and their associations with family background characteristics. The demographic characteristics of the validation samples are given in Table 2. ------ Insert Table 2 about here----------Both samples were mostly urban but had significant differences in several indicators of socio-economic status. The first validation sample, composed of 477 university students, came from relatively well educated, nuclear families with middle to high income levels. The mean age 7 was 20.5 years (SD=1.78), and sixty-two percent were females. The mean number of siblings was 2.6. The self rated family affluence was higher, and the self rated religiosity was lower in the first sample compared with the second sample. The second validation sample, composed of 200 youth (mean age: 20.9; SD=2.12) had a mean education of 9.6 years (SD=2.03). Fifty nine percent had high school degree, and the rest had less than high school education. Half of the participants were males. None were married. A majority (59.8%) were working to earn a living. Women had higher education than men (10.0 years and 9.3 years, respectively, F(1,190)=5.4, p <.05), and men were more likely to be working then women (68% and 51.5% respectively, χ2 =5.6, p < .05). This sample had more siblings, and their parents had less education than the first sample. Thus the second sample has low SES traditional families, as reflected in their family structure (more siblings, larger families, less educated parents) and more conservative values (higher religiosity). However, they may be seen as a ‘transitional’ (or upwardly mobile) group, given their higher level of education compared to their parents. Measures and hypotheses Eight measures were identified to be conceptually related to the ARS scales and were administered to two different validation samples. Four of these measures assess relational self construal and attributes such as internal control and self efficacy. The other four are retrospective measures of childhood experiences such as family control, affection, and childhood environment. The predicted relations with these latter measures provide a developmental perspective on the self. The validation measures are described below, and the predictions regarding their associations with the ARS scales are explained following each description. Internal Control Scale: A new 6-item internal control scale was constructed based on the Turkish adaptation (Kagitcibasi, 1970) of Rotter’s Internal-External Locus of Control Scale 8 (Rotter, 1966). The scale assessed the degree to which respondents believe that their life is under their own control. Items were rated on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher internal control (α=.65). Sample items included “I manage my life myself” and “Everyone is responsible for his/her actions.” We expected a positive association between the Internal Control Scale and the Autonomous Self and the Autonomous-Related Self scales. The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (RISC). This 11-item scale assessed respondent’s tendency to think of himself/herself in terms of his/her relationships with close others (Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000). Items were rated on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher relational-interdependent self. Sample items included “My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am” and “If a person hurts someone close to me, I feel personally hurt as well.” Alpha coefficient of the original scale was .88 (Cross et al., 2000), and was .78 in the present study. We expected the Related Self and the Autonomous-Related Self Scale scores to be positively associated with the RISC scale scores. General Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale: This 10-item scale is a reduced version of the original 20-item self-efficacy scale developed by Schwarzer and Jerusalem (1995) and measures the optimistic self beliefs to cope with difficult demands in life. Items were rated on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher perceived self-efficacy. Sample items included “I am confident that I could deal efficiently with unexpected events” and “I can remain calm when facing difficulties because I can rely on my coping abilities.” Internal reliability of the original scale was between .75 - .91 (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995), and was .85 in the present study. In contrast to other scales that measure optimism, General Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale explicitly refers to personal agency, such as the belief that our actions are responsible for successful 9 outcomes. Thus, we expected the Autonomous Self and the Autonomous-Related Self Scale scores to be positively associated with the Perceived Self-Efficacy scores. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. This 10-item scale is the most widely used self-esteem scale for the measure of global self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). Items are rated on a 6- point scale, with higher scores indicating higher self-esteem. The alpha coefficient of the Turkish scale was found to be .71 (Çuhadaroğlu, 1985), and it was .84 in this validation study. Self-esteem is an assessment of how one perceives his/her self-worth. Similar to the generalized self-efficacy scale, this scale also suggests perceived agency, in addition to an assessment of how much one is proud of himself/herself in general. Hence we expected the Autonomous Self and the Autonomous-Related Self scale scores to be positively associated with the Self-Esteem scores. Family Affection and Family Control Scales: The Family Affection and Family Control scales (Kagitcibasi, 1970) measured the extent of parental affection and control experienced during childhood and adolescence. The Family Affection scale was composed of seven items. Sample items included “When I was a child, my father (mother) gave me a great deal of affection and acceptance” and “My family believed that a teen-ager should be allowed to decide most things for himself/herself” (α=.63). The Family Control scale was composed of five items. Sample items included “I was brought up under strict discipline” and “Sometimes, my parents punished me harshly for a small misdemeanor” (α=.66). Items were rated on 5-point rating scales with higher scores indicating more affection or control. The presence of warmth in a family promotes close relationships among the family members and contributes to the development of a relational self; whereas strict control in the family impedes the development of an autonomous self. Hence we expected a positive association of the Related Self and the Autonomous-Related Self scale scores with the Family 10 Affection scale scores, and a negative association of the Autonomous and Autonomous-Related Self scale scores with the Family Control scale scores. Quality of Childhood Environment Scale. A new 6-item scale was constructed to assess the respondents’ evaluation of the family context they grew up in and the current impact of this context on the respondents and their relationships. Items were rated on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating a positive evaluation of one’s childhood environment. Sample items included “We had a loving atmosphere in our family” and “There are matters from my family experience that negatively affect my ability to form close relationships” (reverse coded). The internal reliability was .88, and the dominant factor accounted for 63% of the variance in the present study. As in the Family Affection scale, higher scores on this scale also suggest a positive and nurturing family environment. Hence, we expected the Related Self and the AutonomousRelated Self scale scores to be positively associated with the Quality of Childhood Environment scores. Teen Timetable Scale: This is a 19-item scale that measured expectations for behavioral autonomy (Feldman, & Rosenthal, 1991). Participants were asked to choose the age that they first engaged in the behaviors mentioned in the items, such as “At what age were you able to choose your own hair style even if your parents disapproved?” and “At what age were you able to go out on dates?” The items were rated on a five-point scale: 1=before age 14; 2=14-15 years, 3=16-17 years, 4=18 and older and 5=never. 19 items are summed to calculate the overall timetable score. High scores indicated a delayed teen timetable. Alpha coefficient of the original scale ranged from .83 to .87, and was .84 in this study. The delay in earning behavioral autonomy in age-related tasks hampers the development of autonomous self. Hence we expected 11 the Autonomous Self and the Autonomous-Related Self scales scores to be negatively associated with a delayed teen timetable. Beyond the measures used for validation purposes, demographic measures were used. These included various indicators of family background and socio-economic status. Religiosity was also measured with an item asking for the respondents’ self perceived degree of religiousness from 1 to 7. Results and Discussion of Study 1 Figure 1 presents the interrelations among the autonomy, relatedness, and autonomousrelatedness scores in a three dimensional chart. This chart shows the mean autonomousrelatedness scores of participants who had Autonomous Self and Related Self scores in the indicated quintiles. As the autonomy and relatedness scores increase, so do the autonomousrelatedness scores, showing the construct validity of the latter. ============Insert Figure 1 about here ============ The Autonomous Self and Related Self scores are negatively correlated (r= -.53, p < .001). The magnitude of this correlation indicates that each scale can explain 28% of the variance in the other one, leaving the majority of the variance unexplained. Hence the two constructs are overlapping but not interchangeable, as suggested by Kagitcibasi (1996). Correlation of the Autonomous – Related Self Scale is r=. 08 (p > .05) with the Autonomous Self Scale and r= .43 with the Related Self Scale (p < .05). It appears that the Turkish young adults in the two samples responded to this scale more on the basis of their feelings of interpersonal distance than in terms of their feelings about autonomy. Thus relatedness weighs more heavily than autonomy in the Autonomous-Related Self Scale. Individuals can be categorized as having an Autonomous Related Self based on their scores on the 9-item A-R Self Scale, or by their relative scores on the 12 two other self scales. The next phase of Study 1 was devoted to the validation of the newly developed ARS scale in the Turkish population. The ARS scales were administered together with other conceptually related measures to establish their construct validity. Table 3 provides the means and the standard deviations of the scales used in the validation study. The two validation samples had almost identical mean autonomous self scores (51.6 and 51.4, for the university student sample and the non-student sample, respectively). However, the university student sample had significantly higher mean Related self scores and Autonomous-related self scores than the non-student sample (Related self: 69.5 and 61.1, respectively, F (1, 675) =41.4, p < .05; and Autonomous-related self: 81.3 and 67.0, respectively, F (1, 675=145.8, p < .05). The reason of relatively low levels of Related self and AutonomousRelated self scores in the non-student sample may be an indication of the transitional nature of this group. As described above, this group was urban, who had families with limited levels of education. They differed substantially from their parents in their level of education. The majority was economically active, and none were married at the age of almost 21. Because of their upwardly mobile aspirations, this sample might have rejected close ties to their families of origin. ----------------- Insert Table 3 about here ---------------------The validity of the ARS scales was established by analyzing their associations with the variables expected to be conceptually related described above. Participants were categorized as the highest 25%, middle 50% and the lowest 25% scorers for each characteristic that was thought to be associated with self construals and differences of mean ARS scale scores were examined across those three categories (see Table 4). This approach allowed the identification of non-linear 13 associations among related constructs, as opposed to the commonly used correlation analyses that allow the identification of linear associations only. -------------------Insert Table 4 about here ------------------------------As can be seen from Table 4, all predictions were confirmed for both validation samples, except for the association between Family Affection and the Related Self scale scores that were not significant though showed a trend in both samples. The analyses revealed some additional associations between Related Self scores and internal control, self-efficacy, and self-esteem in the non-student sample. Internal control, self-efficacy, and self-esteem were positively associated with Related Self scores in addition to the Autonomous Self scores. This result may be explained by the specific characteristics of this sample (i.e., as upwardly mobile working young adults). High values of these scales, which are typically associated with adaptive functioning in a competitive urban environment, appear to affect the participants’ likelihood of developing a Related Self in addition to contributing to the enhancement of an Autonomous Self. This would appear to be a disposition contrary to reactionary separateness. Study 2: Cross-Cultural Analyses of the Autonomous and Related Self Scales The analyses of cross-cultural data had four aims: (1) to establish the cross-cultural measurement invariance of the autonomy and relatedness scales; (2) to explore the associations between autonomy and relatedness constructs in the seven cultural groups; (3) to compare the levels and variability of autonomy and relatedness in seven different groups that were studied; and, (4) to explore whether there were groups of individuals who had similar patterns of autonomy and relatedness among the samples coming from seven different cultural groups. The Autonomous Self and Related Self Scales were used in Study 2; the combined AutonomousRelated Self Scale was not used. One reason for this was our intention to explore the variability 14 in the association between these two conceptually distinct dimensions of self; another reason was that the Autonomous-Related Self Scale was not administered in all of the cultural groups. In order to address the above aims, a variety of methods were adopted. The first aim required the estimation of models that would systematically test whether the factor structures of the items that represent autonomy and relatedness were similar across the 7 groups represented in the cross-cultural sample. These tests were performed using Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) models that tested the equivalence of the factor structures across different groups with progressively more restrictive equivalence restrictions (Wu, Li & Zumbo, 2007). First, configural invariance was tested. This test determined if, for all groups considered, the number and configuration of factors for each set of items was identical. Next, a model of invariance of factor loadings was tested, to determine if these loadings were invariant across the seven groups. Kline (1998) labels this criterion “partial measurement invariance.” A further requirement of invariance of intercepts of all items was not imposed because this test tends to be highly sensitive to the distribution of the responses to each item, which, in this case spans only a 5-point scale. In order to determine the goodness of fit, and to test nested models of measurement invariance, Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990) and Root Mean Square Error (RMSEA, Browne & Cudeck, 1993) were used. In order to obtain statistical criteria that were robust with large samples and models with large numbers of parameters, traditional likelihood ratio tests were not used. The criteria adopted here were: (1) CFI ≥ 0.90; (2) RMSEA ≤ 0.05; and (3) for nested models, ΔCFI ≥ -0.01 (Wu et al., 2007). Tables of goodness of fit statistics also report the conventional likelihood ratio chi-square values alongside the CFI and RMSEA. The second aim of the analyses, i.e., the investigation of the association between the autonomy and relatedness constructs in the seven cultural groups, could be done within the 15 framework of the structural equation models, where correlations between the latent autonomy and relatedness constructs could be estimated. These correlations, in general, are stronger than those that are estimated between raw additive scale scores because the correlations estimated in structural equation models are estimates of the association between latent constructs that, by definition, exclude measurement errors. The third aim, i.e, the comparisons of the levels of and variability in autonomy and relatedness across the seven cultural groups, required the calculation of scale scores that were demonstrated to have comparable factor structures and comparing these scale scores using standard ANOVA across countries. The scale scores were constructed using two alternative methods: additive scale scores based on observed item scores, and the latent factor scores based on CFA results. Further analyses using the latent factor scores yielded results that were very similar to those obtained based on additive scale scores. Only the latter analyses are presented here because they are easier to replicate and interpret than the latent factor scores. The final aim of the cross-cultural study, i.e., whether there were groups of individuals who had similar patterns of autonomy and relatedness across the seven different cultures, was addressed by using cluster analyses that identified groups of individuals that could be characterized with a given pattern of associations between their autonomy and relatedness dimensions. Ward’s method of clustering was used here because of its efficiency when cluster criteria are measured at interval level (Ward, 1963). The analyses were repeated using the within-cluster average linkage method, however the cluster results of the two methods were extremely similar. The results of the Ward’s method of clustering are presented here. Sample 16 Data were collected from 7 different cultural groups: university students from Belgium, Germany, Hong Kong, Turkey, United Kingdom, USA, and immigrant Turks in Europe (in Belgium and Germany). All participants were living in big cities or metropolitan areas at the time of the data collection. Sample characteristics are summarized in Table 5. The Belgian sample also included a group of last year high school students. . ----------------------- Insert Table 5 about here ------------------Results and discussion of Study 2 Exploratory factor analyses of the 9 autonomy and 9 relatedness items for the pooled international sample revealed that the dominant factors accounted for 29% of the joint variance of the nine autonomy items and 31% of the joint variance of the nine relatedness items. These groups of items yielded Cronbach’s reliability coefficients of 0.68 for the autonomy items and 0.70 for the relatedness items. Additive scale scores based on nine autonomy and nine relatedness items were correlated by -0.48 (p<.001). Confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to establish the invariance of the factor structure of the Autonomy and Relatedness scales across the seven groups. The least restrictive model, the model of configural invariance was supported neither for autonomy (χ2(189) = 508.6, p<0.01, CFI=0.80, RMSEA=0.09) nor for relatedness (χ2(189) = 645.6, p<0.01, CFI=0.79, RMSEA=0.10) constructs. Further exploratory factor analyses revealed a more nuanced factor structure for the two constructs than originally postulated. Both the autonomy construct and the relatedness construct were found to consist of two sub-dimensions yielding a total of four substantively meaningful constructs. A two factor configural invariance model for autonomy yielded an adequate fit for the seven distinct cultural groups (χ2(178) = 257.5, p<0.01, Relative χ2=1.4, CFI=0.94, RMSEA=0.05). A similar two factor configural invariance model for relatedness was also adequate (χ2(178) = 270.5, p<0.01, 17 Relative χ2=1.5, CFI=0.95, RMSEA=0.05). The two sub-dimensions of autonomy and relatedness constructs were labeled ‘behavioral’ versus ‘ideational autonomy’, and ‘related self’ versus ‘permeable self’ dimensions, respectively. Behavioral autonomy refers to actual autonomous behaviors, reflecting volitional agency. Ideational autonomy has to do with independent ideas. It may be influenced by individualistic norms. Related self emphasizes the importance of close relationships. Permeable self refers to a self that has rather diffuse boundaries which can be permeated by other close selves, that is, a self construal without interpersonal distance. The items of the subscales are indicated under Table 6. The analyses of the cross-cultural data proceeded for these four constructs (i.e., two constructs representing each of the original autonomy and the relatedness dimensions). The cross-cultural validity of these four constructs was established next using partial measurement invariance criterion (Kline, 1998). The nested tests of goodness of fit conducted on the four constructs are presented in Table 6. Based on these tests, additive scale scores were computed that quantified each construct. ----------------------- Insert Table 6 about here ------------------In order to investigate the pattern of correlations between the autonomy and relatedness dimensions, a multi-group CFA model that included all four constructs and their correlations were estimated for all seven cultures. The pattern of correlations between the latent constructs of behavioral autonomy, ideational autonomy, related self, and permeable self revealed positive associations between the two dimensions of autonomy and the two dimensions of relatedness (see Table 7). The correlations across the overall autonomy and relatedness dimensions were negative. The correlations between behavioral and ideational autonomy constructs were high in Belgium and Hong Kong (about 0.8) and lower elsewhere (about 0.6). The correlations between 18 related and permeable self constructs were higher in Belgium and Germany (about 0.8) than other cultural groups (0.4 in Hong Kong and about 0.6 elsewhere). Behavioral autonomy was weakly correlated with permeable self everywhere (correlations ranging between 0 in the US, UK, and Germany, to 0.3 in Belgium and in immigrant Turks in Europe). Far from being the opposite poles of a single self construal, these two dimensions were nearly orthogonal in many cultural groups. Ideational autonomy and related self constructs were strongly negatively correlated in all cultural groups though showing a range of variation (between -0.7 and -1.0). In all cases ideational autonomy had more negative associations with related and permeable self than behavioral autonomy had, except in only one case (with related self among immigrant Turks in Europe). Thus, the pattern of correlations revealed a dimension of autonomy (ideational) that is aligned with interpersonal distance, and another dimension of autonomy (behavioral) that is relatively independent of interpersonal distance. ----------------------- Insert Table 7 about here ------------------All four construct means significantly differed across the seven groups (all F-tests yielded p<.01). The patterns of differences between each pair of groups were tested using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons. The results were as follows: the students from Turkey, UK, and USA had high scores in behavioral autonomy; pairwise tests yielded no pairs of groups that were significantly different in ideational autonomy; immigrant Turks as a group had low levels of related self; and immigrant Turks and students in Hong Kong had low levels of permeable self. All of the analyses presented thus far addressed cross-cultural differences assuming within-culture homogeneity in the seven groups that constituted the cross-cultural sample. However, there was evidence of substantial variability in self construals in each one of 19 the seven cultural groups. The next set of analyses took a different perspective and focused on the within-culture variability and across-culture similarities of individuals’ self construals. When focusing on within culture variability, a question to explore is whether there exist some combinations of autonomy and relatedness dimensions that tend to be replicated across cultures albeit with varying frequencies. An example could be an “ideal type” of individuals who are high in both dimensions of autonomy and low in both dimensions of relatedness, i.e., an ideal type that is often defined as the “individualist” or “Western” self. It is the autonomous-separate’ self in Kagitcibasi’s model (2005, 2007). The cross cultural comparison of mean autonomy and relatedness dimensions clearly indicated that this ideal type was not the rule in any one of the seven cultural groups. However, it could occur with greater frequency in some than in others. Cluster analysis is used to identify groups of individuals configured such that within group similarities in autonomy and relatedness are high and across group similarities are low. The cluster analysis of four scale scores representing the four dimensions of the self construals from all seven cultural groups yielded four clusters, interpreted as “ideal types” of self. The characteristic patterns of autonomy and relatedness of these four ideal types can be examined using the mean scores of the four scales in these four ideal types (Table 8). All of the mean scale scores significantly differed across the four ideal types (all F statistics yielded a p<.01). ----------------------- Insert Table 8 and Figure 2 about here ------------------The self construals that were depicted by the profiles of the four ideal types could be easily grasped graphically (Figure 2). All four ideal types corresponded to the four self constructs proposed by Kagitcibasi (2005, 2007) in her theory of the self. Ideal Type I was characterized with a high level of behavioral autonomy, a moderate level of ideational autonomy and high levels of related self and permeable self. This Ideal Type is what Kagitcibasi described 20 as the Autonomous-Related self. Ideal Type II differed from the Ideal Type I in behavioral autonomy that was significantly lower than Ideal Type I, moderate levels of ideational autonomy and related self, and a low-moderate level of permeable self. Ideal Type II can be described as the Heteronomous Separate self. However, the low level of (behavioral) autonomy is a more distinguishing characteristic of this type of self than its separateness. The Heteronomousseparate self differed significantly from the Autonomous Related self in behavioral autonomy but not in ideational autonomy based on post hoc tests. This divergence in the differences in behavioral and ideational autonomy between the ideal types further validates the existence of two distinct dimensions of autonomy. Ideal Type III was characterized by low levels of behavioral and ideational autonomy and high levels of related and permeable self. This Ideal Type is what Kagitcibasi (2005) called Heteronomous-Related self. Ideal Type IV was characterized by high levels of behavioral and ideational autonomy and low levels of related and permeable self. This Ideal Type can be described as the Autonomous-Separate self (Kagitcibasi, 2005). The levels of behavioral autonomy of the Autonomous Related self and the Autonomous Separate self types were not significantly different from each other, based on post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction. The presence of the Autonomous Related self (the Ideal Type I) and the Heteronomous Separate self (the Ideal Type II) does not fit with the prevailing conceptualizations of independent self (highly autonomous and not related) versus interdependent self (highly related and not autonomous), as discussed earlier. Yet , in a sample of 1,300 individuals representing seven distinct cultural groups, about one half of the participants could be characterized by these two ideal types that defy the uni-dimensional view of self on a single axis of independence/ interdependence. 21 The distribution of the four ideal types of self in the seven cultural groups considered in the current study revealed the structure of distinct self construals that resulted in the observed differences in average levels of the four scale scores representing the dimensions of autonomy and relatedness. Table 9 provides the percentage distributions of the four ideal types of self in each cultural group. There is a significant difference in the distributions of the ideal types across the seven samples (χ2(128)=123.4, p<.01). ----------------------- Insert Table 9 about here ------------------Further analyses of differences in the four autonomy and relatedness scores were carried out to investigate whether the mean levels of these scores differed within a given ideal type across different cultures. In other words, it was hypothesized that the ideal types could be nuanced, somewhat, in different cultures. Univariate ANOVAs were conducted of the four scale scores, investigating the main effects of cultural group and the ideal type, and the interaction effect of these. The interaction effects for behavioral autonomy and ideational autonomy were significant (F(18, 1272)=4.6, p<.01 and F(18, 1272)=1.6, p<.05, respectively), but they were not significant for related and permeable self scores (F(18, 1272)=0.9, p>.10 and F(18, 1272)=1.4, p<.10, respectively). Thus, behavioral and ideational autonomy levels of the same ideal type in different cultural groups slightly varied, revealing that ideal types may have some differences across cultures. General Discussion and Conclusions Notwithstanding the impressive literature on culture and self of the last two decades, there is a continuing need for more clear definition and operationalization of self construals across cultures. This is because the seemingly attractive dichotomies of independentinterdependent self construals tend to confound some important underlying dimensions that are 22 conceptually distinct. In particular, the constructs of autonomy and relatedness need to be better conceptualized and operationalized in different cultures for a better understanding of the complex psychological phenomena involved. This study, therefore, aimed to bring some clarification to the conceptual and operational definitions of autonomy and relatedness, informed by Kagitcibasi’s (2005, 2007) theoretical perspectives and using comparative cross-cultural data. The findings provided empirical evidence for the distinctness and compatibility of autonomy and relatedness, also substantiating the validity of the proposed scales of autonomous, related, and autonomous-related self scales. The use of two quite different validation samples offered insights into the developmental aspects of these self construals. The confirmatory factor analyses on the cross-cultural comparative data pointed to further refinement, yielding two sub-dimensions each of autonomy and relatedness. The cross-cultural validity of these four constructs was established and provided empirical evidence for Kagitcibasi’s (2005, 2007) theory of self. The present study proposed scales of autonomy and relatedness that can be used to characterize individuals across different cultural backgrounds. The findings demonstrated that the ‘independent’ and ‘interdependent’ self construals, combining autonomy and relatedness (or agency and interpersonal distance dimensions) are indeed much too broad to do justice to the inherent complexity of the phenomena in question, both within a society and especially across societies with different cultures. Given the distinctness of the agency and interpersonal distance dimensions, the Autonomous and Related Self construals are conceptually defined as two distinct constructs. These constructs are then operationally defined together with an AutonomousRelated self construal. Proposed scales are validated in the first study. It was shown that the Autonomous and Related self scales were distinct, though negatively correlated. This is 23 important given the widespread use of independent-interdependent self scales that confound these constructs. Thus, it is possible and desirable to measure autonomy without tapping relatedness (separateness), and to measure relatedness without tapping autonomy (heteronomy). A piece of empirical evidence that agency and interpersonal distance can vary independently is the existence of the autonomous-related self which increases as the Autonomy and Relatedness scores increase (Figure 1). This type of self can be more prevalent in some cultural contexts than others. An Autonomous-Related Self Scale is proposed here to measure the degree to which agency and relatedness coexist. Autonomous-Related Self scores had a higher correlation with Related than with Autonomous Self Scale scores, showing that in the validation samples from Turkey the autonomous-related self is engendered more by relatedness than by autonomy. This is likely because of the greater variability in relatedness than in autonomy among young adult participants. The observed associations of the Autonomous, Related, and Autonomous-Related self construals with family background variables provided insights into the possible developmental processes that may contribute to the generation of these self construals. Notwithstanding their traditional background, the upwardly mobile young adults from low SES backgrounds had low levels of relatedness but high levels of autonomy. In this case, the high level of autonomy may have been developed at the cost of their relatedness, taking the form of ‘reactionary separateness’ in response to restrictive parenting. A similar pattern was found among migrant Turkish youth in Europe (Gungor, Phalet & Kagitcibasi, 2008; Kagitcibasi & Otyakmaz, 2006). The higher levels of relatedness and autonomous-relatedness among college students point to the likely development of these self construals in nurturant environments. 24 The negative effects of strict family control (also operationalized in terms of teen timetables) on autonomy and on relatedness and the positive effect of quality of childhood environment and family affection (trend) on the development of relatedness are theoretically important (Kagitcibasi, 2007). The positive associations of self efficacy, self esteem and internal control with relatedness in the low SES but upwardly mobile sample is notable. These findings, together with the finding of lower relatedness and autonomous-relatedness in this sample are suggestive of the conditions conducive to the development of relatedness and autonomousrelatedness. It appears that the balanced synthesis of autonomy and relatedness (the Autonomous-Related self) is more likely to emerge in supportive home contexts. Where there is high level of family control, it may trigger reactive separation from parents, further aggravated by a low level of education, low socio-economic standing, and youth’s earlier labor force participation (see also Georgas, Berry, van de Vijver, Kagitcibasi & Poortinga, 2006; Gungor et al., 2008; Kagitcibasi & Otyakmaz, 2006). The cross cultural validation of the constructs of autonomy and relatedness (Study 2) revealed two subdimensions of autonomy (behavioral vs. ideational) and two subdimensions of relatedness (related vs. permeable self), pointing to further complexity. Behavioral autonomy refers to behaviors reflecting volitional agency. Ideational autonomy reflects an attitude of independence that has to do with the independent formulation and maintenance of opinions probably influenced by individualistic norms. The related self emphasizes the importance of close relationships, whereas the permeable self refers to diffuse boundaries of self and a lack of personal distance. This distinction between relatedness and boundary diffusion has been an important one in family research (Minuchin, 1974). For example, Manzi and colleagues (Manzi, 25 et al, 2006) use the concepts of family cohesion and enmeshment to refer to this distinction. At the level of the self, also, the two emergent configurations appear conceptually different. These sub-dimensions have culturally varying associations with each other, providing insights into the substantive meanings of autonomy and relatedness. Ideational autonomy emerged as the opposite of related self, but behavioral autonomy was nearly orthogonal to the permeable self. Ideational autonomy dimension appears to be responsible for the negative correlation between autonomy and relatedness in nearly all cultures in the study. The crosscultural perspective on autonomy and relatedness highlights a substantive difference between behavioral and ideational autonomy. In line with the common assumptions in the literature, ideational autonomy is conflicting with relatedness, because the former concerns values. However, behavioral autonomy is not antithetical to relatedness, as proposed by Kagitcibasi (2005, 2007) and also substantiated in previous research (Beyers, et al., 2003; Huiberts, et.al, 2006; Meeus, Oosterwegel and Vollebergh, 2002). Ideational autonomy appears to be akin to what Kagitcibasi (1996) called ‘normative individualism.’ It has to do with the value or ideology of independence, but may not manifest itself in interpersonal behavior. On the other hand, behavioral autonomy is akin to Kagitcibasi’s (1997) ‘relational individualism’ which is reflected in self-other relations. This distinction is in line with the Oyserman, et al. (2002) metaanalysis finding of a high level of relationality among Americans in contrast to the highest level of (normative) individualism in the U.S. among all the countries in Hofstede’s classic work (1980). In addition to the pursuit of cross cultural similarity in conceptualization and measurement of self construals, this study also pursued the sources of within culture variability in self construals. One way of describing within culture variability in self construals would be the examination of variances and covariances of the two autonomy and two relatedness measures 26 within each culture. While informative, this approach would have the inherent assumption that there are an infinite number of combinations of these four aspects of the self construal. Alternatively, the within culture variability may be modeled as the variability that emerges because there exists a mixture of a finite number of self prototypes within a culture where the self prototypes are defined by given relative levels of autonomy and relatedness. This approach would require the identification of a limited number of self prototypes that exist in all cultures, albeit in varying proportions. The pursuit of the finite number of self prototypes revealed four combinations of autonomy and relatedness dimensions that were present in varying proportions in all cultures. These four ideal types of self coincided with Kagitcibasi’s (2005, 2007) four self types and were labeled, accordingly, as Autonomous-Related, Heteronymous-Separate, Heteronymous-Related, and Autonomous-Separate selves. The four subdimensions of autonomy and relatedness help clarify the conceptual differences between the ideal types. Heteronomous-Separate self differs from the Autonomous-Related self in behavioral autonomy but not in ideational autonomy. The low level of behavioral autonomy is a more distinguishing characteristic of the HeteronomousSeparate self than its separateness. The levels of behavioral autonomy of the AutonomousRelated self and the Autonomous-Separate self types are also not different. Of the four ideal types, the Heteronomous-Related and the Autonomous-separate selves are most clearly defined with regard to contrasting high or low levels of both autonomy and relatedness scores, followed by the Autonomous-Related ideal type, which demonstrates the compatibility of behavioral autonomy with relatedness. The Heteronomous-Separate self is the least well defined ideal type, nevertheless, it does manifest low levels of both behavioral autonomy and permeable self(Figure 27 2). Thus, cluster analyses produced configurations that fit with the conceptual distinctions made by Kagitcibasi earlier (2005; 2007). The distribution of the ideal types across the cultural groups is informative. As expected, cultures that are known to be similar, have distributions of self construals that are similar. For example, Germany and Belgium are characterized by a low proportion of autonomous related self, whereas the Anglo American cultures are characterized by almost one-fifth of their samples with this Ideal Type. More than one half of the sample from Hong Kong is characterized with the Heteronomous-Separate self, an individual who keeps highly personal matters to himself/herself and at the same time is influenced by the close others in his/her decisions, expressed by low behavioral autonomy and low permeable self. While this picture does not quite fit the general collectivistic stereotype, it is in line with research findings of low self disclosure in Eastern cultures (Rime, Corsini & Herbette, 2002). Ideational autonomy being especially low among the heteronomous-separate respondents in Hong Kong appears to reflect collectivistic values and norms there. The profiles of the immigrant Turks in Europe differ from those in Turkey, due, probably, to the influence of the host culture. The immigrants’ low level of related and permeable self may be a reaction to the relatively restrictive parenting they experience in a host culture of individualism and relatively lenient parenting (Gungor et al., 2008; Kagitcibasi & Otyakmaz, 2006). Surprisingly, the proportions of Autonomous-Separate individuals in Belgium and Germany were lower than in any other group. Possibly as a reaction to excessive individualism and with the rise of post modern values, the changes in values toward a higher level of relatedness in Western Europe have been noted in other studies, also (e.g. Georgas et al., 2006; Inglehart, 2003). 28 The measures of autonomy and relatedness presented here are promising for use in different socio-cultural contexts. However, the number of items in the scales is small, and following the confirmatory factor analyses, the items of the subscales assessing sub-dimensions were further reduced. In the future the measures need to be strengthened to target the dimensions of autonomy and relatedness identified here. Many cultural groups were not represented in the present study. This is a limitation, and there is a need for further studies in more cultures and with more varied samples. Despite these limitations, conceptually meaningful self construal patterns were empirically established in this study which future studies can explore further. In conclusion, research presented here provides us with new conceptualizations and measures of autonomy and relatedness that show cross cultural validity. This research reconfirms the need to disaggregate the construals of independent and interdependent self and provides extensive empirical evidence to support a new perspective on the theory, conceptualization and measurement of the self construals. The complexity of culture-self dynamics defies simple depictions and requires more nuanced conceptualization and operationalization. This study has aimed to do this task. The results substantially improve our understanding of self in terms of the interaction between autonomy and relatedness. The distinction of four types of self proposed by Kagitcibasi (2005, 2007) is now supported with empirical evidence. 29 REFERENCES Bentler, P. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246. Beyers, W., Goossens, L., Vansant, I. & Moors, E. (2003). A structural model of autonomy in the middle and late adolescence: Connectedness, separation, detachment, and agency. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 351-365. Browne, M. WW., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen, K. A., & Long, J. S. (Eds.) Testing Structural Equation Models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 136-162. 30 Blos, P. (1979). The adolescent passage. New York: International Universities Press. Celenk, O. (2007). Adult attachment styles, autonomous-relational self and gender roles as mediators between culture and relationship satisfaction for British and Turkish people. Master’s Thesis. Brunel University. Chirkov, V., Kim, Y., Ryan, R., Kaplan, U. (2003). Differentiating autonomy from individualism and independence: A self-determination theory perspective on internalization of cultural orientations and well being, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 97-110. Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., Morris, M. L. (2000). The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal an Relationships. , Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791-808. Cuhadaroglu, F. (1985). Ergenlerde benlik saygisi [Self-esteem in the adolescence]. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Hacettepe University, Ankara. Feldman, S. S., & Rosenthal, D. A. (1991). Age expectations of behavioral autonomy in Hong Kong, Australian, and American youths: The influence of family variables and adolescents values. International Journal of Psychology, 26, 1-23. Georgas, J., Berry, J.W., Vijver van de, F., Kagitcibasi, C., Poortinga, Y.H. (Ed.) (2006) Families across cultures: A 30 Nation psychological study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Greenfield, P.M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461-90. Gudykunst, W.B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., & Nishida, T. (1996). The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on communication styles accross cultures. Human Communication Research, 22, 510-543. Gungor, D., Phalet, K., & Kagitcibasi, C. (2008). Autonomy and Relatedness in Turkish, Belgian, and Turkish-Belgian Adolescents: Extending the Model of Family Change to the Context of 31 Acculturation. Paper presented at the invited symposium “Autonomous-Related Self and Family Change in Cultural Context“, 20th Biennial ISSBD Meeting, 13-17 July, Wurzburg, Germany. Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T. L., Bhagwat, A. A. (2004) Factor Sturucture of the Self-Construal Scale Revisited: Implications for the Multidimensionality of Self-Construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 327-345. Hoffman, J. A. (1984). Psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 170-178. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences. Beverly Hills, California: SAGE. Hui, C.H. & Yee, C. (1994). The shortened individualism-collectivism scale: Its relationship to demographic and work related variables. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 409-424. Huiberts, A., Oosterwegel, A., Vandervalk, I., Vollebergh, W., & Meeus, W. (2006). Connectedness with parents and behavioural autonomy among Dutch and Moroccan adolescents. Ethnic & Racial Studies, 29(2), 315-330. Inglehart, R. (2003) Human values and social change. Findings from the values surveys (International Studies in Sociology and Social Anthropology). Leiden, Netherlands: Brill. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349-366 Kagitcibasi, C. (1970). Social norms and authoritarianism: A Turkish-American comparison. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 16, 444-451. Kagitcibasi, C. (1990). Family and socialization in cross-cultural perspective: A model of change. In J. Berman (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives: Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1989 (pp. 135200), 37, Nebraska University Press. 32 Kagitcibasi, C. (1996). The autonomous-relational self: A new synthesis. European Psychologist, 1, 180-186. Kagitcibasi, C. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall & C. Kagitcibasi (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology (2nd ed.,vol 3, pp. 1-50). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and Relatedness in Cultural Context. Implications for Self and Family. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 403-422. Sage. Kagitcibasi, C. (2007). Family, Self and Human Development Across Cultures: Theory and Applications. (Revised Second Edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kagitcibasi, C. & Otyakmaz, B.O. (2006). “Autonomy and relatedness in immigration societies- A comparison of German and Turkish-German university students”, paper presented at symposium “Family Issues” at IACCP Congress, 11-15 July, Spetses, Greece: Kashima, E. S., & Hardie, E. A. (2000). The development and validation of the relational, individual and collective self-aspects (RIC) scale. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 19-48. Killen, M. (1997). Culture, self, and development: Are cultural templates useful or stereotypic? Developmental Review, 17, 239-249. Kim, Y., Butzel, J. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1998). ‘Interdependence and well-being: A function of culture and relatedness needs’. Paper presented at The International Society for the Study of Personal Relationships, Saratoga Spring, NY. Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural and basic psychological process-toward a system view of culture: Comment on Oyserman et al. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 189-196. 33 Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Uchida Y., (2007) Self As Cultural Mode of Being. Handbook of Cultural Psychology, 6,136-174, Guilford Press. Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Kurokawa, M., Tummala, P., & Kato, K. (1991). Self-other similarity judgments depend on culture. University of Oregon, Institute of Cognitive Decision Sciences, Technical Report, No. 91-17. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press, 1998. xiv, 354 pp. Kroger, J. (1998). Adolescence as a second separation-individuation process: Critical review of an object relations approach. In E.E.A. Skoe & A.L.von der Lippe (Eds.), Personality development in adolescence: A cross-national and life span perspective. Adolescence and society. (pp. 172-192). New York: Routledge. Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations for immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development, 46, 15-36. Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the human infant. New York: Basic Books). Manzi, C., Vignoles, V.L., Regalia, C. & Scabini, E. (2006). Cohesion and enmeshment revisited: Differentiation, identity, and well-being in two European cultures. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 68, 673-689. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In V.M.Berman & J.J.Berman (Eds.) Nebraska symposium on motivation: Crosscultural differences in perspectives on the self (Vol. 49, pp. 1-58). Nebraska: Nebraska University Press. 34 Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Markus and Kitayama’s theory of independent and interdependent self-construal. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 289310. Matsumoto, D., Weissman, M.D., Preston, K., Brown, B.R., & Kupperbusch, C. (1997). Contextspecific measurement of individualism-collectivism on the individual level: the individualismcollectivism interpersonal assessment inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 743767. Meeus, W., Oosterweegel, A., & Vollebergh, W. (2002). Parental and peer attachment and identity development in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 93-106. Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Oishi, S. (2000). Goals as cornerstones of subjective well-being: Linking individuals and cultures. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Cross-cultural psychology of subjective well-being (pp.87-112). Boston: MIT Press. Oyserman, D., Kemmelmeier, M., Coon, H. (2002a). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of Theoretical Assumptions and Meta-Analyses, Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72. Raeff, C. (2004). Within-culture complexities: multifaceted and interrelated autonomy and connectedness characteristics in late adolescent selves. In M.F. Mascolo & J. Li (Eds.), Culture and Developing Selves: Beyond Dichotomization, 104, 61-79. Rime, B., Corsini, S. & Herbette, G. (2002). Emotion, verbal expression, and the social sharing of emotion. In S.R.Fussell (Ed.), The verbal communication of emotions: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 185-208). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rosenberg, M., (1965). Society and Adolescent self-image. New Jersey: Princeton University Press. 35 Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000a). The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71, 1121-1142. Rothbaum, F., & Trommsdorff, G. (2007). Do roots and wings complement or oppose one another? The socialization of relatedness and autonomy in cultural context. In J. Grusec and P. Hastings (Eds.), The Handbook of Socialization. New York: Guilford Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, Whole No.609, 1-28. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78. Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). Autonomy, relatedness, and the Self: Their relation to development and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, 618-655. NY: Wiley. Schimmack , U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: A valid and important dimension of cultural differences between nations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(1), 17-31. Schwarzer, R. & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In Weinman J., Wright S. and Johnston M., Measures in health psychology: A user's portfolio. Causal and control beliefs, pp. 35-37. Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON. Singelis, T.M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580-591. Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. B. (1986). The vicissitudes of autonomy in early adolescence. Child Development, 57, 841-851. 36 Takano, Y., & Osaka, E. (1999). An unsupported common view: Comparing Japan and the U.S. on individualism/collectivism. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 311-341. Triandis, H.C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press. Triandis, H.C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M.J., Asai, M., & Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 323-338. Uleman, J.S., Rhee, E., Bardoliwalla, N., Semin, G., Toyama, M. (2000) The relational self: Closeness to ingroups depends on who they are, culture and the type of closeness. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 1-17 Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchial grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58, 236. Wu, A. D., Zhen, L. & Zumbo, B. D. (2007). Decoding the Meaning of Factorial Invariance and Updating the Practice of Multi-group Confirmatory Factor Analysis: A Demonstration With TIMSS Data. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation, 12(3), 1-26. 37 Table 1: The Factor Structures and Reliabilities of the Self - Scales Factor loadings Autonomous Self Scale 1. People who are close to me have little influence on my decisions. .56 2. I do not like a person to interfere with my life even if he/she is very close to me. .55 3. I feel independent of the people who are close to me. .54 4. I lead my life according to the opinions of people to whom I feel close. (R) .65 5. The opinions of those who are close to me influence me on personal issues. (R) .59 6. While making decisions, I consult with those who are close to me. (R) .58 7. On personal issues, I accept the decisions of people to whom I feel very close. (R) .57 8. I usually try to conform to the wishes of those to whom I feel very close. (R) .68 9. I can easily change my decisions according to the wishes of those who are close to me. (R) .63 % Variance explained 36 Cronbach alpha .77 Related Self Scale 1. I need the support of persons to whom I feel very close. .74 2. I prefer to keep a certain distance in my close relationships. (R) .57 3. Generally, I keep personal issues to myself. (R) .51 4. The people who are close to me strongly influence my personality. .58 5. I think often of those to whom I feel very close. .62 6. I do not worry about what people think of me even if they are close to me. (R) .62 7. Those who are close to me are my top priority. ..46 8. My relationships to those who are close to me make me feel peaceful and secure. .67 38 9. I do not share personal matters with anyone, even if very close to me. (R) .51 % Variance explained 35 Cronbach alpha .75 Autonomous-Related Self Scale 1. It is important to have both close relationships and also to be autonomous .55 2. Even if the suggestions of those who are close are considered, the last decision should .70 be one’s own 3. A person who has very close relationships cannot make his/her own decisions (R) .57 4. A person should be able to oppose the ideas of those who are close .63 5. Giving importance to the opinions of those who are close to me means ignoring my .70 own opinions (R) 6. Being very close to someone prevents being independent (R) .59 7. A person can feel both independent and connected to those who are close to him/her .66 8. In order to be autonomous, one should not form close relationships (R) .60 9. A person may be attached to those who are close, and at the same time, expect respect .69 for any differences of opinion % Variance explained 40 Cronbach alpha .81 Note: (R) reversed item 39 Table 2: The demographic characteristics of the validation samples Validation sample 1 Validation sample 2 University students Less than university education Urban family background 92.3 % 90% Grew up in a nuclear family 81.1 % 57.9 % Both parents were at home while growing up 95.3 % 77.5 % Number of siblings 2.6 3.5 Mother’s level of education 9.7 years 6.4 years Mother worked while growing up 33.8 % 20.5 % Father’s level of education 11.4 years 7.16 years Father worked while growing up 97.3% 90.5 % Level of religiosity (1-7) 3.8 4.7 40 Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations of the Scales (in parenthesis) in the validation samples Sample 1 1 Sample 2 Related Self 69.46 (15.07) 61.09 (16.23) Autonomous Self2 51.57 (15.60) 51.37 (15.42) Autonomous-Related Self 81.34 (13.43) 66.97 (15.68) Internal Control Scale 84.02 (14.01) 73.86 (18.68) The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale3 68.34 (14.89) NA General Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale 76.80 (13.87) 80.50 (15.68) Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale 75.31 (14.42) 66.65 (15.60) Family Control Scale 29.56 (19.58) 45.10 (22.39) Family Affection Scale 69.20 (16.41) 66.26 (16.84) Quality of Childhood Environment 76.67 (23.59) 70.36 (24.09) Teen Timetable Scale 50.69 (15.31) 64.34 (17.33) Notes: 1. All scale scores are converted to 0-100 scale for ease of interpretation. 2. All scale means were significantly different between the two samples, except for the Autonomous Self score. 3. The RISC scale was not available for Sample 2. 41 Table 4: Mean Autonomous Self, Related Self and Autonomous –Related Self Scale scores for groups defined as having low, medium or high values in characteristics predicted to be associated with these self construals (Predicted Relationships). Characteristics Validation sample 1 Validation sample 2 Autonomous Related AR Autonomous Related AR Self Self Self Self Self Self Internal Control Low 47.54 a 75.61 a 45.46a 58.17 a Medium 49.40 a 82.54 b 51.40a 69.07 b High 58.31 b 84.98 b 58.95b 73.78 b Relational-Interdependent Self Constural Low 58.06a 77.98 a Medium 70.07b 80.66 a High 80.89c 86.00 b Self-Efficacy 48.08a 78.58 a 53.12 60.36 a 51.30a,b 81.80 a,b 49.74 69.12 b High 55.31b 83.40 b 53.84 71.70 b Low 48.01a 75.84 a 49.43a 60.19 a Medium 52.67b 81.78 b 52.62a 71.24 b Low Medium Self-Esteem 42 High 54.98b 85.76 b 61.04b 77.22 b Family Affection Low 66.87 79.36 a 57.02 62.03 a Medium 70.09 81.09 a,b 62.26 68.35 a,b High 71.27 84.22 b 64.06 70.18 b Family Control Low Medium High Quality of Childhood Environment 54.51a 87.07 a 57.43a 71.12 a 51.56a,b 81.09 b 50.52a,b 68.19 a 48.22b 76.42 c 47.18b 60.31 b F 3.477 3.149 10.813 11.738 Low 65.89a 78.93 a 54.98a 61.01 a Medium 69.02a,b 81.05 a,b 59.90a 65.16 a High 71.37b 83.38 b 69.95b 75.75 b Teen Timetable Low 58.35a 84.33 a 57.95a 67.31 Medium 50.48b 82.12 a 51.01a,b 68.47 High 47.78b 76.26 b 48.69b 66.37 Note: mean scores with different subscripts within each validation sample indicate significant differences at p < .05 43 Table 5: Student Samples of the Cross-Cultural Study N Gender % Age F M Mean (SD) Range Belgium 131 45 55 18.07 (9.54) 17-21 Germany 94 72 28 22.26 (1.74) 18-25 Hong Kong 128 57 43 20.06 (1.13) 18-23 Turkey 468 62.4 37.6 20.43 (1.62) 17-25 218 60 40 19.27 (1.81) 17-25 UK 88 73.9 26.1 19.75 (1.53) 18-25 USA 173 75.3 24.7 19.77 (1.18) 18-23 Total 1300 62.8 37.2 19.96 (1.78) 17-25 Immigrant Turks in Europe 44 Table 6. The goodness of fit of the nested models testing the measurement invariance of the four constructs of interest. Invariance of the Factor Structure Invariance of the Factor Loadings Likeli hood Construct Ratio Likelihood Df CFI 12.3 14 1.00 44.2 33 RMSEA Diff. Ratio Df CFI RMSEA CFI .00 31.6 29 1.00 .02 .00 0.98 .04 71.3 54 0.97 .04 .01 45.9 34 0.99 .04 76.9 57 0.98 .04 .01 21.4 14 0.99 .05 41.2 30 0.98 .05 .01 Behavioral Autonomy (i) Ideational Autonomy (ii) Related Self (iii) Permeable Self (iv) Note: CFI: Comparative Fit Index; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error Item numbers (from Table 1) of the Constructs: (i): Autonomous self4, 7, 8, 9; (ii): Autonomous self1, 2, 3, 5, 6; (iii): Related self 1, 4, 5, 7, 8; (iv): Related self 2, 3, 6, 9 45 Table 7. Correlations between the latent constructs of behavioral autonomy, ideational autonomy, related self, and permeable self. Ideational Related Permeable Autonomy Self Self .87 -.69 -.34 -.83 -.83 Belgium Behavioral Autonomy Ideational Autonomy Related Self .79 Germany Behavioral Autonomy .64 Ideational Autonomy -.40 -.06 -.91 -.78 Related Self .78 Hong Kong Behavioral Autonomy .83 Ideational Autonomy -.50 -.13 -1.00 -.76 Related Self .40 Turkey Behavioral Autonomy .62 Ideational Autonomy -.43 -.14 -.79 -.62 Related Self .57 Immigrant Turks in Europe Behavioral Autonomy .63 -.76 -.31 46 Ideational Autonomy -.67 Related Self -.96 .57 UK Behavioral Autonomy .55 Ideational Autonomy -.25 -.03 -.97 -.79 Related Self .62 USA Behavioral Autonomy Ideational Autonomy Related Self .69 -.34 .05 -.90 -.60 .62 47 Table 8. The means and standard deviations of the autonomy and relatedness scores of the four “ideal types” of self: the results of the cluster analysis. Behavioral Ideational Related Permeable Autonomy Autonomy Self Self Ideal Type I 3.86 2.63 4.14 3.90 Autonomous Related Self (0.43) (0.54) (0.37) (0.47) Ideal Type II 2.74 2.60 3.95 3.26 Heteronomous Separate Self (0.47) (0.46) (0.44) (0.48) Ideal Type III 2.80 1.96 4.46 4.28 Heteronomous Related Self (0.55) (0.41) (0.35) (0.43) Ideal Type IV 3.78 3.15 3.38 2.73 Autonomous Separate Self (0.61) (0.60) (0.65) (0.66) N 240 437 265 358 48 Table 9. Distribution of the four Ideal Types of self in seven cultural groups Ideal Type I Ideal Type II Ideal Type III Ideal Type IV Autonomous Heteronomous Heteronomous Autonomous Related Self Separate Self Related Self Separate Self Belgium 8.4% 44.3% 27.5% 19.8% Germany 10.6% 36.2% 38.3% 14.9% Hong Kong 10.2% 53.1% 10.9% 25.8% Turkey 27.6% 23.3% 18.8% 30.3% 11.9% 42.7% 14.2% 31.2% Kingdom 19.3% 28.4% 20.5% 31.8% USA 19.7% 28.9% 24.3% 27.2% Total 18.5% 33.6% 20.4% 27.5% Immigrant Turks in Europe United 49 Figure 1. The mean Autonomous-Related Self scores for individuals grouped by quintile scores in Autonomous Self and Related Self scales in validation sample 1 ………… 50 Figure 2. Mean standard autonomy and relatedness scores (z-scores) of four Ideal Types of self 51