Supplementary Figure legends Figure S1. Position-defined priming in Experiment 1 with all nine participants. (a) Schematic of the trial sequence. The prime stimulus was always defined by position cues. The probe stimulus could either also be defined by position cues (within-cue) or by orientation cues (cross-cue). Shown here are primed trials, in which the probe and prime are the same shape. In unprimed trials, the probe would be the opposite shape as the prime. (b) The magnitude of the priming effect (how much faster response times were for primed relative to unprimed trials) is plotted for the four priming conditions: visible and invisible primes, within-cue and cross-cue condition. As in the main analysis, there was a significant interaction between awareness and integration (F(1,8)=6.88, p=0.03), but no main effects (awareness: F(1,8)=2.81, p=0.132; integration: F(1,8)=1.04, p=0.339). (c) Median response time is plotted for every trial type. In both b and c, data reflect mean across participants, error bars denote ±1 standard error of the mean (between-subject variance removed). Figure S2. Orientation-defined primes in Experiment 2 with all nine participants. (a) Schematic of the trial sequence. The prime stimulus was always defined by orientation cues. The probe stimulus could either also be defined by orientation cues (within-cue) or by position cues (cross-cue). Shown here are primed trials, in which the probe and prime are the same shape. In unprimed trials, the probe would be the opposite shape as the prime. (b) The magnitude of the priming effect (how much faster response times were for primed relative to unprimed trials) is plotted for the four priming conditions: visible and invisible primes, within-cue and cross-cue condition. As in the main analysis, there was a significant interaction between awareness and integration (F(1,8)=30.66, p=0.001), but no main effects (awareness: F(1,8)=0.06, p=0.807; integration: F(1,8)=0.11, p=0.754). (c) Median response time is plotted for every trial type. In both b and c, data reflect mean across participants, error bars denote ±1 standard error of the mean (between-subject variance removed). Supplementary Information Analysis of response times separately for each condition Experiment 1 A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA with factors awareness (visible, invisible), integration (within-cue, cross-cue) and priming (primed, unprimed) revealed a significant effect of priming (F(1,6)=8.84, p=0.025). Moreover, there was a significant three-way interaction which is the statistical equivalent of the interaction we reported above for the priming effect (F(1,6)=15.06, p=0.008). The lack of cross-cue priming for invisible primes was probably due to shorter response times when the prime was visible compared to when it was invisible, although this difference did not reach significance (paired t-test: t(6)=-2.39, p=0.054). For unprimed trials, however, response times were the same for visible and invisible primes (t(6)=0.04, p=0.97). This suggests that conscious processing of the position-defined prime afforded participants with a benefit speeding up their behavioural responses. Experiment 2 A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA with factors awareness (visible, invisible), integration (within-cue, cross-cue) and priming (primed, unprimed) revealed a significant effect of priming (F(1,6)=11.71, p=0.014). Moreover, overall response times visible trials were faster than those to invisible trials (F(1,6)=7.8, p=0.031). There also was a significant interaction between awareness and integration (F(1,6)=17.38, p=0.006), and a significant three-way interaction which is the statistical equivalent of the interaction we reported above for the priming effect (F(1,6)=28.67, p=0.002). The response times for primed trials in the within-cue condition were significantly shorter for visible than invisible primes (paired t-test: t(6)=-4.65, p=0.003). In contrast, for unprimed trials response times were similar regardless of whether the prime was visible or not (t(6)=-1.8, p=0.122). This suggests that conscious processing of orientation-defined primes is necessary to speed up participants’ responses when the probe is also defined by orientation. Experiment 3 Analysing the response times for all experimental conditions separately (Figure 4c) revealed that responses to within-cue primed trials were faster than that to unprimed trials when the prime was visible. In all other conditions, the response times did not change between visible and invisible trials. This was supported by a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA with factors awareness (visible, invisible), integration (within-cue, cross-cue) and priming (primed, unprimed), which showed a significant difference between within- and cross-cue conditions (F(1,5)=9.98, p=0.025) and a main effect of priming (F(1,5)=7.9, p=0.038), but no difference between visible and invisible trials (F(1,5)=1.84, p=0.233). The response time to primed within-cue trials was significantly shorter than for invisible primed trials (t(5)=-2.99, p=0.031), but there was no difference for unprimed trials (t(5)=0.29, p=0.787). Experiment 4 We also analysed the response times in all of the conditions separately (Figure 5c). A twoway repeated-measures ANOVA with factors awareness (visible, invisible) and priming (primed, unprimed) showed that there was a significant priming effect (F(1,12)=8.06, p=0.015). Moreover, overall response times for visible trials were significantly shorter than for invisible trials (F(1,12)=16.6, p=0.002). Confirming effectiveness of masking Experiment 1 In order to confirm that our method of rendering the primes invisible was effective, participants performed a brief test session after the main experiment. Here, the trial sequence was truncated after the presentation of the first stimulus and participants judged whether it was a square or diamond. This showed that while performance at this task was unsurprisingly very high when the stimulus was visible (mean accuracy: 0.96±0.01), when the stimulus was invisible, participants were unable to judge the shape: performance was low (0.52±0.02) and not significantly different from chance (t(8)=0.99, p=0.18). Consistent with this, no participant reported being aware of the fact that any stimulus had been presented during invisible intervals when debriefed after the experiment. Experiment 2 As in the first experiment, after the main experiment participants performed a quick test session to test that rendering the primes invisible had been successful. Again, performances for visible stimuli was very high when the stimulus was visible (mean accuracy: 0.95±0.02), but when the stimulus was invisible, performance was low (0.47±0.02) and not significantly different from chance (t(9)=-1.81, p=0.104). As with the Gaussian elements in the previous experiment, after the experiment none of the participants reported being aware that invisible Gabor patches had been presented in invisible trials. Supplementary Materials and Methods Participants Nine healthy participants (3 male, 1 left-handed, age: 22-41) with normal or corrected-tonormal visual acuity, gave written informed consent to participate in Experiments 1 and 2. Six participants (2 male, all right-handed, age: 22-32) participated in Experiment 3. Thirteen healthy participants (9 female, 2 left-handed, age: 22-41) participated in Experiment 4. All except for one (author DSS), were naïve to the purpose of the study. The procedures were approved by the local ethics committee. Stimulus parameters Orientation cues were Gabor patches (standard deviation and carrier wavelength: 0.42°, i.e. carrier frequency: 2.38 cycles/°) that either had cardinal or oblique orientations to create a square or diamond shape, respectively. Position cues had luminance profile described by a difference of two Gaussians (standard deviation: 0.42° and 0.21°, respectively). Cues were presented on a uniform grey background at low but suprathreshold contrast (0.07). Stimuli subtended 17.45° of visual angle and were presented at the centre of a CRT screen (resolution: 1024*768, refresh rate: 120Hz). Participants viewed the screen at a distance of 67cm and were stabilised by a chin rest with forehead support. In Experiment 4, stimuli also comprised 8 elements, all of which were Gabor patches, i.e. orientation cues. Elements were placed randomly with the constraint that the distance between any two elements must be at least 2.7° of visual angle. The position of elements was randomized on each trial. The parameters of Gabor patches was identical to the other experiments. In half of the stimuli, the orientation of all elements was cardinal (half orientated at 0°, half at 90°). In the remaining half of stimuli, the orientations were all oblique (half at 45°, half at 135°). This ensured that the content of local information was comparable to the orientation-defined shape stimuli of the other experiments, while there was no global shape interpretation. Procedure Participants completed 800 trials in Experiments 1-3, which were subdivided into 25 blocks with participant-terminated resting breaks. There were 16 trial conditions each of which appeared twice within each block in a pseudo-randomized order: 2 probe shapes (square, diamond) X 2 cues (orientation, position) X 2 priming states (primed, unprimed) X 2 awareness conditions (visible, invisible). Altogether, there were thus 100 trials for all the combinations of interest. Since in Experiment 4 there was no cross-cue priming condition, trial numbers were half than that in the other experiments. Confirming effectiveness of masking After each main experiment, participants further completed a brief test session to ensure that rendering the primes invisible had been successful. Here, the trial sequence was similar to the main experiment, except that it was truncated after the presentation of the prime and participants were asked to distinguish whether the shape they saw was a square or a diamond. The fixation dot during the stimulus presentation was blue. In half the trials the stimulus was again rendered invisible. There were thus four stimulus conditions (2 shapes X 2 awareness conditions) each of which was presented 8 times per block, with four blocks in total. This resulted in 64 trials each for the visible and invisible condition.