Acute compensatory eating following exercise is associated with implicit hedonic wanting for food 5 1 Finlayson G, 1Bryant E, 1Blundell JE & 2King NA. 1 BioPsychology Group, Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS29JT, UK; 2Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Human Movement Studies, 10 Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane. Australia Correspondence to: Graham Finlayson 15 Biopsychology group Institute of Psychological Sciences University of Leeds Leeds LS2 9JT United Kingdom 20 Tel: 44 113 343 7601 Fax: 44 113 343 6674 Authors’ email addresses: g.s.finlayson@leeds.ac.uk 25 e.j.bryant@leeds.ac.uk 2 j.e.blundell@leeds.ac.uk n.king@qut.edu.au 5 Running head: Acute compensatory eating following exercise 3 Abstract The efficacy of exercise to promote weight loss could potentially be undermined by its influence on explicit or implicit processes of liking and wanting for food which in turn alter food preference. The present study was designed to examine hedonic and homeostatic 5 mechanisms in the acute effects of exercise on food intake. 24 healthy female subjects were recruited to take part in two counterbalanced activity sessions; 50 minutes of high intensity (70% max heart rate) exercise (Ex) or no exercise (NEx). Subjective appetite sensations, explicit and implicit hedonic processes, food preference and energy intake (EI) were measured immediately before and after each activity session and an ad libitum test meal. Two 10 groups of subjects were identified in which exercise exerted different effects on compensatory EI and food preference. After exercise, compensators (C) increased their EI, rated the food to be more palatable, and demonstrated increased implicit wanting. Compensators also showed a preference for high-fat sweet food compared with non-compensators (NC), independent of the exercise intervention. Exercise-induced changes in the hedonic response to food could play an 15 important role in weight control. The exercise-induced increase in implicit wanting for food may help to explain the variable efficacy of exercise as a method of weight loss. Some individuals could be resistant to the beneficial effects of exercise due to a predisposition to compensate for exercise-induced energy expenditure as a result of changes in food preferences. 20 4 Introduction Physical activity is currently strongly promoted as a method of weight control. However, in some individuals, a compensatory increase in energy intake (EI) might serve as a barrier for the potential for exercise to promote a negative energy balance (1). Therefore, the 5 efficacy of exercise as a successful method of weight management will vary between individuals (2). EI is commonly put forward as the compensatory mechanism responsible for a lack of, or lower than expected, exercise-induced weight loss. Therefore, it is important to identify the homeostatic and hedonic processes which could be mobilised by exercise to influence food intake. For example, exercise may be linked to food intake through changes in 10 food preferences and macronutrient selection (3;4). This relationship seems to depend on the characteristics of the population studied (5;6;7) and may reflect a biological drive for foods with particular sensory or nutrient properties (8;9;10). Exercise and food intake may also be linked by their rewarding potential. Evidence indicates that vigorous exercise can stimulate brain substrates which are associated with reward and 15 dependence (11;12). More recently, ‘Sensitivity to Reward’ – thought to reflect an innate characterological trait – has been identified (13;14) and is suggestive of a common neural substrate for determining the impact of pleasurable activities including exercise and feeding (15). It has been hypothesised that exercise may act as a buffer for reward driven eating (15), however it is also possible that exercise has a sensitising action that enhances food reward. 20 Differences in the impact of exercise on food reward may explain some inter-individual variability in compensatory eating after exercise. Of course, deliberate inappropriate food choices and allowance of food ‘treats’ could also be responsible for increases in EI (2). Nevertheless, the influence of hedonic processes on the behavioural action of eating can be considered as two-fold: Strength of motivational response 25 to obtain available food, and the degree of subjective pleasure induced. Recently, the terms 5 liking and wanting have been used to distinguish such hedonic processes that are either explicitly affective or implicitly motivational (16;17) and these may represent dual components of food preference (18;19;20). Therefore, it can be hypothesised that hedonic processes – via their influence on food preference – could be modulated by exercise-induced 5 energy expenditure to promote overconsumption in those prone to compensatory eating. A key issue is whether changes in exercise-induced food preference and nutrient selection are associated with hedonic or homeostatic mechanisms. Recent considerations of homeostatic and non-homeostatic determinants of eating imply that such systems interact in the overall expression of appetite (21;22;23). Since most of the evidence from acute exercise 10 interventions indicates that there is no automatic increase in hunger following exercise (24;25;2), other non-homeostatic mechanisms need to be explored. Traditionally, methods such as test meals providing a selection of foods varying in nutrient composition (26), palatability visual analogue scales (27), and titration of the ratio of sweetness to fat in drinks (28) can be used to assess the acute effects of exercise on food preference. However, these 15 methods are not designed – and therefore may not be sensitive enough – to detect more subtle exercise-induced alterations in the hedonic processes that influence food preference. Therefore, this study used a novel computer-based procedure to allow the separate assessments of explicit and implicit operations of liking and wanting for an array of food items (19) to determine the acute effects of exercise on food preference, and explore 20 individual variability in the compensatory response to exercise. 6 Method Participants Participants were recruited from the staff and student population of the University of Leeds. 5 Screening sessions involved completion of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (29), acceptance of the study foods and level of habitual exercise (mean = 2.4 SD = 1.2 engagements/week) were verified by self report, and body composition was measured using bioelectrical impedance analysis (Inbody, Biospace, California). 24 healthy females aged 1840 years (mean = 24.0, SD = 6.1 yr) and body mass index (BMI) between 20-25 kg/m2 (mean 10 = 22.3, SD = 2.9 kg/m2) were selected from the initial screening process to exclude those who were taking medication, smokers, those currently following a weight loss routine, or reported a history of eating or psychological disorders. Following criteria from previous studies (Brunstrom et al. 2004; Tepper, 1992), all were classed as non-dietary restrained indicated by a score of less than 11 for Restraint on the TFEQ (mean = 6.1, SD = 3.1). Two cases were 15 excluded from the analyses due to incomplete data, leaving a final sample of N=22. Design All the manipulations and measures were administered in the Human Appetite Research Unit at University of Leeds. The study conformed to a within-subjects design consisting of two 20 counterbalanced conditions separated by approximately one week. The two conditions were moderate-intensity exercise (Ex) which involved stationery cycling at approximately 70% maximum heart rate for 50 minutes, and 50 minutes of no exercise (NEx). The NEx condition involved being sedentary (e.g. reading, sitting quietly). The exercise sessions were conducted under supervision. Physical activity levels prior to testing were standardised by asking 25 participants to refrain from exercise on the day before each condition. Prior to their 7 involvement in the study all participants were informed about the procedures and gave their written consent. On the final test session, they received written and verbal debriefing and were invited to ask questions. All procedures were followed in accordance with the standards of the institutional ethics committee. 5 Measures Subjective appetite sensations and hedonic evaluation of the test meal Subjective appetite sensations were recorded throughout the test days using 100mm visual analogue scales (VAS), anchored at each end with the statements “not at all” and “extremely”. 10 Appetite sensations were assessed by questions relating to hunger, thirst, fullness and desire to eat. These were assessed immediately before and after the Ex and NEx sessions, and following the test meal. VAS hedonic ratings of the test meal were recorded immediately following consumption according to the pleasantness of the food. 15 Liking and wanting computer procedure The computer procedure comprised two tasks designed to assess 1) explicit liking, 2) implicit wanting, and 3) relative preference for the same visual food stimuli. This experimental procedure has been described in more detail previously (19) and follows other laboratories successful use of reaction time as an indicator of implicit processes (for example, the Implicit 20 Association Test; 30). The procedure uses a ‘forced choice’ reaction time measure of implicit wanting in addition to explicit subjective measures of liking for visual food stimuli varying in fat content and taste. Implicit wanting is operationalised as the reaction time of each pair trial decision. Thus, the speed with which one category of stimuli is chosen relative to alternative categories provides a quantifiable measure of implicit wanting for each food category in the 25 procedure. The visual food stimuli were selected from a database of photographs and sorted 8 according to their fat content and taste properties into one of four separate categories: high fat savoury (HFSA); low fat savoury (LFSA); high fat sweet (HFSW); and low fat sweet (LFSW). Each category was represented by five different foods; hence a total of 20 different food stimuli were presented in the procedure (see table 1). Participants completed the task 5 immediately before Ex or NEx, immediately after Ex or NEx, and immediately after consuming the test meal. Stimuli were presented using experiment generator software (EPrime v1.1.4) via 17” monitor and measured 150×100 mm2. Table 1 about here 10 Explicit liking trials The explicit task recorded subjective hedonic ratings for each food stimulus using VAS. The trials consisted of the twenty food stimuli presented one at a time and rated according to a 100-mm VAS anchored at each end by the statements “not at all” and “extremely”. Subjects 15 were prompted with the statement “How pleasant would it be to taste some of this food now?”. The VAS was presented on-screen beneath each food stimulus and subjects used the mouse to move a centred cursor along the line to indicate their response. When a rating was made, the procedure automatically cycled to the next stimulus trial. Mean ratings for each food category (HFSA, LFSA, etc.) were automatically computed. 20 Implicit wanting trials and relative preference Implicit wanting and relative preference were measured by a behavioural ‘forced choice’ methodology. In this task, a food stimulus from one of the four food categories was paired with one stimulus from the remaining categories to form a series of 150 trials in which the 25 subjects were given the standardised instruction to select the food they “most want to eat 9 now”. In addition to measuring relative preference for the stimuli by recording the frequency of selections made in each category (with a possible range of 0-75), reaction time (in ms) of each choice was also captured. By covertly recording reaction time, subjects remained unaware of implicit changes in their behaviour on the task, while remaining free to determine 5 the direction of their choices. Data from the forced choice task – including relative preference (freq. of choice) and implicit wanting (reaction time in ms) – were recorded online for later calculation of the means. Test meal 10 The ad libitum test meal consisted of cheese salad sandwiches, ready-salted crisps and fruit yoghurt. The average percentage energy provided by protein, fat and carbohydrate in the test meal was 13%, 45% and 42% respectively. Energy intake was calculated by weighing the food before and after consumption (to the nearest 0.1g) and with reference to the manufacturers energy values and food tables (30). 15 Heart rate Prior to the experimental sessions, subjects were fitted with a heart rate monitor (Polar Electronic-4000, Finland). This was to ensure that exercise was performed at a fixed intensity (~70% maximum heart rate) and to estimate net energy expenditure from the exercise bout. 20 Maximum heart rate was assessed on a screening day prior to test days, using an incremental test on a cycling ergometer (Proergo, Tunturi Ltd, Japan). Participants were required to cycle until volitional exhaustion, at which point maximum heart rate was inferred. Mean heart rate from the 50-minute exercise bout was calculated to predict energy expenditure using the Keytel-Hiiloskorpi equation for women (Keytel et al, 2005). Net energy expenditure was 25 then estimated by subtracting the energy cost of 50-minutes of no exercise using the Mifflin- 10 St Jeor equation for resting metabolic rate in women multiplied by the predicted energy cost of sitting from the World Health Organisation technical report series appendices (FAO/WHO/UNU, 1985). 5 Procedure Participants arrived at 0900 and were required to stay in the research unit until the end of the test session after lunch. On arrival at the unit they were provided with a fixed energy breakfast (cereal with milk). Immediately before the exercise session, the liking and wanting computer procedure and ratings of appetite were completed (T1). Participants were navigated 10 through the procedures at all times by written instructions presented on-screen. At 1100 they began either the Ex or NEx activity. Immediately after the activity, participants completed the liking and wanting procedure and subjective ratings of appetite (T2). They then showered and changed before being provided with an ad libitum test meal at 1230. Participants ate alone and were instructed to eat to comfortable fullness. Immediately after the test meal they 15 completed the liking and wanting procedure and a final set of appetite ratings (T3). Figure 1 shows a schematic diagram of the procedure. The testing procedure lasted approximately 2 hours. The experimental protocol was approved by the Institute of Psychological Sciences Ethical Committee at University of Leeds. Participants gave their written consent and were not paid for their participation. 20 Figure 1 about here Treatment of data and statistical analyses The main analyses for this report focused on the variability in compensatory eating measured 25 from energy intake at the test meal. Differences among participants classified on the basis of 11 relative energy intake (Ex-NEx) were used to guide further post hoc analyses and tested by within-between subjects ANOVA. Analysis of the liking and wanting measures focused on the explicit liking, implicit wanting and relative food preference measures. The parametric data were analysed in separate 3-way 5 ANOVA with exercise activity (Ex or NEx), time (T1, T2, T3), and food category (HFSA, LFSA, HFSW and LFSW) treated as repeated factors. Trends in the relative preference measure were assessed descriptively. From the initial sample of 24 women, two cases were excluded from the analyses due to incomplete data, leaving a final sample of N=22. 10 12 Results Energy intake and energy expenditure 5 As expected, differences in energy expenditure were highly significant between Ex and Nex sessions. Mean heart rate and predicted energy expenditure were 94.2 ± 2.7 bpm and 189.3 ± 13.0 kcal for the Ex session and 81.4 ± 2.7 bpm and 47.0 kcal for the Nex session. No differences were found between Ex and Nex for energy intake. Total energy intakes for the Ex and NEx conditions were 1128.2 ± 72.8 and 1018.1 ± 73.0 kcal respectively. 10 Inter-individual variability in the acute effect of exercise on compensatory eating Individual variability in relative energy intake (REI) was examined by subtracting energy intake after NEx from energy intake after Ex. In addition, net differences in compensatory eating were calculated by subtracting net energy expenditure (EE during Ex – EE during 15 NEx) from REI. These calculations provided measures of the impact of the exercise bout on compensatory eating. Figure 2 illustrates this variability in compensatory eating and demonstrates the range of responses including individuals who suppressed or made no adjustment to their intake and some for whom exercise stimulated energy intake. As the mean energy cost of exercise for the whole sample (n=22) was 149.0 kcal ±11.2, REI of >150kcal 20 indicated complete acute compensation. Figure 2 about here On the basis of this variation in relative energy intake, the sample was subdivided into two 25 groups of response. These groups corresponded to a) those who ate approximately the same 13 or less after exercise (non-compensators; N=11) and b) those who compensated or ate more after exercise (compensators; N=11). The exercise-induced energy expenditure for the compensators (146.3 kcal ±8.5) was similar to the non-compensators (151.2 kcal ±13.9). 5 Subject characteristics The demographic and eating behaviour trait characteristics of compensators (C) and noncompensators (NC) were compared to try and identify factors that might explain the differences in compensatory eating. C scored higher on BMI, % body fat and lower on self reported frequency of habitual exercise compared with NC. Independent samples t-tests did 10 not reveal the differences to be statistically significant (highest t= -1.26, p=n.s.). Subjective sensations of appetite and hedonic evaluation of the test meal Examination of appetite sensations revealed no group differences in terms of hunger, fullness, thirst or desire to eat. There was an overall main effect of time for hunger (F(2,40) = 46.35, 15 p<0.01) and desire to eat (F(2,40) = 54.13, p<0.01), with higher ratings after the intervention compared to baseline (p<0.01) and lower ratings after consumption of the test meal (p<0.01). As expected, a time*intervention interaction was observed for ratings of thirst (F(2,40) = 4.34, p<0.05) which increased following Ex compared to NEx. Analysis of the hedonic evaluation of the test meal revealed a group*intervention interaction 20 in the NEx condition. Compensators rated the test meal as significantly less pleasant (F(1,20) = 4.78, p<0.05) after NEx compared to NC. Figure 3 shows that the hedonic ratings after Ex are similar in C and NC. Figure 3 about here 25 14 Food preference computer procedure Explicit liking No group differences in explicit liking were found. There was a main effect of time (F(2,40) = 19.53, p<0.01) with higher ratings after both Ex and NEx compared to baseline (p<0.01), 5 and lower ratings after the test meal compared to after Ex or NEx (p<0.01). Implicit wanting Analysis of implicit wanting responses revealed a significant three-way group*intervention*time interaction (F(2,32) = 6.13, p<0.01). Further examination of the 10 means suggested that Ex exerted different effects on C compared with NC. Table 2 shows mean values for implicit wanting (data for food categories pooled) for each time point. Compensators’ reaction time decreased after exercise (T1-T2) relative to NC, while NC’s reaction time increased after the test meal (T2-T3) relative to C. Post-hoc independent sample t-tests revealed that C’s reaction time was slower for low fat stimuli at baseline (LFSA: t(20) 15 = -2.54, p<0.05), LFSW: t(20) = -3.61, p<0.01), while their reaction time after the test meal was faster for high fat stimuli (HFSA: t(20) = 2.36, p<0.05), HFSW: t(20) = 2.16, p=0.06). Table 2 about here 20 On examination of the mean frequency of choice for each food category, C showed a higher preference for high-fat sweet foods compared to NC. This trend was relatively stable and did not seem contingent on time or exercise condition. 15 Discussion This study used a novel procedure to assess the role of food preference in the acute effect of exercise on food intake and revealed some interesting and novel findings. Examination of individual EI following exercise compared to no exercise revealed two diverging response 5 types in these participants; those who ate approximately the same or less after exercise compared to no exercise (NC) and those who ate more after exercise compared to no exercise (C). The identification of this individual difference in acute compensatory response revealed some interesting further comparisons. Examination of the anthropometric characteristics of both groups did not reveal any statistically significant differences; however C had on average 10 more body fat and a higher BMI. These are characteristics of individuals who have experienced a positive energy balance and might be more susceptible to weight gain. It is therefore surprising that no differences in subjective sensations of appetite were revealed between the groups. This suggests that hunger levels were unlikely to have played a role in the differences in food intake observed after Ex. The lack of an automatic exercise-induced 15 increase in hunger is in line with most of the recent literature that shows no relationship between exercise and enhanced appetite (10;24;32;33). However, examination of homeostatic mechanisms in isolation ignores the notion that other psychological and cognitive factors may be influenced by volitional exercise to bring about changes in EI (34). In the present study, some interesting group differences were apparent in the hedonic 20 evaluation of the test meal. Hedonic ratings of pleasantness differed between C and NC. Compensators rated the meal as less pleasant after NEx while ratings were higher and did not differ between groups after a bout of exercise. This suggests that while lower in the absence of exercise, Ex exerted an effect on hedonic ratings of the meal in C but not in NC. Similar exercise- induced increases in hedonic evaluation of sweetened preloads have been reported 25 in previous research (35;4), however intra-individual differences in the effect of exercise on 16 hedonic response to food has not previously been empirically demonstrated. These findings may indicate a role for reward-based mechanisms in the tendency to overcompensate following exercise. This hypothesis was explored further by use of the novel food preference computer procedure 5 incorporating both explicit and implicit measures of liking and wanting. No significant group differences were revealed in explicit liking for food stimuli presented in the task. However, significant group differences were detected on the implicit wanting measure of the task, which is less likely influenced by the cognitive interpretation of subjective feelings. Indeed, the effect of exercise on C was pronounced. A single bout of high-intensity exercise 10 resulted in compensators responding faster compared to NC. Furthermore, after the test meal (i.e. in a satiated state), the response time for NC was slower for savoury foods compared with before the test meal (i.e. hungry state), while C maintained their pre-meal response rate. These data reveal two distinct patterns of exercise-induced responses in C and NC. The exercise-induced decrease in C’s response time, which was maintained in a fed state, may 15 partially explain the differences in test meal energy intake. It is also of interest to note the unexpected differences at baseline between C and NC. Responses were slower overall in C compared to the NEx treatment, and compared to NC. It is possible that anticipatory responses to impending exercise could partly explain this group*condition interaction at baseline. Implicit wanting for food may be influenced by the anticipation of exercise, and this 20 response may be different in C (who showed relatively less wanting/increased RT) and NC (who showed a decreased RT at baseline under exercise). It is also interesting that acute exercise appears to dissociate implicit and explicit hedonic response to food. We speculatively suggest that metabolic signals associated with acute physical activity (for example ghrelin (40; 41)) may selectively modulate neural pathways associated with wanting 25 for food in susceptible individuals. 17 Finally, further differences between the groups were expressed through the food choices in the procedure (revealing relative preferences). It was noted that C had greater and sustained preference for HFSW stimuli compared to NC. Therefore, C’s tendency to adjust EI after exercise may be accompanied by other factors that could promote a positive energy balance. 5 Interestingly, selective preference for high-fat sweet foods has been demonstrated in subjects with artificially-induced hypoglycaemia (36). Assuming that exercise caused a lowering of blood glucose, one possibility is that differences in food preference may have resulted from differences in glucostatic metabolism or status. Compensators were characterised by enhanced implicit wanting for the food stimuli in the 10 task. Therefore, implicit processes rather than hunger or explicit liking for a range of food are associated with compensatory feeding behaviour and may play a role in the failure of exercise to facilitate weight loss in some individuals. The lack of compensatory adjustment in EI experienced by the NC is in line with the general consensus of research that homeostatic mechanisms of appetite are acutely insensitive to 15 perturbations in energy balance brought about by energy expenditure (e.g. 37;38). Recent evidence from a long-term exercise intervention showed that despite large inter-individual variability, some people don’t compensate and do experience the anticipated weight loss (39). In the current study, compensators differed from NC in that they demonstrated some degree of immediate compensatory response which in several of these subjects was enough to cancel 20 out the acute exercise-induced increase in energy expenditure. These individuals may represent a wider population for whom exercise alone is not an appropriate strategy for weight control. Conclusion 18 Exercise-induced changes in the hedonic response to food could play an important role in weight control. The enhanced implicit wanting for food after acute exercise may help to explain the variable efficacy of exercise as a method of weight loss. Some individuals could be resistant to the beneficial effects of exercise due to a predisposition to compensate for 5 exercise-induced energy expenditure as a result of changes in food preferences. List of Abbreviations BMI 10 15 C Compensator EI Energy intake EIe Energy intake after Ex condition EIn Energy intake after NEx condition Ex Exercise HARU Human appetite research unit HFSA High fat savoury HFSW High fat sweet LFSA Low fat savoury LFSW Low fat sweet NC 20 Body Mass Index Non Compensator NEx No Exercise VAS Visual Analogue Scale Author Contributions EB conceived of the study and carried out the experimental work. GF participated in design of 25 the study, analysed the data and prepared the draft manuscript. JB participated in the design of 19 the study and helped revise the manuscript. NK participated in coordination and design of the study and helped revise the manuscript. GF, JB and NK read and approved the final manuscript. 5 20 References 01. King NA, Caudwell P, Hopkins M, Byrne NM, Colley R, Hills AP, Stubbs RJ, Blundell 5 JE: Automatic and volitional compensatory responses to exercise interventions: metabolic and behavioural barriers to weight loss. Obesity, 2007, 15:1373-83. 02. Blundell JE, Stubbs RJ, Hughes DA, Whybrow S, King NA: Cross talk between physical activity and appetite control: does physical activity stimulate appetite? Proc Nutr Soc, 2003, 10 62:651-61. 03. Leshem M, Abutbul A, Eilon R: Exercise increases the preference for salt in humans. Appetite, 1999, 32:251-260. 15 04. Bellisle F: Food choice, appetite and physical activity. Public Health Nutrition, 1999, 2:357-361. 05. King NA, Snell L, Smith RD, Blundell JE: Effect of short-term exercise on appetite responses in unrestrained females. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1996, 50:663-667. 20 06. Lluch A, King NA, Blundell JE: Exercise in dietary restrained women: no effect on energy intake but change in hedonic ratings. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1998, 52:300-307. 07. Lluch A, King NA, Blundell JE: No energy compensation at the meal following exercise 25 in dietary restrained and unrestrained women. Br J Nutr, 2000, 84:219-225. 21 08. King NA, Blundell JE: High-fat foods overcome the energy expenditure induced by highintensity cycling or running. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1995, 49:114-23. 5 09. Tremblay A, Plourde G, Despres JP: Impact of dietary fat content and fat oxidation on energy intake in humans. Am J Clin Nutr, 1998, 49:799-805. 10. Westerterp-plantenga MS, Verwegen CRT, Ijedema MJW, Wijckmans NEG, Saris WHM: Acute effects of exercise or sauna on appetite in obese and nonobese men. Physiol Behav, 10 1997, 62:1345-1354. 11. Berg C, Sodersten P: Anorexia nervosa, self-starvation and the reward of stress. Nat Med, 1996, 2:21-22. 15 12. Lett BT, Grant VL, Gaborko LL: A small amount of wheel running facilitates eating in non-deprived rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1996, 110:1492-1495. 13. Davis C, Strachan S, Berkson M: Sensitivity to reward: implications for overeating and overweight. Appetite, 2004, 42:131-138. 20 14. Davis C, Fox J: Sensitivity to reward and body mass index (BMI): Evidence for a nonlinear relationship. Appetite, in Press. 15. Davis C, Woodside D: Sensitivity to rewarding effects of food and exercise in the eating 25 disorders. Compr Psychiatry, 2002, 43:189-194. 22 16. Berridge KC: Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 1996, 20:1-25. 17. Berridge KC, Robinson TE: Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci, 2003, 26:507-513. 5 18. Mela DJ: Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite, 2006, 47:10-17. 19. Finlayson GS, King NA, Blundell JE: The role of implicit wanting in relation to explicit 10 liking and wanting for food: Implications for appetite control. Appetite, 2008, 50, 120-127. 20. Finlayson GS, King NA, Blundell JE: Liking vs. wanting food: Importance for human appetite control and weight regulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2007, 31, 987-1002. 15 21. Blundell JE, Finlayson GS: Is susceptibility to weight gain characterised by homeostatic or hedonic risk factors for overconsumption? Physiol Behav, 2004, 82:21-25. 22. Berthoud HR: Neural control of appetite: cross-talk between homeostatic and nonhomeostatic systems. Appetite, 2004, 43:315-317. 20 23. Berthoud HR: Homeostatic and non-homeostatic pathways involved in the control of food intake and energy balance. Obesity, 2006, Suppl 2:197-200. 24. King NA, Lluch A, Stubbs RJ, Blundell JE: High dose exercise does not increase hunger 25 or energy intake in free living males. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1997, 51:478-483. 23 25. Blundell JE, King NA: Effects of exercise on appetite control: loose coupling between energy expenditure and energy intake. Int J Obes, 1999, 22:1-19. 5 26. Westerterp-Plentenga MS, Rolland V, Wilson SA, Westerterp KR: Satiety related to 24h diet-induced thermogenesis during high protein/carbohydrate vs high fat diets measured in a respiration chamber. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1999, 53:495-502. 27. King NA, Burley VJ, Blundell JE: Exercise-induced suppression of appetite: effects on 10 food intake and implications for energy balance. Eur J Clin Nutr, 1994, 48:715-726. 28. Horio T, Kawamura Y: Influence of physical exercise on human preferences for various taste solutions. Chem Senses, 1998, 23:417-21. 15 29. Stunkard AJ, Messick S: The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res, 1985, 29:71-83. 30. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and 20 Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. 31. McCance RA, Widdowson EM: The composition of foods. The Royal Society of Chemistry, Cambridge. 24 32. Blundell JE, King NA: Exercise, appetite control, and energy balance. Nutrition, 2000, 16:519-522. 33. Stubbs RJ, Hughes DA, Johnstone AM, Horgan GW, King NA, Blundell JE: A decrease 5 in physical activity affects appetite, energy, and nutrient balance in lean men feeding ad libitum. Am J Clin Nutr, 2004, 79:62-69. 34. Hill JO, Melby C, Johnson SL, Peters JC: Physical activity and energy requirements. Am J CLin Nutr, 62, 1995, Suppl 4:1059-1066. 10 35. King NA, Appleton K, Rogers PJ, Blundell JE: Effects of sweetness and energy drinks on food intake following exercise. Physiol Behav, 1999, 66:375-379. 36. Dewan S, Gillett A, Mugarza JA, Dovey TM, Halford JCG, Wilding JPH: Effect of 15 insulin-induced hypoglycaemia on energy intake and food choice at a subsequent test meal. Diabetes Metabolism Research Reviews, 2004, 20:405-410. 37. Stubbs RJ, Harbron CG, Murgatroyd P, Prentice A: Covert manipulation of dietary fat and energy density: effect on substrate flux and food intake in men eating ad libitum. Am. J. Clin. 20 Nutr, 1995, 62:316-329. 38. Murgatroyd PR, Goldberg GR, Leahy FE, Gilsenan MB, Prentice AM: Effects of inactivity and diet composition on human energy balance. Int J Obes, 1999, 23:1269-1281. 25 39. King NA, Caudwell P, Hopkins M, Stubbs RJ, Blundell JE. Individual variability following 12 weeks of supervised exercise: identification and characterisation of compensation for exercise-induced weight loss. Int J Obes, 2008, 32:177-184. 5 40. Mackelvie KJ, Meneilly GS, Elahi D, Wong AC, Barr SI, Chanoine JP. Regulation of appetite in lean and obese adolescents after exercise: role of acylated and desacyl ghrelin. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 2007, 92:648-654. 41. Malik S, McGlone F, Bedrossian D, Dagher A. Ghrelin Modulates Brain Activity in Areas 10 that Control Appetitive Behavior. Cell Metabolism, 2007, 92:400-409. 26 Figure legends Figure 1. A schematic diagram of the experimental protocol. Legend: A=Appetite ratings, H=Hedonic ratings, L&W=Liking & Wanting. 5 Figure 2. Inter-individual variability in the acute effect of exercise on compensatory eating. Cases are presented in rank order. Solid line indicates where energy intake was equal for Ex and Nex conditions (no adjustment). Dashed line indicates where energy intake corrected for net energy expenditure was equal for Ex and Nex conditions (perfect compensation). 10 Figure 3. Mean hedonic ratings (mm) of the test meal after Ex and NEx for C and NC groups. Standard error shown as vertical lines. * p<0.05 Table 1. Photographic food stimuli used in the liking and wanting computer task (grouped by 15 food category). HFSA = high fat savoury, LFSA = low fat savoury, HFSW = high fat sweet, LFSW = low fat sweet Table 2. Mean (standard deviation) implicit wanting (choice reaction time in ms) averaged 20 25 across stimuli categories for Ex and NEx over time in C and NC groups. 27