Workplace aggression has been defined as the purposeful infliction

advertisement
From Driver Stress to Workplace Aggression
Dwight A. Hennessy
Department of Psychology
Buffalo State College
Paper Presented at the 111th Annual American Psychological Association Convention
August 7-10, 2003
Toronto Ontario Canada
From Driver Stress to Workplace Aggression
Dwight A. Hennessy, Ph.D.
Buffalo State College
Paper Presented at the 111th Annual American Psychological Association Convention
Following their regular commute to work, part time employment completed a questionnaire battery
containing a state driver stress questionnaire, trait aggressiveness, demographic and vehicle
information. Then, following completion of work, they completed a state version of the Workplace
Aggression Scale (Neuman & Baron, 1998). Expressed hostility and obstructionism were both
predicted by the interaction of gender X state driver stress. As state driver stress increased, the
reported frequency of both forms of aggression increased (independently) among male employees
only. In contrast, overt aggression was predicted by the interaction of gender X trait physical
aggressiveness. Specifically, overt aggression was greatest among males that were higher in physical
aggressiveness as a general trait characteristic. The present study highlights the interactive nature of
the traffic environment, in that negative experiences in the traffic environment, for some individuals,
may “spillover” and influence non-driving interactions.
Workplace aggression has been defined as the purposeful infliction of physical or psychological harm to
current or previous co-workers, or to organizations in which an individual is currently employed or were previously
employed (Baron & Neuman, 1996). While workplace aggression can take many forms, Neuman and Baron (1998)
have proposed a three factor structure which distinguishes between expressed hostility, obstructionism, and overt
aggression. Expressed hostility typically involves the verbal and symbolic release of angry feelings, discontent, or
negative attitudes towards others (e.g. yelling, ostracism, and character assassination). Obstructionism includes
obscure actions intended to intentionally impede another’s performance, largely to damage the image or reputation of
specific employees or the organization (e.g. work slowdowns, inaction, and employee sabotage). Acts of overt
aggression entail predominantly physical actions directed toward an individual or their personal and corporate property
(e.g. physical assault and property damage). Despite the fact that the media has emphasized the dilemma of overt
aggression and fatal violence in the workplace, the vast majority of aggressive such incidents are verbal, passive, or
indirect actions, in large part due to their ambiguous nature which make it more difficult for victims, bystanders, and
employers to identify true aggressive motives (Baron & Neuman, 1996; Glomb 2002).
Previous research has uncovered a number of personal (e.g. Type A, vengefulness, anger, stress) and
organizational (e.g. workforce diversity, overload, unfair treatment) factors that interactively heighten aggression and
violence in the workplace (see Barling, 1996; Douglas & Martinko, 2001; Folger & Skarlicki, 1998; Neuman & Baron,
1998). However, recent attention has also focused on the issue of “spillover” in which workplace attitudes and
behavior are influenced by non-work factors (Leiter & Durup, 1996). The notion is that unresolved daily hassles from
one domain can persist, even when no longer in conscious awareness, and add to the pressures of subsequent hassles in
other life contexts (Kohn & Macdonald, 1992; Lazarus, 1981; Taylor,1991). These "after-effects" can continue to do
psychological and physiological damage, and may intensify over time as they accumulate with other previously
unresolved stress reactions (Glass & Singer, 1972). While there is greater evidence that workplace stress increases
stress, social withdrawal, and conflict at home (Koizumi, Sugawara, & Kitamura, 2001; Repetti, 1989; Thompson,
Kirk-Brown, & Brown, 2001), negative experiences at home have been found to magnify job overload, occupational
stress, psychological distress, and arguments/conflict in the workplace (Barnett, Marshall, & Pleck, 1992; Behson,
2002; Bolger, DeLongis, Kessler, & Wethington, 1989; Jenkins, 1996; Jones & Fletcher, 1996; Leiter & Durup, 1996).
The latter issue is particularly pressing given that numerous studies have independently identified conflict and
workplace stress as prevalent antecedents to workplace aggression and violence (Denenberg & Braverman, 1999;
Glomb, 2002; Jenkins, 1996; Neuman & Baron, 1998; Rai, 2002).
Notwithstanding the importance and prevalence of the home environment in an individual’s total life space,
there is currently little information on the potential impact of other domains on workplace reactions, such as daily
commuting. The driving environment is a common context that has been found to elicit elevated levels of stress and
arousal, particularly in highly frustrating conditions (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1997, 1999; Hennessy, Wiesenthal, &
Kohn, 2000; Novaco, Stokols, and Milanesi, 1990; Rasmussen, Knapp, & Garner, 2000; Stokols & Novaco, 1981;
Wiesenthal, Hennessy, & Totten, 2000). Within any driving encounter, there exists a multitude of stimuli that may be
perceived or interpreted as stressful, such as bad weather, time pressures, slow moving vehicles, or traffic congestion.
According to Broome (1985), stress typically occurs when goals, such as getting to work on time or travelling at a
desired pace, are blocked. Most regular commuters report facing numerous frustrations or irritations that can lead to
driver stress (Novaco et al., 1990; Gulian, Matthews, Glendon, Davies, & Debney, 1989b). In fact, a recent workplace
survey found that traffic congestion was the most prevalent source of daily stress among U.K. drivers (BBC News,
2000).
This issue is particularly pressing in that 60% of those over the age of 16 in the U.S. are part of the labor force
and, of these, 88% commute to work via personal vehicle (76% alone) with a median commute time of approximately
20 minutes in each direction (U.S. Census, 2000). While the consequences of commuter stress are individual and
varied, such conditions have been linked to a number of negative outcomes, such as increased blood pressure and heart
rate (Novaco, Stokols, Campbell, & Stokols, 1979; Stokols, Novaco, Stokols, & Campbell, 1978), negative mood
(Gulian, Debney, Glendon, Davies & Matthews, 1989a), emotional arousal (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1997), poor
concentration levels (Matthews, Dorn, & Glondon, 1991), driving errors, lapses, violations (Westerman & Haigney,
2000), traffic offences (Matthews, Tsuda, Xin, & Ozeki, 1999), traffic collisions (Legree, Heffner, Psotka, Martin, &
Medsker, 2003; Selzer & Vinokur, 1974), and aggression (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1999).
Stress experienced while driving is embedded in, and interacts with, the total life settings of home, work, and
leisure (Gulian et al., 1989b; Novaco et al., 1990). There is growing evidence driver stress is influenced by stressors
experienced in other life contexts that accumulate and carry forward to the driving environment (Gulian et al., 89b;
Hennessy et al., 2000). Unresolved hassles such poor weather, sleep problems, or conflicts at work and home may
exacerbate situational problems faced in the traffic environment, and increase the potential for driver stress and related
pathologies (Gulian et al., 1989b; Gulian, Glendon, Matthews, Davies, & Debney, 1990; Hennessy et al., 2000).
However, much less is known regarding the potential impact of the traffic environment on subsequent domains such as
the workplace. Hence the present study was designed to examine the possible “spillover” effect of traffic stress to the
workplace environment. Specifically, it was predicted that drivers experiencing greater state levels of driver stress
would exhibit greater expressed hostility and obstructionism during that work day.
Method
Participants
Participants consisted of 70 female and 44 male students from Buffalo State College that worked part time and
commuted to work. The age range was from 18 to 56 years, with an average of 19.95 years. Participants were
obtained through course recruitment, word of mouth, or campus advertisements.
Measures
The Aggression Questionnaire (AQ) is a 29 items self report measure of an overall trait aggressiveness (Buss
& Perry, 1992). The AQ consists of 4 subscales that measure a trait physical aggressiveness (9 items), trait verbal
aggressiveness (5 items), trait anger proneness (7 items), and trait hostility (8 items). Participants are asked to rate
how characteristic each item is of them in general using a 1-5 Likert scale (1 = “extremely uncharacteristic” and 5 =
“extremely characteristic”) . The AQ demonstrates high internal consistency (alpha = .92, Williams, Boyd, Cascardi,
& Poythress, 1996) and good test-retest reliability (r = .80) over a 9 week period. Subscale scoring consisted of
summing across items, with higher scores representing greater trait aggressiveness.
Driver Stress was measured using a variation of the Driving Behaviour Inventory—General (DBI-Gen) driver
stress questionnaire (Gulian et al., 1989b). The DBI-Gen consists of 16 items designed to measure a susceptibility to
stress while driving. Each item represents a situation that is often experienced by highly stressed drivers. Consistent
with Hennessy and Wiesenthal (1997), items were reworded to represent state experiences from their current commute;
for example, “Trying but failing to overtake was frustrating me”, was used rather than “Trying but failing to overtake
frustrates me”. Participants were asked to rate how much they agree with each item, using a 0 – 5 Likert scale (0 =
“totally disagree” and 5 = “totally agree”). Hennessy and Wiesenthal (1997) have found similar revisions to predict
actual stress levels measured in both low and high traffic congestion. Scoring consisted of the mean response to the 16
items, with higher scores indicating greater trait driver stress susceptibility.
The Short Form Driving Anger Scale (DAS) includes 14 items that measure a single driver anger dimension
(Deffenbacher, Oetting, & Lynch, 1994). Items present common driving scenarios that have been found to elicit
varying degrees of anger. For the purpose of the present study, participants were asked to imagine that each scenario
had just happened during their commute and rate the level of anger they would have experienced using a 1- 5 Likert
scale (1 = “not at all” and 5 = “very much”). The short form of the DAS has been found to demonstrate good
reliability (alpha = .80) and to correlate highly with the long form of the DAS (r = .95) (Deffenbacher, Huff, Lynch,
Oetting, & Salvatore, 2000). Scoring consisted of summing the responses to the 14 items, with higher scores
representing greater driver anger.
The Workplace Aggression Scale (WPAS) was developed as an overall measure of the self report prevalence of
workplace aggression (Neuman & Baron, 1998). The WPAS consists of three factors representing unique categories
of aggression: 1) Expressed Hostility involves 15 items representing verbal and symbolic release of feelings of anger,
discontent, or negative attitudes towards others (e.g. staring, obscene gestures, belittling); 2) Obstructionism involves 9
items based on actions intended to impede another’s performance (e.g. failing to return phone calls, work slowdowns,
showing up late for meetings); and 3) Overt Aggression involves 8 items that focus on physical actions toward an
individual or their property (e.g. attack with a weapon, threats of physical violence, sabotaging company property
needed by target). The present study revised the WPAS to represent a more “state” measure of the prevalence of
aggression during the past work day. Participants are asked to rate the extent that they personally initiated each form
of aggression during the target work day, using a 0-5 Likert scale (0 = “not at all” and 5 = “nearly all the time”).
Subscale scoring consisted of summing across items, with higher scores representing greater aggression during that
work day.
Procedure
The researchers initially met with participants to gain informed consent and to fully explain the research
procedure. During this meeting, participants completed the Aggression Questionnaire to measure their trait
aggressiveness and were provided the state questionnaire batteries to be completed following their next commute to
work. They were instructed to drive to work as usual and upon arrival in the parking lot, complete the State Stress and
Driver Anger scales. Subsequently, following that work day they were to complete the state version of the Workplace
Aggression scale.
Results
Table 1 contains all bivariate correlations, means, and alpha reliabilities for all scale and subscales. Overall,
all measures demonstrated moderate to high reliability (α = .61 - .91), although trait physical aggression (α = .67) and
trait anger (α = .61) showed the weakest consistency. Significant relationships were found between the state driver
measures (state stress and anger), between the workplace aggression subscales (expressed hostility, obstructionism,
and overt aggression), and between trait aggression subscales (physical, verbal, anger, and hostility). In addition, the
state driving indices, workplace aggression subscales, and trait aggression subscales all were significantly related to
one another.
Table 1. Intercorrelations, Means, Standard Deviations, and Reliabilities for State Driver Stress, Driver Anger,
Workplace Aggression, and Trait Aggression
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1. Driver Stress
—
2. Driver Anger
—
3. Expressed Hostility —
4. Obstructionism
—
5. Overt Aggression
—
6. Physical Aggression —
7. Verbal Aggression
—
8. Anger
—
9. Hostility
—
Mean
37.47
SD
13.7
Alpha
.86
Note: n=114; *p<.05; **p<.01
.66**
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
43.78
10.40
.87
.41**
.43**
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
16.47
11.30
..89
.32**
.29**
.72**
—
—
—
—
—
—
6.32
6.49
.87
.23*
.21*
.52**
.51**
—
—
—
—
—
1.60
3.37
.85
.31**
.27*
.40**
.29**
.41**
—
—
—
—
17.13
5.28
.67
.66**
.53**
.50**
.32**
.33**
.60**
—
—
—
29.17
8.71
.85
.44**
.38**
.42**
.43**
.35**
.58**
.78**
—
—
19.76
5.39
.61
.49**
.41**
.51**
.38**
.38**
.40**
.76**
.68*
—
18.38
6.36
.85
In order to examine the influences of state driver stress on workplace aggression, separate hierarchical entry
multiple regressions were conducted for expressed hostility, obstructionism, and overt aggression. The procedure was
to enter demographics (age and gender) in the first step, aggressive personality aspects (physical aggression, verbal
aggression, anger, hostility) in the second step, state driving factors (stress and anger) in the third step, and to add all
cross product interactions stepwise in the fourth step.
Both expressed hostility (R2=0.532, F(9,61)=7.70, p<.01; see Table 2) and obstructionism (R2=0.427,
F(9,61)=5.05, p<.01; see Table 3) were predicted by the interaction of gender X driver stress. Specifically, both
expressed hostility and obstruction were more prevalent among male drivers that experienced elevated stress during
their commute to work. Overt aggression was predicted by the interaction of gender X trait physical aggressiveness
(R2=0.389, F(9,61)=4.31, p<.01; see Table 4), where overt aggression was greatest among males with an elevated trait
physical aggressiveness.
Table 2. Regression Model For Expressed Hostility
b
t
Intercept
18.795
Demographics
Age
-.345
-.92
Gender
-10.488
-1.44
Trait Aggression
Hostility
.454
1.64
Anger
-.322
-.82
Physical Aggression
.447
7.79
Verbal Aggression
.094
.39
State Driving
Driver Anger
.117
.78
Driver Stress
-.626
-2.37*
Interaction Effects
Gender X Driver Stress
.484
2.56*
Table 3. Regression Model For Obstructionism
b
t
Intercept
10.084
Demographics
Age
-.066
-.29
Gender
-7.368
-1.66
Trait Aggression
Hostility
.135
.80
Anger
.288
1.21
Physical Aggression
.333
2.18
Verbal Aggression
-.236
-1.62
State Driving
Driver Anger
-.077
.78
Driver Stress
-.308
-1.91
Interaction Effects
Gender X Driver Stress
.298
2.59*
Table 4. Regression Model For Overt Aggression
b
t
Intercept
5.418
Demographics
Age
-.046
-.40
Gender
-4.844
-1.89
Trait Aggression
Hostility
.113
1.33
Anger
.027
.23
Physical Aggression
-.371
-1.56
Verbal Aggression
-.056
-.76
State Driving
Driver Anger
.001
.02
Driver Stress
.003
.07
Interaction Effects
Gender X Driver Stress
.364
2.60*
∆R2
∆F
.251
11.41**
.198
5.76**
.032
1.90
.050
6.57*
∆R2
∆F
.172
7.06**
.184
4.57**
.008
0.40
.063
6.72*
∆R2
∆F
.136
5.33**
.171
3.95**
.014
0.63
.068
6.79*
In order to investigate potential gender differences on key factors identified in the hierarchical regression, an
ANOVA was conducted using gender as the IV and state driver stress, state driver anger, expressed hostility,
obstructionism, and overt aggression as DV (see Table 5). Men reported greater state driver anger, expressed hostility,
obstructionism, and overt aggression than women, while no gender difference was found for state driver stress.
Table 5. ANOVA Results Comparing State Driving Measures and Workplace Aggression Factors Across Gender
Males
Females
Mean (SD)
Mean (SD)
Driver Stress
F(1,110) = .896, p<.ns
39.04 (12.67)
36.50 (14.31)
Driver Anger
F(1,110) = 5.18, p<.05
2.15 (1.04)
1.70 (.99)
Expressed Hostility
F(1,86) = 17.69, p<.01
22.05 (12.38)
12.61 (8.68)
Obstructionism
F(1,86) = 17.98, p<.01
9.55 (6.67)
4.09 (5.36)
Overt Aggression
F(1,101) = 21.04, p<.01
3.35 (4.67)
0.49 (1.30)
Discussion
As expected, state driver aggression was related to subsequent acts of workplace aggression, but only for
males and only in the form of expressed hostility and obstructionism. This suggests that, some form of spillover from
the traffic environment to the workplace environment is possible. Commuter stress is a serious issue that can have
significant physical and psychological consequences. But of particular note in this realm is the fact that it may
influence the cognitive processes involved in attention, information processing, and appraisal that are essential to
effective coping (Matthews, 2002). It is possible that the efforts to deal with traffic stress may exhaust coping
resources needed to deal with subsequent stressors in the workplace environment, while simultaneously priming
individuals to construe workplace strains as more demanding and stressful; consequently, increasing the possibility of
aggressive responses. According to Viitasara and Menckel (2002), such aggression and violence are strongly
influenced by immediate contextual factors, but broader situational factors help shape the nature and outcome of those
events. In the present study, it is possible that the broader situational factors for male commuters include previously
experienced traffic difficulties.
Interestingly, driver anger did not predict workplace aggression, despite the well established general link
between anger and aggression (see Berkowitz, 1993) and the strong relationship between state driver stress and state
driver anger in the present study. One potential reason may be that anger is an affective state based on the
interpretation of arousing conditions (see Geen, 1990). In the absence of the source of arousal, the affective reaction of
anger may diminish. This is consistent with Van Rooy and Rotton (2003) who found that arousal and negative affect
increased during a commute, but returned to baseline levels relatively quickly following its completion. However, as
with other sources of daily hassles, it is possible that any unresolved issues in the traffic environment may have
continued to influence and intensify subsequent workplace reactions to potential stressors, even though their
immediate, or “state”, effects may have become indistinguishable in conscious awareness. This notion is the crux of
the spillover effect; that unresolved difficulties from one domain accumulate and influence the interpretation and
intensify negative reaction to stressors in subsequent domains (Leiter & Durup, 1996).
Another finding of note is the fact that the spillover effects from traffic stress to workplace aggression
appeared only for men. Previous research has provided some evidence of gender differences in spillover, in that the
sources and reactions to work and family stressors and their ultimate outcomes across domains sometimes differ
between men and women, although the exact nature of this gender effect is mixed and not fully clear (Grzywacz &
Marks, 2000; Maume, 2001; Thompson et al., 2001). However, Bolger et al. (1989) did find that, for men only,
conflicts at home were proceeded by greater work stress the next day. Further, according to Almeida, Wethington, &
Kessler (2002), men are more prone than women to report workplace stress as a major daily hassle, and, consistent
with the present study, workplace aggression is typically committed by men rather than women (Neuman & Baron,
1998). In this respect, men may be more inclined to accept workplace aggression as an appropriate indirect coping
response, particularly if the consequences of previous acts of aggression have been “successful” in solving disputes or
reducing stress. Douglas and Martinko (2001) have stressed the importance of understanding personal factors in
understanding workplace aggression. While this is not to minimize the impact of social and organizational
antecedents, it is individuals that must interpret these experiences and their coping responses. Two such factors
include anger and hostile attribution biases in that anger prone and vengeful individuals are more prone to interpret
unpleasant events as stressful and demonstrate workplace aggression (Douglas & Martinko, 2001). State driver anger
was found to be greater among men than women in the present study, and previous research has consistently found that
men are more prone to hold a vengeful attitude in general (Stuckless & Goronson, 1992) and while driving
(Wiesenthal, Hennessy, & Gibson, 2000; Hennessy, 1999). As a result, the present study may have uncovered a
spillover effect only among men due to their greater tendency to interpret negative workplace events as stressful or
provocative and select aggression as an appropriate coping strategy under the cumulative effects of heightened life
hassles.
Future Directions
The potential for difficulties from one life context to carry forward to other domains, typically without
conscious awareness of the spillover process, can have profound effects on the understanding and treatment of a wide
range of problems, including workplace aggression. Despite the growing interest in the reciprocal influence of work
and family factors, there is still a great deal that is unknown about the process and product of spillover effects. The
present study has attempted to include a new direction into this area in that the transition between home and work for
the majority of the workforce involves the traffic environment, which on its own is a salient source of daily stress and
hassles. However the present study involved only a single measure of state stress and workplace aggression. State
sources of driver stress are often transient and reactions can vary temporally and, as such, may not be tapped in a
single time period. Similarly, workplace aggression is more accurately represented as a process involving a range of
overlapping and repeated behaviours (Viitasara & Menckel, 2002). Future research should look at the continuing
influence of state stressors on workplace behavior through multiple measurements over a longer time period. In a
similar respect, there are numerous personal and organizational factors that have been found to influence workplace
aggression that were not tapped in the present study. Future research should include a broader mix of potential
stressors from within the workplace (e.g. differences in job type, part time versus full time, conflict, time pressures),
the home environment (family conflict, time schedules, social support), the traffic context (time urgency, perceived
control, traffic conditions), and the individual (vengeance, job aspirations, desire for control). Further, examination of
indirect effects, rather than simply direct effects, may provide a more accurate model of spillover between family,
traffic, and the workplace (see Barnett, 1998). Finally, there is evidence that the work – home spillover process is
different than the home – work process, and that spillover can also lead to positive outcomes (Grzywacz & Marks,
2000). Future research is needed to understand how interpretations and actions in the traffic environment are impacted
by positive and negative work experiences, and how this environment alters thoughts, feelings, and actions in the
subsequent work or home domain.
References
Almeida, D. M., Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (2002). The daily inventory of stressful events: An interview based
approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment, 9, 41-55.
Barling, J. (1996). The prediction, experience, and consequences of workplace violence. In E. Q. Bulatao & G. R.
VandenBos (Eds.), Violence on the job: Identifying risks and developing solutions (pp. 29-49). Washington
DC: American Psychological Association.
Barnett, R. C. (1998). Toward and review and reconceptualization of the work/family literature. Genetic,
Social & General Psychology Monographs, 124, 8756-7547.
Barnett, R. C., Marshall, N. L., & Pleck, J. H. (1992). Men's multiple roles and their relationship to men's
psychological distress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 358-367.
Baron, R. A., & Nueman, J. H. (1996). Workplace violence and workplace aggression: Evidence of their relative
frequency and potential causes. Aggressive Behaivior, 22, 161-173.
BBC News. (2000, November 1). Commuting is "biggest stress." BBC News Online [On-line]. Available:
http://news.bbc.co.uk/hi/english/health/newsid-999000/999961.stm
Behson, S. J. (2002). Coping with family-to-work conflict: The role of informal work accommodations to
family. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 324-341.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., Wethington, E. (2001). The contagion of stress across multiple roles.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 51, 175-183.
Broome, M. R. (1985). The implication of driver stress. Proceedings of the Planning and Transportation Research and
Computation (PTRC) Seminar N., Summer Meeting Vol. P270. London, UK: PTRC Education Research
Services Ltd.
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
452-459.
Deffenbacher, J. L., Huff, M. E., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Salvatore, N. F. (2000). Characteristics and treatment
of high anger drivers. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 5-17.
Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., & Lynch, R. S. (1994). Development of a driving anger scale.
Psychological Reports, 74, 83-91.
Denenberg, R. V., & Braverman, M. (1999). The violence prone workplace. London: Cornell University Press.
Douglas, S. C., & Martinko, M. J. (2001). Exploring the role of individual differences in prediction of workplace
aggression. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 547-559.
Folger, R., & Starlicki, D. P. (1998). A popcorn model of workplace violence. In R.W. Griffin, A. O’Leary-Kelly, &
J. Collins (Eds.), Dysfunctional behavior in organizations: Nonviolent dysfunctional behavior (pp. 43-82).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Geen, R. (1990). Human aggression. Pacific Grove CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.
Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. (1972). Urban Stress. New York: Academic.
Glomb, T. M. (2002). Workplace anger and aggression: Informing conceptual models with data from specific
encounters. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 20-36.
Grzywacz, J. G., & Marks, N. F. (2000). Reconceptualizing the work-family interface: An ecological perspective on
the correlates of positive and negative spillover between work and family. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 5, 111-126.
Gulian, E., Glendon, A. I., Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., & Debney, L. M. (1990). The stress of driving: A diary study.
Work and Stress, 4, 7-16.
Gulian, E., Debney, L. M., Glendon, A. I., Davies, D. R., & Matthews, G. (1989a). Coping with driver stress. In F. J.
McGuigan, W. E. Sime, & J. M. Wallace (Eds.), Stress and tension control (Vol. 3) (pp. 173-186). New York:
Plenum.
Gulian, E., Matthews, G., Glendon, A. I., Davies, D. R., & Debney, L. M. (1989b). Dimensions of driver stress.
Ergonomics, 32, 585-602.
Hennessy, D. A. (1999). The influence of driving vengeance on aggression and violence. Proceedings of the
Canadian Multidisciplinary Road Safety Conference XI. Daltech Vehicle Safety Institute: Dalhousie
University, Halifax, Canada.
Hennessy, D. A., & Wiesenthal, D. L. (1997). The relationship between traffic congestion, driver stress, and direct
versus indirect coping behaviours. Ergonomics, 40, 348-361.
Hennessy, D. A., & Wiesenthal, D. L. (1999). Traffic congestion, driver stress, and driver aggression. Aggressive
Behavior, 25, 409-423.
Hennessy, D. A., Wiesenthal, D. L., & Kohn, P. M. (2000). The influence of traffic congestion, daily hassles, and
trait stress susceptibility on state driver stress: An interactive perspective. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral
Research, 5, 162-179.
Jenkins, E. L. (1996). Violence in the workplace: Risk factors and prevention strategies. NIOSH Publication 96-100
(pp. 1-18). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Jones, F., & Fletch, B. (1996). Taking work home: A study of daily fluctuations in work stressors, effects of mood and
impacts and marital partners. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 69, 89-106.
Kohn, P. M., & Macdonald, J. E. (1992). Hassles, anxiety, and negative well-being. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 5,
151-163.
Koizumi, T., Sugawara, M., & Kitamura, T. (2001). The impact of spillover from work to family on depression,
marital relationships and child rearing stress among Japanese dual-earner couples with school children.
Journal of Mental Health, 47, 65-75.
Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Little hassles can be hazardous to health. Psychology Today, July, 58-62.
Legree, P. J., Heffner, T. S., Psotka, J., Martin, D. E., & Medsker, G. J. (2003). Traffic crash involvement:
Experiential driving knowledge and stressful contextual antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1526.
Leiter, M. P., & Durup, M. J. (1996). Work, home, and in-between: A longitudinal study of spillover. Journal of
Applied Behavioral Science, 32, 29-47.
Matthews, G. (2002). Toward a transactional ergonomics for driver stress and fatigue. Theoretical Issues in
Ergonomics Science, 3, 195-211.
Matthews, G., Dorn, L., & Glendon, A. I. (1991). Personality correlates of driver stress. Personality and Individual
Differences, 12, 535-549.
Matthews, G., Tsuda, A., & Xin, G., & Ozeki, Y. (1999). Individual differences in driver stress vulnerability in a
Japanese sample. Ergonomics, 42, 401-415.
Maume, D. (2001). Job segregation and gender differences in work-family spillover among white collar workers.
Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 22, 171-189.
Neuman, J. H., & Baron, R. A. (1998). Workplace violence and workplace aggression: Evidence concerning specific
forms, potential causes, and preferred targets. Journal of Management, 24, 391-419.
Novaco, R. W., Stokols, D., Campbell, J., & Stokols, J. (1979). Transportation, stress, and community psychology.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 7, 361-380.
Novaco, R. W., Stokols, D., & Milanesi, L. (1990). Objective and subjective dimensions of travel impedance as
determinants of commuting stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 231-257.
Rai, S. (2002). Preventing workplace aggression and violence: A role for occupational therapy. Work,18, 15-22.
Rasmussen, C., Knapp, T. J., & Garner, L. (2000). Driving induced stress in urban college students. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 90, 437-443.
Repetti, R. L. (1989). Effects of daily workload on subsequent behavior during marital interaction: The roles of social
withdrawal and spouse support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 651-659.
Selzer, M. L., & Vinokur, A. (1974). Life events, subjective stress and traffic accidents. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 131, 903-906.
Stokols, D., & Novaco, R. W. (1981). Transportation and well being. In G. Altman, J. F. Wohlwill, & P. B. Everett
(Eds.), Human behavior and environment (Vol. 5): Transportation and behavior. London: Plenum Press.
Stokols, D,. Novaco, R. W., Stokols, J., & Campbell, J. (1978). Traffic congestion, type A behavior, and stress.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 467-480.
Stuckless, N., & Goranson, R. (1992). The vengeance scale: Development of a measure of attitudes toward revenge.
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7, 25-42.
Taylor, S. (1991). Health psychology (2nd Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Thompson, B., Kirk-Brown, A., & Brown, D. (2001). Women police: The impact of work stress on family members.
In P.A. Hancock & P.A. Desmond (Eds.), Stress, workload, and fatigue: Human factors in transportation (pp.
200-210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
U.S. Census (2000). Profile of selected economic characteristics: Chart DP-3. Available online at
http://www.census.gov/main/www/cen2000.html.
Van Rooy, D. L., & Rotton, J. (2003). Effects of congestion and distance on affect and candidate evaluations. Paper
presented at the 111th Annual American Psychological Association Convention, August 7-10, Toronto Canada.
Viitasara, E., & Menckel, E. (2002). Developing a framework for identifying individual and organizational risk factors
for the prevention of violence in the health care sector. Work, 19,
117-123.
Westerman, S. J., & Haigney, D. (2000). Individual differences in driver stress, error and violation. Personality and
Individual Differences, 29, 981-998.
Wiesenthal, D. L., Hennessy, D. A., & Gibson, P. (2000). The Driving Vengeance Questionnaire (DVQ): The
development of a scale to measure deviant drivers' attitudes. Violence and Victims, 15, 115-136.
Wiesenthal, D. L., Hennessy, D. A., & Totten, B. (2000). The influence of music on driver stress. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 30, 1709-1719.
Williams, T. Y., Boyd, J. C., Cascardi, M. A., & Poythress, N. (1996). Factor structure and convergent validity of the
Aggression Questionnaire in an offender population. Psychological Assessment, 8, 398-403.
Download