Mediating Differences in Children's Interaction with Digital Libraries Through Modeling Their Tasks Dania Bilal School of Information Sciences, University of Tennessee, 1345 Circle Park, Knoxville, TN 37919. Email: dania@utk.edu Sonia Sarangthem School of Information Sciences, University of Tennessee, 1345 Circle Park, Knoxville, TN 37919, Knoxville, TN 37919. Email: sonia@utk.edu Abstract This paper presents four task-based models that were developed based on Arabicspeaking children’s information seeking behavior in using the International Children’s Digital Library (ICDL). Three general models of user information seeking behavior in digital environments were used as frameworks for generating the task-based models: Ellis & Haugan (1997), Choo, Detlor, & Turnball (2000), and Marchionini (1995). The models show that children’s information seeking was characterized by these seven modes of behavior: Start, Recognize, Browse, Examine, Differentiate, Read, Explore, and Finish. Each of the modes has a number of moves associated with it. Underlying the taskbased models is the information need and affective reactions that were driven by each of the four tasks. Children’s behavior consisted of iterative transitions between the modes, with the exception of the behavior on the closed, fact-based task that showed minimal iterative transitions. Task characteristics, context, interface design of the ICDL, and language influenced children’s information seeking behavior and the amount of iterations they made between the modes. The models show new patterns of children’s information seeking behavior that are Web-specific and that are absent from the three models used as frameworks. Implications are made for research, practice, and system design improvements. 1 INTRODUCTION Many studies have investigated the relationships between a user task and information seeking behavior (Xie, 2007; Marchionini, 2006; Jarvelin & Ingwersen, 2004; Jarvelin & Wilson, 2003; Ingwersen & Jarvelin, 2005; Vakkari, 2003; Bystrom & Jarvelin, 1995; Belkin, Chang, Downs, Saracevic, & Zhao, 1990; Saracevic & Kantor, 1988; Belkin, 1980). There has been a growing interest in examining task influence on user information behavior in Web environments. Various types of tasks have been investigated in previous research, including collaborative vs. individual (Hyldegård, 2009), open vs. closed (Bilal, Sarangthem, & Bachir, 2008); transactional vs. informational (Terai, et al. 2008); complex/goal-oriented (Bartlett & Neugebauer, 2008); directed vs. general purpose (Thatcher, 2008); factual vs. interpretive vs. exploratory (Kim, 2007); known-item vs. subject-oriented (Kim & Allen, 2002); imposed vs. selfselected (Gross, 2004); and assigned vs. semi-assigned vs. fully self-generated (Bilal, 2002a; Bilal, 2001; Bilal, 2000). Studies have also examined task specificity, monitoring, and interruption (Bailey, Adamczyk, Chang, & Chilson, 2006); manipulation (Vakkari, 2003); and complexity (Bystrom & Jarvelin, 1995; Vakkari, 1999). Arguments have recently been raised about embedded tasks and their influence on information behavior (Wildemuth & Hughes; 2006). However, there are insufficient empirical studies that focused on young users (i.e., children), analyzed their task-based information seeking behavior in Web environments, and depicted the behavior in graphical models. In addition, there is scarce body of literature that investigated this behavior in Web interfaces that are multicultural and multilingual in nature. The purpose of this study was to fill this research gap by modeling 2 the task-based information seeking behavior of Arabic-speaking children in using the International Children’s Digital Library (ICDL). RESEARCH QUESTION The overarching research question was: What task-based empirical models can be generated based on Arabic-speaking children’s information seeking behavior in the ICDL and what factors influenced this behavior (context, interface design, language, etc.)? RELATED LITERATURE Three bodies of literature are relevant for this study: 1.Task characteristics and children’s information seeking, 2. Models of children’s information seeking behavior, and 3. Children’s use of the ICDL. 1. Task characteristics and children’s information seeking There is a small body of literature that investigated task influence on children’s information seeking behavior in Web environments including digital libraries. In a recent study of the ICDL, Bilal & Bachir (2007a-b) examined the information seeking behavior and affective reactions of ten Arabic-speaking children, ages 6-10 on four tasks that varied in nature (closed vs. open-ended), type (simple vs. complex), and administration (assigned vs. semi-assigned vs. fully self-generated). Younger children (ages 6-7) experienced difficulty performing two fact-based tasks (tasks 1 and 2). On task 1 (closed, simple, and assigned), one child did not transcribe the answer to the task and another provided an incorrect answer. Similarly, two children failed task 2 (simple known-title search; closed, and assigned) as a result of skipping the task completely. All children 3 completed tasks 3 and 4 (open-ended) successfully. Task 3 was open-ended, semicomplex, and semi-assigned, whereas Task 4 was open-ended, complex, and fully selfgenerated. Children’s information seeking was characterized by browsing as searching the Arabic book collection using Arabic keywords was not supported in the ICDL. In a series of three studies, Bilal investigated the cognitive, affective, and physical information seeking behavior of twenty-two 7th grade science children in using Yahooligans! Children were given three different tasks, one assigned and fact-based (Bilal, 2000), one assigned and research-based (Bilal, 2001), and one fully self-generated (Bilal, 2002a). Children’s interaction activities, success, and affective states were influenced by each task. Children’s insufficient skills of the research process surfaced as a problem especially on the open-ended task. Task characteristic also influenced children’s affective states. Children preferred the fully self-generated task to the other two tasks because they were able to choose topics of interest to them and modify them as many times as needed.. Large & Beheshti (2000) examined middle school students’ Web use on a semiassigned open-ended task for a classroom project dealing with Winter Olympics. Students were asked to a sport activity from the list of activities their teacher prepared. Children worked collaboratively in a group of two to three, forming 20 groups. The authors elicited the students’ reactions to the Web, including term selection, browsing, multimedia content, information content, affect, and use of online help. However, no reactions were gathered from the students about the tasks they performed. 4 Using two fully assigned tasks, one closed (fact-based) and one open-ended (research-based), Schacter, Chung, & Dorr (1998) investigated elementary school students’ use of the Web and success. Children were much more successful on the openended than the fact-based task and exhibited different searching and browsing behavior on each task. Neither the results of this study, nor those generated from the previous studies yielded models of children’s interaction with the Web. 3. Models and children’s information seeking There is scarcity of empirical work that modeled children’s information seeking behavior, in general, and in using Web-based interfaces (e.g., digital libraries), in particular. This section reviews studies that concluded with models of children’s information seeking behavior in different Web environments. Using the findings and data sets collected from two previous studies (Bilal & Bachir, 2007a-b), Bilal, Sarangthem, & Bachir (2008) developed a general model of Arabic-speaking children’s information seeking behavior in using the ICDL. The model consisted of 7 modes of information behavior: Start, Recognize, Browse, Differentiate, Read, Explore, and Finish. The model was informed by the models developed by Ellis & Haugan (1997), Choo, Detlor, & Turnball (2000), and Marchionini (1995). The model begins with the information need (cognitive activity) that is characterized by certainty and anxiety (affective states) and ends with a need fulfillment that is characterized by certainty and satisfaction. Based on this model, four task-based models were generated of Arabic-speaking children’s information seeking behavior in the ICDL. These are described later in this paper. . 5 Shenton (2007) developed a graphical model of information seeking failure of young users, ages 3-18, based on data collected from six schools in Whitely Bay, England. His model consists of five dimensions: Source Dimension, Knowledge Dimension, Skills Dimension, Social Dimension, and Psychological Dimension. Each dimension lists the issues that contributed to information seeking failure. Relationships among the dimensions are clearly described. In discussing the model, Shenton made reference to instances when children experienced difficulty finding information for a general topic that was open-ended and the relative ease of locating information for a topic that was closed in nature. However, his model does not show the “task” as a factor in any of the dimensions. Further development of this model is needed to include task influence, context in which information seeking takes place, and user characteristics. The imposed query framework developed by Gross (1997) pointed out the issues in queries assigned by teachers in elementary schools. Use of the framework was investigated in varied information environments including adult reference desk in public libraries where Gross & Saxton (2002) examined the effect of task type on user assessment and satisfaction. The framework has demonstrated utility in informing practice and guiding professional training in reference services. However, it is descriptive and exploratory in nature. Additional work is needed to test the validity of the framework in Web environments. 3. Children and the ICDL The goal of the ICDL (http://www.icdlbooks.org ) is to build a collection of digital books worldwide to children ages 3-13. The ICDL has a collection of over 3000 6 books in over 45 languages including Arabic. The Arabic collection is small compared to other foreign language collection, 26 books. These have been contributed by Bibliotheca Alexandrina (Alexandria, Egypt). The ICDL was developed by involving children of various ages in the design process. It has three main interfaces for book finding: Simple, Advanced, and Location. The Simple interface allows searching by keyword and language. The Advanced interface is a directory of subject categories. The Location interface provides access to books by continent and countries. In a two-part study, Bilal & Bachir (2007a-b) examined the information seeking behavior of ten Arabic-speaking children, ages 6-10, in using the ICDL to find Arabic books on four different tasks. The study took place at Bibliotheca Alexandrina in late December 2004. In part one, Bilal & Bachir (2007a), assessed the cross-cultural usability of the ICDL as an international Web interface. The authors developed a measure by which they assessed children’s understanding of the ICDL interface design representations and their meanings. Findings revealed that 80% of the children (older ones, ages 8-10) identified the keyword search box in the default interface correctly and understood its meaning, whereas younger children (20%) were unable to do so. No child was able to identify the representations embedded in the Simple, Advanced, and Location interfaces, although the features of each interface contained both textual and pictorial representation. As to the navigation bar that resided on top of the screen, older children identified and recognized the meaning of 3 out of the 4 icons, whereas younger children were unable to do so. The design of the ICDL navigation controls (e.g., Back, Forward, Home) was much more supportive of the navigation of all children than those provided in 7 Internet Explorer. The ICDL was found to be more supportive of older than younger children. In the second part of this study, Bilal & Bachir (2007b) investigated the children’s information seeking behavior, success, and affective reactions in using the ICDL. Children were given four different tasks to perform in the ICDL: one assigned and factbased, one assigned known-title and fact-based, one semi-assigned and research-based, and another fully self-generated. Children’s interaction activities were captured using HyperCam, a software package that records all activities a user performs online (e.g., searching, browsing, clicking, scrolling, etc.). Their affective reactions were elicited during exit interviews. Children’s information seeking behavior was characterized by browsing using a single function; that is, looking under Arabic language from the pulldown menu to view the Arabic book collection. Lack of Arabic keyword searching capability and representative icons for each of the search and browse options available in both the Simple and Advanced interfaces surfaced as barriers to children’s full exploitation of the ICDL. Children expressed anxiety and uncertainty as to their ability to use the ICDL successfully in the beginning of the study, but were certain and satisfied with their performance after completing the tasks. Children enjoyed using the ICDL, reading books, and looking at book images. They made recommendations for improving the design and content of book collection of the ICDL. Massey, Druin, & Weeks (2007) examined the affective reactions of twelve children from four different countries to reading and reviewing books in the ICDL. 8 Children were selected from the United States, Honduras, New Zealand, and Germany. Children were given a book review form to use for answering five questions about each book they read. They were given diverse books to read: a nonfiction book, a book in English about an unfamiliar culture, a book in an unfamiliar language, a book with a strong emotional message, and a chapter book. Children preferred books with happy endings. Language capabilities were a stronger factor in choosing books than culture or nationality as children did not choose books in languages they did not speak. When children were unable to read books in different languages, they relied on images and colors to understand the story and to express how it made them feel. Therefore, their interpretations of book images influenced their emotional classifications of the books. The findings provided insights into the role of emotions in children’s book selection and reading, a factor that information professionals should consider in developing collections and in advising children on what to read. In examining the utility of the design of subject categories in the ICDL, Hutchinson, Bederson, & Druin (2005) involved elementary school children who searched and browsed in two new interfaces developed for the ICDL, one simultaneous and another sequential to find information on six assigned tasks. They found that children created more Boolean searches in the simultaneous interface than in the sequential interface. Older children were more successful than younger ones, performed the tasks faster, took less time, and needed less assistance in navigating the ICDL. The hierarchical structure of subject categories (sequential) was less effective and supportive of children’s information seeking than the simultaneous flat structure. 9 In a related study, Reuter & Druin (2004) investigated the searching and book selection behavior of ninety-six first- through fifth-grade students from the suburbs of Maryland in using the ICDL. Age and gender influenced searching and book selection. Younger children preferred simple and more interactive interfaces; whereas older children favored more sophisticated interfaces. Children in grades one through five were able to navigate the category structure to browse, but were unable to use Boolean logic. The authors suggested new search and browse features to implement in the ICDL to support younger children’s behavior. These studies provided understanding of how younger and older children seek and use information in a Web-based structured digital environment and informed us about children’s information needs. However, none of the studies that involved children in the U.S. yielded a model of their information seeking behavior. The present study is a first attempt towards filling this research gap by modeling Arabic-speaking children’s taskbased information seeking behavior in the ICDL. METHOD The task-based models described in this study were developed based on data sets generated from the previous studies (Bilal & Bachir, 2007a-b). The data sets were the result of using both qualitative and quantitative methods. Participants Ten Arabic-speaking children ages 6-10 participated in data collection. Children were recruited by staff at Bibliotheca Alexandrina’s Children’s Library. Four children 10 were male and six were female. One child was six-years old, three were seven-years old, two were eight, two were nine, one child was nine and a half years-old, and one was tenyears old. Children possessed adequate level of computer and Internet experience. Only one child was familiar with the ICDL. All children had limited English language skills. Children read books on a regular basis. Procedures Data collection took place in late December 2004 at Bibliotheca Alexandrina Children’s Library. Children were divided into two groups of five and their use of the ICDL was scheduled on two separate days. Children in a group were interviewed individually to collect background information about them. Each group was escorted to a computer lab equipped with five PCs HyperCam was installed on all PCs and made available on their task bars. The researchers introduced the purpose of using the ICDL, provided a demonstration of how to use it, explained the tasks to be performed, and gave each child a written task sheet in the Arabic language with instructions. Each child’s interaction activities on each task was captured, saved, and later replayed and burned onto a CD. The researchers conducted exit interviews and gathered children’s affective reactions about using the ICDL. Tasks Children were asked to perform these four tasks: 1. How many books does the ICDL have in the Arabic language, and 2. Find a book in the Arabic language named Dima and open the first page of the book, 3. Find a book about animals in the Arabic 11 language and write the name of the book on your sheet, and 4. Find any book in the Arabic language and read as many pages as you can. Data Sets Each child’s ICDL interaction activity on each task was coded with the child’s ID number, and input into an excel sheet along with the sequence of the activity, a description of the activity (e.g., scan, browse, select book, open page, move to next page), and comments0r /observations about the activity. This process resulted in a total of forty excel sheets on the four tasks (10 children x 4 tasks). These sheets formed the basis of the data sets that were utilized to create the models. Data generated from the exit interviews were also employed to reflect affective states. Models used as frameworks The models described in this paper were originated due to a lack of models that depict children’s task-based information seeking behavior in Web environments. The researchers were guided by three distinct models that are often used to depict user information seeking behavior in digital environments: Ellis & Haugan (1997), Choo, Detlor, & Turnball (2000), and Marchionini (1995). From each of these models, the researchers adapted certain modes of behavior and moves associated with them. The adapted modes and moves were first applied to generate a general model of Arabicspeaking children’s information seeking behavior in the ICDL (Bilal, Sarangthem, & Bachir, 2008). RESULTS 12 This study sought to identify the type of task-based models that characterized Arabic-speaking children’s information seeking behavior in the ICDL on various types of task. Using the general model of developed based these children’s behavior in the ICDL (Bilal, Sarangthem, & Bachir, 2008), we were able to generate four task-based models that represented the children’s interaction with the ICDL. These are described below. Task-based models Children’s information seeking was characterized by seven modes of behavior and a range of moves associated with them. The modes are labelled as M, the moves as MV, and the transitions between the modes as T. Children’s use of navigation controls (Forward, Backward, Zoom in/out, Page, Scroll, Home, etc.) were categorized under the Explore mode. Since children backtracked more than used other navigation controls, we categorized all backtracking activities under the Backtrack move and the rest of navigation activities under the Navigate moves. Underlying the children’s information seeking is the “information need” that was motivated by each task. This need was associated with affective states children experienced before and after using the ICDL. These states are reflected in these models as bipolar representing the feelings before and after completing the tasks. Modes and moves of information behavior The following modes and moves of children’s information seeing characterize the overall behavior on the four tasks. As seen in Figures 1-4, Searching is absent in the four models due to lack of support of Arabic keyword searching in the ICDL. 13 Start (M1): This is the first step for beginning a task after a child recognizes the information need. It generally begins with scanning (M1V1) the default interface features of the ICDL homepage (icons, Simple search, Advanced search, and Keyword search). The resulting action is selecting (M1V2) the Simple search (by clicking on it). A child returns to the homepage to restart a task or to choose a different interface feature to explore. Recognize (M2): In this step, a child scans (M2V1) the language pull-down menu from the Simple Search interface and selects (M2V2) Arabic language. This activity results in a display of thumbnails of Arabic books. Browse (M3): In this step, a child browses the list of book thumbnails and moves to the next page to view additional thumbnails. Two types of browsing are observed: Directed and Semi-directed. Directed browsing includes viewing (M3V1) and verifying (M3V2) the information. It is guided by a target such as that required for the assigned fact-based task (find the number of Arabic books in the ICDL). Semi-directed browsing consists of examining (M3V3) the information found. It is achieved when the target is less definite such as the behavior observed on the fully self-generated open-ended task (find any book you like and read as many pages as you can). Here, a child needs to examine the ICDL Arabic book collection to identify a book of interest to read. Examining may result in making a decision about a book of choice. Differentiate (M4): In this step, a child filters and selects from the sources examined by noticing differences in the books based on a book cover, images, book length, topic of interest, affect (happy/sad), or other factors. Two types of discrimination behavior were 14 observed: Directed and Undirected. Directed differentiation is focused and consists of finding the specific aspect of interest in the filtered books (viewing) (M4V1). This behavior was observed on task 2 (find a book named Dima) and three (find any book about animals and open the first page of the book). Undirected differentiation has very little focus and is characterized by paging through book thumbnails and titles and sometimes jumping from one thumbnail on a screen to another one in a different screen (sweeping) (M4V2). This behavior occurred on task four (find any book in Arabic and read as many pages as you can), which is fully self-generated. Read (M5): In this step, a child reads the total number of Arabic book collection (task one), find the title of a book named Dima (task 2), find a book of interest about Animals and reads the first three pages (task 3), or finds one or two books on any topic of interest to read (task 4). Reading can be minimal (task 1) or maximal (task 4). Two types of behaviors were Directed and Undirected. Directed reading consists of viewing (M5V1) that entails engagement to achieve a goal (observed on tasks 1-4). Undirected reading is characterized by flipping (M5V2) book pages and sometimes without actually reading the text. Flipping takes less time than viewing and engaging in reading. This behavior occurred more on the semi-self-generated and fully self-generated tasks (3-4) where more freedom for making decisions on book selection and reading was provided. Explore (M6): In this step, a child activates selected navigation controls (e.g., icons, Back, Forward, and Home buttons; arrows, etc.) embedded in the ICDL and/or the Internet Explorer browser. Exploration occurred throughout information seeking, but was more frequent during book reading. For example, a child clicks on the plus sign (+) icon located at the top navigation bar of the screen and finds that a page is amplified. The 15 child then clicks on the minus sign (-) and the page returns to its original size. Use of the Back button was more frequent than the other navigation controls. Therefore, the Explore mode was categorized into two moves: Navigate (M6V1) that includes use of navigation controls except for backtracking, and Backtrack (M6V2) that consists of use of the ICDL back arrows and/or Internet Explorer Back button. Finish (M7): In this step, a child ends a task and moves to another until all tasks are completed. Finish results in note taking based on task requirements. As were instructed, children used the task sheet to transcribe the number of Arabic books in the ICDL (Task 1) and the title of the book they selected about Animals (Task 3). The iterative transitions between the seven modes of information seeking behavior shown in the four models were influenced by the characteristics of the four tasks. The iterative transitions children made to find information increased with task complexity. On the fact-based task (simple and closed), children’s behavior was of the linear type as iterative transitions between the modes were minimal (Figure 1). On Task 2, which was a step up in complexity (simple, known-title search), children exhibited more interaction activities and iterative transitions in their behavior. As shown in Figure 2, children’s iterations focused on Browse-Explore-Differentiate-Read-DifferentiateBrowse-Start-Browse. On Task 3 (open-ended), children’s behavior included an increased number of transitions between the modes of behavior over Task 2 and Task 4. This finding was unexpected. However, it is believed that since the topic for Task 3 was semi selfgenerated (main topic assigned was Animals and children had to find a book of interest 16 on this topic) children were uncertain as to whether the books they were viewing were of interest or satisfied the task requirements. Uncertainty is an affective state that influences cognition and actions (Nahl & Bilal, 2007; Kuhlthau, 2004). This explanation is justified further based on the number of moves children exhibited under the Differentiate mode (MV=29), which was the highest on all tasks. Differentiate consists of filtering what has been viewed based on specific criteria. On Task 3, children had to employ their filtering skills to find a book about “animals” and to write its title on the task sheet. Conversely, children did not have to be concerned about selecting a book about a specific topic for Task 4, which was fully self-generated. On Task 4, children could have employed their own book selection criteria based on a book title, cover, images, color, themes, affect, and/or subject matter rather than one sole dictated subject matter (i.e., Animals) as was the case on Task 3. The fact that their iterative transitions on Task 3 were lower than those made on Task 4 can also be attributed to their prior exposure to the Arabic book collection to perform Tasks 1-3. The requirements of Task 4 influenced children’s amount of reading. Children were asked to read as many pages as they could in the book they chose. The fact that they made MV187 reading moves is not surprising because they are avid readers, as was found in the data generated from the open interview (Bilal & Bachir, 2007b). In fact, two children read two books instead of one using the same amount of time other children took to read one book. Other Factors influencing information seeking Beside the tasks characteristics, children’s behavior was impacted by the design of the ICDL, language, and context. 17 ICDL design and information behavior Children desired to find book using keyword, but were unable to do so due to lack of Arabic keyword searching feature in the ICDL. Lack of keyword searching capability in the Arabic language was a barrier to children’s use of analytic searching. One child attempted keyword searching using Arabic words but failed due to lack of indexing of these words for the Arabic book collection. Language Children were unable to browse the subject taxonomies (Advanced interface), or the Location interface due not only to language barrier, but also to lack of iconic representation of these taxonomies. Had children been able to use the keyword searching function in Arabic, we would have been able to analyze their analytic search behavior and identify the moves associated with it. Context Two contexts could have also influenced children’s information seeking behavior. The first one is the non-naturalistic environment where children performed the tasks as volunteers. In addition, the tasks were not part of everyday life information seeking or of a classroom environment where children have more freedom in pursuing the tasks and without time constraints, although they did not have problems completing the tasks within the time allotted for each task. The second one is the ICDL Web space that is structured and small, compared to a search engine web space that is characterized by cognitive overload and disorientation (Bilal, 2000; Bilal, 2001; Bilal, 2002a). Hence, 18 getting lost in the ICDL Web space was unlikely to occur, especially since children’s attention was focused on one collection (e.g., Arabic) to perform the tasks. Although providing a structured space for children to navigate and find information is fruitful, enjoyable, and less distracting than using the whole Web, it can limit their interaction behavior, creativity, and information discovery to a limited space and small collections. To circumvent this limitation, the ICDL should include links to other book collections available on the Web. Conclusion This study described and analyzed four task-based models that were generated based on data sets collected from two previous studies of Arabic-children’s information seeking behavior in using the ICDL (Bilal & Bachir, 2007a-b). The four task-based models were guided by the general model of the children’s information behavior that was guided by the models of Ellis & Haugan (1997), Choo, Detlor, & Turnball (2000), and Marchionini (1995). The four task-based models are empirical in nature and consist of seven modes of behavior: Start, Recognize, Browse, Examine, Differentiate, Read, Explore, and Finish. The Read and Explore along with affective reactions shown in the models are new patterns of behavior. The Read mode may be ICDL-specific, whereas the Explore mode is Web-specific. The overall children’s behavior was non-linear in nature, with the exception of the behavior shown on the fact-based task that was characterized by minimal iterative transitions. This finding indicates that in using a structured Web environment such as the 19 ICDL to find fact-based information, one should expect children to be sequential in their modes of behavior. Further studies are needed to validate this finding. The task-based models showed the children’s affective reactions before and after completing each task. The certainty and confidence children gained after completing the tasks was due to their satisfaction with their performance. Future research should examine children’s affective reactions during information seeking to identify stages of the process where intervention is needed (Bilal, 2007). Children’s task-based information seeking behavior represented in the four taskbased models was influenced by task characteristics, context, interface design of the ICDL, and language. Researchers interested in using the ICDL to explore children’s information seeking behavior should be cognizant that while the ICDL is structured Web space that may not impose cognitive overload and disorientation, it can restrict children’s information discovery due to its small book collection in a limited number of languages. The task-based models could be used as a basis for developing refined and validated models of children’s information seeking behavior in any Web environment. The more these models are validated in future research, the greater our understanding of children’s behavior and the factors influencing them. Such understanding can inform practice, research, and system design improvements. References Bailey, B.P., Adamczyk, P.D., Chang, T.Y, & Chilson, N.A. (2006). A framework for specifying and monitoring user tasks. Computers in Human Behavior 22, 709–732. 20 Bartlett, J.C., & Neugebauer, T. (2008). A Task-based information retrieval interface to support bioinformatics analysis. Proceedings of the IIiX’08, Information Interaction in Context, October 14-17, 2008, London, UK (pp. 97-101). Belkin, N.J. (1980). Anomalous state of knowledge as a basis for information retrieval. Canadian Journal of Information Science, 5, 133-144 Belkin, N.J., Chang, S., Downs, Saracevic, T., & Zhao, S. (1990). Taking account of user tasks, goals and behavior for the design of online public access catalogues. Proceedings of the 53rd ASIS Annual Meeting. Medford, NJ.: Learned Information, Inc. (pp. 69-79). Bilal, D. (2000). Children’s use of the Yahooligans! Web search engine: I. Cognitive, physical, and affective behaviors on fact-based search tasks. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 51, 646–665. Bilal, D. (2001). Children’s use of the Yahooligans! Web search engine: II. Cognitive and physical behaviors on research tasks. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 52, 118–136. Bilal, D. (2002a). Children’s use of the Yahooligans! Web search engine: III. Cognitive and physical behaviors on fully self-generated search tasks. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53, 1170–1183. Bilal, D. (2007). Grounding children's information seeking and system design in child development theory. In D. Nahl & D. Bilal (eds), Information and Emotion: the Emergent Affective Paradigm in Information Behavior Research and Theory. Medford, NJ: Information Today, Inc. Bilal, D., & Bachir, I. (2007a). Children’s interaction with international and multilingual digital libraries. I. Understanding interface system design representations. Information Processing & Management, 43(1), 47-64. Bilal, D., & Bachir, I. (2007b). Children’s interaction with international and multilingual digital libraries. II. Information seeking, success, and affective experience. Information Processing & Management, 43(1), 65-80. Bilal, D., Sarangthem, S., & Bachir, I. (2008). Towards a model of children’s information seeking behavior in using digital libraries. Proceedings of the IIiX’08, Information Interaction inContext, October 14-17, 2008, London, UK (pp. 145-151). Bystrom, K., & Jarvelin, K. (1995). Task complexity affects information seeking and use. Information Processing & Management, 31(191-213. Choo, C. W., Detlor, B., & Turnbull, D. (2000). Information seeking on the Web: An integrated model of browsing and searching. First Monday, 5(2). http://www.uic.edu/htbin/cgiwrap/bin/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/729/638. Accessed 03.14.08. 21 Ellis, D., & Haugan, M. (1997). Modelling the information seeking patterns of engineers and research scientists in an industrial environment. Journal of Documentation, 49(4), 356-369. Gross, M. (2004). Children’s Questions: Information Seeking Behavior in School. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Press, Inc. Gross, M. (1997). Pilot study on the prevalence of imposed queries in a school library media center. School Library Media Quarterly, 25, 157-166. Gross, M., & Saxton, M.L. (2002). Integrating the imposed query into the evaluation of reference: A dichotomous analysis of user ratings. Library & Information Science Research, 24, 251-263. Hyldegard, J. (2009). Beyond the search process – Exploring group members’ information behavior in context. Information Processing & Management, 45, 142-158. Ingwersen, P., & Jarvelin, K. (2005). The Turn: Integration of Information Seeking and Retrieval in Context. Heidelberg: Springer/Kluwer. Jarvelin, K., & Ingwersen, P. (2004). Information seeking research needs extension towards tasks and technology. Information Research, 10(2).http://informationr.net/101/paper212.htm. Accessed on 02.15.09. Jarvelin, K., & Wilson, T.D. (2003). On conceptual models for information seeking and retrieval research. Information Research, 9 (1). http://InformationR.net/ir/91/paper163.html. Accessed on 02/2/09. Kim, J. (2007). Modeling task-based information seeking on the Web: Application of information seeking strategy schema. Proceedings of the 70th Annual Meeting of the American Society for Information Science & Technology 2007. Milwaukee, WI, October 18-25. Available on CD-ROM. Kim, K., & Allen, B. (2002). Cognitive and task influences on web searching behavior. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53(2):109– 119. Large, A., & Beheshti, J. (2000). The Web as a classroom resource: Reactions from the users. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 51, 1069–1080. Marchionini, G. (2006). Exploratory search: from finding to understanding. Commun. ACM, 49(4):41–46. Marchionini, G.(1995). Information seeking in electronic environments. New York: Cambridge University Press. Massey, S.A., Druin, A., & Weeks, A.C. (2007). Emotion, response, and recommendations: The role of affect in children’s book review in a digital library. In D. 22 Nahl & D. Bilal (eds.), Information and Emotion: the Emergent Affective Paradigm in Information Behavior Research and Theory. Medford, NJ: Information Today, Inc., pp. 135-160. Saracevic, T., & Kantor, P. (1988). A study of information seeking and retrieving. II. Users, questions, and effectiveness. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 39(3):177–196. Schacter, J., Chung, G. K. W. K., & Dorr, A. (1998). Children’s Internet searching on complex problems: Performance and process analyses. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 49, 840–849. Shenton, A. K. (2007). Causes of information-seeking failures: Some insights from an English research project. In Mary Kay Chelton and Colleen Cool (eds), Youth Information-Seeking Behavior II. Context, Theories, Models, and Issues. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Press, pp. 313-364. Terai, et al. (2008). Differences between informational and tansactional tasks in information seeking on the Web. Proceedings of the IIiX’08, Information Interaction in Context, 2008, October 14-17. London: UK (pp., 152-159). Thatcher, A. (2008). Web search strategies: The influence of web experience and task type. Information Processing & Management, 44(3):1308–1329. Vakkari, P. (1999). Task complexity, problem structure and actions: Integrating studies on information seeking and retrieval. Information Processing & Management, 35, 819– 837. Vakkari, P. (2003). Task-based information searching. Annual Review of Information Science and Technology, 37, 413-464. Wildmuth, B.M., Hughes, A. (2006). Perspectives on the tasks in which information behaviors are embedded. In Theories of Information Behavior, Karen E. Fisher, Sanda Erdelez, & Lynne F.F. McKechnie (Eds.). Meford, NJ: Information Today, Inc., pp. 275279. Xie, H.I. (2007). Shifts in information-seeking strategies in information retrieval in the digital age. A planned-situation model. Information Research, 12(4). Http://InformationR.net/ir/12-4/colis/colis22.html. 23